Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 27
Filtrar
1.
Exp Brain Res ; 236(10): 2589-2601, 2018 Oct.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29951904

RESUMO

Previously we developed a method that supports active movement generation to allow practice with improvement of good compliance control in tracing and drawing. We showed that the method allowed children with motor impairments to improve at a 3D tracing task to become as proficient as typically developing children and that the training improved 2D figure copying. In this study, we expanded the training protocol to include a wider variety of ages (5-10-year-olds) and we made the figures traced in training the same as in figure copying, but varied the scale of training and copying figures to assess the generality of learning. Forty-eight children were assigned to groups trained using large or small figures. All were tested before training with a tracing task and a copying task. Then, the children trained over five sessions in the tracing task with either small or large figures. Finally, the tracing and copying tasks were tested again following training. A mean speed measure was used to control for path length variations in the timed task. Performance on both tasks at both baseline and posttest varied as a function of the size of the figure and age. In addition, tracing performance also varied with the level of support. In particular, speeds were higher with more support, larger figures and older children. After training, performance improved. Speeds increased. In tracing, performance improved more for large figures traced by children who trained on large figures. In copying, however, performance only improved significantly for children who had trained on small figures and it improved equally for large and small figures. In conclusion, training by tracing smaller figures yielded better learning that was not, however, specific to the scale of drawn figures. Small figures exhibit greater mean curvature. We infer that it yielded better general improvement.


Assuntos
Desenvolvimento Infantil , Complacência (Medida de Distensibilidade)/fisiologia , Movimento/fisiologia , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Ensino , Fatores Etários , Análise de Variância , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Gráficos por Computador , Percepção de Profundidade/fisiologia , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Percepção de Tamanho/fisiologia , Inquéritos e Questionários , Interface Usuário-Computador
2.
Exp Brain Res ; 233(7): 2225-38, 2015 Jul.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25929551

RESUMO

Under certain conditions, learning can transfer from a trained task to an untrained version of that same task. However, it is as yet unclear what those certain conditions are or why learning transfers when it does. Coordinated rhythmic movement is a valuable model system for investigating transfer because we have a model of the underlying task dynamic that includes perceptual coupling between the limbs being coordinated. The model predicts that (1) coordinated rhythmic movements, both bimanual and unimanual, are organised with respect to relative motion information for relative phase in the coupling function, (2) unimanual is less stable than bimanual coordination because the coupling is unidirectional rather than bidirectional, and (3) learning a new coordination is primarily about learning to perceive and use the relevant information which, with equal perceptual improvement due to training, yields equal transfer of learning from bimanual to unimanual coordination and vice versa [but, given prediction (2), the resulting performance is also conditioned by the intrinsic stability of each task]. In the present study, two groups were trained to produce 90° either unimanually or bimanually, respectively, and tested in respect to learning (namely improved performance in the trained 90° coordination task and improved visual discrimination of 90°) and transfer of learning (to the other, untrained 90° coordination task). Both groups improved in the task condition in which they were trained and in their ability to visually discriminate 90°, and this learning transferred to the untrained condition. When scaled by the relative intrinsic stability of each task, transfer levels were found to be equal. The results are discussed in the context of the perception-action approach to learning and performance.


Assuntos
Lateralidade Funcional/fisiologia , Movimento/fisiologia , Periodicidade , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Transferência de Experiência/fisiologia , Adolescente , Adulto , Análise de Variância , Feminino , Humanos , Julgamento , Aprendizagem , Masculino , Adulto Jovem
3.
Exp Brain Res ; 232(12): 3821-31, 2014 Dec.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25146573

RESUMO

Locomoting-to-reach to a target is a common visuomotor approach behavior that consists of two nested component actions: locomotion and reaching. The information and control strategies that guide locomotion and reaching in isolation are well studied, but their interaction during locomoting-to-reach behavior has received little attention. We investigated the role of proportional rate control in unifying these components into one action. Individuals use this control strategy with hand-centric disparity-based τ information to guide seated reaching (Anderson and Bingham in Exp Brain Res 205:291-306. doi: 10.1007/s00221-010-2361-9 , 2010) and use it with sequential information to perform targeted locomotion to bring an outstretched arm and hand to a target; first with eye-centric τ information and then hand-centric τ information near the target (Anderson and Bingham in Exp Brain Res 214:631-644. doi: 10.1007/s00221-011-2865-y , 2011). In the current study, participants performed two tasks: locomoting to bring a rigidly outstretched arm and hand to a target (handout), and locomoting to initiate and guide a reach to a target (locomoting-to-reach). Movement trajectories were analyzed. Results show that participants used proportional rate control throughout both tasks, in the sequential manner that was found by Anderson and Bingham (Exp Brain Res 214:631-644. doi: 10.1007/s00221-011-2865-y , 2011). Individual differences were found in the moment at which this information switch occurred in the locomoting-to-reach task. Some participants appeared to switch to proportional rate control with hand-τ once the hand came into view and others switched once the reaching component was complete and the arm was fully outstretched. In the locomoting-to-reach task, participants consistently initiated reaches when eye-τ specified a time-to-contact of 1.0 s. Proportional rate control provides a solution to the degrees-of-freedom problem in the classic manner, by making multiple things one.


Assuntos
Fenômenos Biomecânicos/fisiologia , Locomoção/fisiologia , Atividade Motora/fisiologia , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Adulto , Feminino , Mãos/fisiologia , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Movimento/fisiologia , Adulto Jovem
4.
Exp Brain Res ; 225(1): 75-84, 2013 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23212470

RESUMO

This study examined perception-action learning in younger adults in their 20s compared to older adults in their 70s and 80s. The goal was to provide, for the first time, quantitative estimates of perceptuo-motor learning rates for each age group and to reveal how these learning rates change between these age groups. We used a visual coordination task in which participants are asked to learn to produce a novel-coordinated rhythmic movement. The task has been studied extensively in young adults, and the characteristics of the task are well understood. All groups showed improvement, although learning rates for those in their 70s and 80s were half the rate for those in their 20s. We consider the potential causes of these differences in learning rates by examining performance across the different coordination patterns examined as well as recent results that reveal age-related deficits in motion perception.


Assuntos
Envelhecimento/psicologia , Aprendizagem/fisiologia , Destreza Motora/fisiologia , Percepção/fisiologia , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Análise de Variância , Interpretação Estatística de Dados , Feminino , Humanos , Análise dos Mínimos Quadrados , Modelos Lineares , Masculino , Desempenho Psicomotor , Retenção Psicológica/fisiologia , Inquéritos e Questionários , Adulto Jovem
5.
Scientometrics ; 128(6): 3225-3255, 2023.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36818051

RESUMO

This paper uses accounting concepts-particularly the concept of Return on Investment (ROI)-to reveal the quantitative value of scientific research pertaining to a major US cyberinfrastructure project (XSEDE-the eXtreme Science and Engineering Discovery Environment). XSEDE provides operational and support services for advanced information technology systems, cloud systems, and supercomputers supporting non-classified US research, with an average budget for XSEDE of US$20M+ per year over the period studied (2014-2021). To assess the financial effectiveness of these services, we calculated a proxy for ROI, and converted quantitative measures of XSEDE service delivery into financial values using costs for service from the US marketplace. We calculated two estimates of ROI: a Conservative Estimate, functioning as a lower bound and using publicly available data for a lower valuation of XSEDE services; and a Best Available Estimate, functioning as a more accurate estimate, but using some unpublished valuation data. Using the largest dataset assembled for analysis of ROI for a cyberinfrastructure project, we found a Conservative Estimate of ROI of 1.87, and a Best Available Estimate of ROI of 3.24. Through accounting methods, we show that XSEDE services offer excellent value to the US government, that the services offered uniquely by XSEDE (that is, not otherwise available for purchase) were the most valuable to the facilitation of US research activities, and that accounting-based concepts hold great value for understanding the mechanisms of scientific research generally.

6.
Exp Brain Res ; 215(2): 89-100, 2011 Nov.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21952789

RESUMO

Following many studies showing that the coupling in bimanual coordination can be perceptual, Bingham (Ecol Psychol in 16:45-53, 2001; 2004a, b) proposed a dynamical model of such movements. The model contains three key hypotheses: (1) Being able to produce stable coordinative movements is a function of the ability to perceive relative phase, (2) the information to perceive relative phase is relative direction of motion, and (3) the ability to resolve this information is conditioned by relative speed. The first two hypotheses have been well supported (Wilson and Bingham in Percept Psychophys 70:465-476, 2008; Wilson et al. in J Exp Psychol Hum 36:1508-1514, 2010a), but the third was not supported when tested by de Rugy et al. (Exp Brain Res 184:269-273, 2008) using a visual coordination task that required simultaneous control of both the amplitude and relative phase of movement. The purposes of the current study were to replicate this task with additional measures and to modify the original model to apply it to the new task. To do this, we conducted two experiments. First, we tested the ability to produce 180° visual coordination at different frequencies to determine frequencies suitable for testing in the de Rugy et al. task. Second, we tested the de Rugy et al. task but included additional measures that yielded results different from those reported by de Rugy et al. These results were used to elaborate the original model. First, one of the phase-driven oscillators was replaced with a harmonic oscillator, so the resulting coupling was unidirectional. This change resulted in the model producing less stable 180° coordination behavior beyond 1.5 Hz consistent with the results obtained in Experiment 1. Next, amplitude control and phase correction elements were added to the model. With these changes, the model reproduced behaviors observed in Experiment 2. The central finding was that the stability of rhythmic movement coordination does depend on relative speed and, thus, all three of the hypotheses contained in the original Bingham model are supported.


Assuntos
Modelos Neurológicos , Percepção de Movimento/fisiologia , Movimento/fisiologia , Periodicidade , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Adulto , Lateralidade Funcional/fisiologia , Humanos , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Tempo de Reação/fisiologia , Fatores de Tempo , Adulto Jovem
7.
Exp Brain Res ; 205(4): 513-20, 2010 Sep.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20703872

RESUMO

A common perception-action learning task is to teach participants to produce a novel coordinated rhythmic movement, e.g. 90 degrees mean relative phase. As a general rule, people cannot produce these novel movements stably without training. This is because they are extremely poor at discriminating the perceptual information required to coordinate and control the movement, which means people require additional (augmented) feedback to learn the novel task. Extant methods (e.g. visual metronomes, Lissajous figures) work, but all involve transforming the perceptual information about the task and thus altering the perception-action task dynamic being studied. We describe and test a new method for providing online augmented coordination feedback using a neutral colour cue. This does not alter the perceptual information or the overall task dynamic, and an experiment confirms that (a) feedback is required for learning a novel coordination and (b) the new feedback method provides the necessary assistance. This task-appropriate augmented feedback therefore allows us to study the process of learning while preserving the perceptual information that constitutes a key part of the task dynamic being studied. This method is inspired by and supports a fully perception-action approach to coordinated rhythmic movement.


Assuntos
Retroalimentação Sensorial/fisiologia , Aprendizagem/fisiologia , Periodicidade , Estimulação Luminosa/métodos , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Adolescente , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Adulto Jovem
8.
Exp Brain Res ; 198(4): 527-33, 2009 Oct.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19626315

RESUMO

Previous research has shown that adults perceive affordances like the passability of apertures, climbability or crossability of steps and graspability of objects. In this study, the affordance for stepping over or onto barriers was examined in young children. This was done by placing three distinct barriers (a foam obstacle, a gap, and a single step up), which were scaled to body size, in the walking paths of 4- and 6-year olds and adults, and observing how they crossed the barriers. Age-related differences in the scaling of these actions corresponded to levels of movement variability, indicating that children as young as 4 years old are sensitive to their own constraints and scale their actions accordingly. These results indicate that affordances are not directly related to leg geometry, but rather entail the dynamics of the developing perception-action system.


Assuntos
Desenvolvimento Infantil , Desempenho Psicomotor , Caminhada , Adolescente , Adulto , Envelhecimento , Análise de Variância , Fenômenos Biomecânicos , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Atividade Motora , Análise de Regressão , Dedos do Pé , Adulto Jovem
9.
Infancy ; 14(1): 101-116, 2009 Jan 02.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32693472

RESUMO

Learning to walk is a dynamic process requiring the fine coordination, assembly, and balancing of many body segments at once. For the young walker, coordinating all these behavioral levels may be quite daunting. In this study, we examine the whole-body strategies to which infants resort to produce their first independent steps and progress over the first months of walking experience. Six infants were followed weekly from the onset of independent upright locomotion for 8 weeks, and then every other week until 4 months of walking experience. The walking kinematics from the infants' earliest steps were cluster-analyzed and the infants were classified into 3 groups. Follow-up comparisons with kinematics recordings were used to quantify the infants' strategies more precisely and track how these early forms of walking evolved over time. Results revealed that in the first weeks of independent walking, 3 infants used a stepping strategy, 1 used a twisting strategy, and 2 used a falling strategy to move their body forward and perform their first unsupported steps. As the infants gained walking experience, their walking patterns became more similar. These findings indicate that infants discover different solutions to use their body and control their balance when beginning to walk. With time, infants adopt a more efficient solution that incorporates and integrates elements of the different strategies.

10.
Hum Mov Sci ; 652019 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29656792

RESUMO

A large proportion of school-aged children exhibit poor drawing and handwriting. This prevalence limits the availability of therapy. We developed an automated method for training improved manual compliance control and relatedly, prospective control of a stylus. The approach included a difficult training task, while providing parametrically modifiable support that enables the children to perform successfully while developing good compliance control. The task was to use a stylus to push a bead along a 3D wire path. Support was provided by making the wire magnetically attractive to the stylus. Support was progressively reduced as 3D tracing performance improved. We report studies that (1) compared performance of Typically Developing (TD) children and children with Developmental Coordination Disorder (DCD), (2) tested training with active versus passive movement, (3) tested progressively reduced versus constant or no support during training, (4) tested children of different ages, (5) tested the transfer of training to a drawing task, (6) tested the specificity of training in respect to the size, shape and dimensionality of figures, and (7) investigated the relevance of the training task to the Beery VMI, an inventory used to diagnose DCD. The findings were as follows. (1) Pre-training performance of TD and DCD children was the same and good with high support but distinct and poor with low support. Support yielded good self-efficacy that motivated training. Post training performance with no support was improved and the same for TD and DCD children. (2) Actively controlled movements were required for improved performance. (3) Progressively reduced support was required for good performance during and after training. (4) Age differences in performance during pre-training were eliminated post-training. (5) Improvements transferred to drawing. (6) There was no evidence of specificity of training in transfer. (7) Disparate Beery scores were reflected in pre-training but not post-training performance. We conclude that the method improves manual compliance control, and more generally, prospective control of movements used in drawing performance.


Assuntos
Escrita Manual , Transtornos das Habilidades Motoras/terapia , Destreza Motora/fisiologia , Criança , Desenvolvimento Infantil/fisiologia , Pré-Escolar , Feminino , Humanos , Magnetismo , Masculino , Transtornos das Habilidades Motoras/fisiopatologia , Transtornos das Habilidades Motoras/psicologia , Movimento/fisiologia , Estudos Prospectivos , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Transferência de Experiência
11.
Hum Mov Sci ; 60: 98-106, 2018 Aug.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29852338

RESUMO

How is information from different sensory modalities coordinated when learning an action? We tested two hypotheses. The first is that the information is amodal. The second is that the information is modality specific and one modality is used in first learning the action and then is used to teach the other modality. We investigated these hypotheses using a rhythmic coordination task. One group of participants learned to perform bimanual coordination at a relative phase of 90° using kinesthesis. A second group used vision to learn unimanual 90° coordination. After training, performance using the alternate modality was tested in each case. Snapp-Childs, Wilson, and Bingham (2015) had found transfer of 50% of learned performance of 90° coordination between the unimanual and bimanual tasks when each had included use of vision. Now, we found essentially no transfer (≈5%) indicating that the information was modality specific. Next, post-training trials performed using the untrained modality were alternated with trials in which kinesthesis and vision were used. The result was that performance using the untrained modality progressively improved. We concluded that trained modality was used to teach the untrained modality and that this likely represents the way information from different sensory modalities is coordinated in performance of actions.


Assuntos
Lateralidade Funcional/fisiologia , Cinestesia/fisiologia , Visão Ocular/fisiologia , Adolescente , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Aprendizagem , Masculino , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Transferência de Experiência , Adulto Jovem
12.
Atten Percept Psychophys ; 79(6): 1830-1840, 2017 Aug.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28508115

RESUMO

Many studies have shown that rhythmic interlimb coordination involves perception of the coupled limb movements, and different sensory modalities can be used. Using visual displays to inform the coupled bimanual movement, novel bimanual coordination patterns can be learned with practice. A recent study showed that similar learning occurred without vision when a coach provided manual guidance during practice. The information provided via the two different modalities may be same (amodal) or different (modality specific). If it is different, then learning with both is a dual task, and one source of information might be used in preference to the other in performing the task when both are available. In the current study, participants learned a novel 90° bimanual coordination pattern without or with visual information in addition to kinesthesis. In posttest, all participants were tested without and with visual information in addition to kinesthesis. When tested with visual information, all participants exhibited performance that was significantly improved by practice. When tested without visual information, participants who practiced using only kinesthetic information showed improvement, but those who practiced with visual information in addition showed remarkably less improvement. The results indicate that (1) the information is not amodal, (2) use of a single type of information was preferred, and (3) the preferred information was visual. We also hypothesized that older participants might be more likely to acquire dual task performance given their greater experience of the two sensory modes in combination, but results were replicated with both 20- and 50-year-olds.


Assuntos
Lateralidade Funcional/fisiologia , Cinestesia , Aprendizagem , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Percepção Visual , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Movimento , Estimulação Luminosa/métodos
13.
Hum Mov Sci ; 45: 172-81, 2016 Feb.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26684725

RESUMO

Previous empirical and theoretical work suggests that effective skill acquisition requires movements to be generated actively and that learning new skills supports the acquisition of prospective control. However, there are many ways in which practice can be structured, that may affect the acquisition and use of prospective control after training. Here, we tested whether the progressive modulation and reduction of support during training was required to yield good performance after training without support. The task was to use a stylus to push a bead over a complex 3D wire path. The support "magnetically" attracted and held the stylus onto the wire. Three groups of adult participants each experienced one of three training regimes: gradual reduction of magnetic attraction, only a medium level of attraction, or low magnetic attraction. The results showed that use of a single (medium) level of support was significantly less effective in yielding good performance with low support after training. Training with low support yielded post-training performance that was equally good as that yielded by training with progressive reduction of support; however, performance during training was significantly poorer in the former. Thus, less support during training yields effective learning but more difficult training sessions. The results are discussed in the context of application to training with special populations.


Assuntos
Magnetismo , Destreza Motora , Prática Psicológica , Desempenho Psicomotor , Adulto , Antecipação Psicológica , Feminino , Humanos , Controle Interno-Externo , Masculino , Estudos Prospectivos , Adulto Jovem
14.
Perception ; 45(4): 446-73, 2016 Apr.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26614099

RESUMO

Zannoli, Cass, Alais, and Mamassian (2012) found greater audiovisual lag between a tone and disparity-defined stimuli moving laterally (90-170 ms) than for disparity-defined stimuli moving in depth or luminance-defined stimuli moving laterally or in depth (50-60 ms). We tested if this increased lag presents an impediment to visually guided coordination with laterally moving objects. Participants used a joystick to move a virtual object in several constant relative phases with a laterally oscillating stimulus. Both the participant-controlled object and the target object were presented using a disparity-defined display that yielded information through changes in disparity over time (CDOT) or using a luminance-defined display that additionally provided information through monocular motion and interocular velocity differences (IOVD). Performance was comparable for both disparity-defined and luminance-defined displays in all relative phases. This suggests that, despite lag, perception of lateral motion through CDOT is generally sufficient to guide coordinated motor behavior.


Assuntos
Percepção de Movimento/fisiologia , Atividade Motora/fisiologia , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Visão Binocular/fisiologia , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino
15.
Hum Mov Sci ; 48: 171-83, 2016 Aug.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27219739

RESUMO

There are a large number of children with motor difficulties including those that have difficulty producing movements qualitatively well enough to improve in perceptuo-motor learning without intervention. We have developed a training method that supports active movement generation to allow improvement in a 3D tracing task requiring good compliance control. Previously, we tested a limited age range of children and found that training improved performance on the 3D tracing task and that the training transferred to a 2D drawing test. In the present study, school children (5-11years old) with motor difficulties were trained in the 3D tracing task and transfer to a 2D drawing task was tested. We used a cross-over design where half of the children received training on the 3D tracing task during the first training period and the other half of the children received training during the second training period. Given previous results, we predicted that younger children would initially show reduced performance relative to the older children, and that performance at all ages would improve with training. We also predicted that training would transfer to the 2D drawing task. However, the pre-training performance of both younger and older children was equally poor. Nevertheless, post-training performance on the 3D task was dramatically improved for both age groups and the training transferred to the 2D drawing task. Overall, this work contributes to a growing body of literature that demonstrates relatively preserved motor learning in children with motor difficulties and further demonstrates the importance of games in therapeutic interventions.


Assuntos
Transtornos das Habilidades Motoras/reabilitação , Educação Física e Treinamento/métodos , Desempenho Psicomotor , Envelhecimento , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Computadores de Mão , Estudos Cross-Over , Feminino , Humanos , Aprendizagem , Masculino , Destreza Motora , Transtornos das Habilidades Motoras/fisiopatologia , Resultado do Tratamento
16.
PLoS One ; 11(3): e0151354, 2016.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26967993

RESUMO

Motor deficits are linked to a range of negative physical, social and academic consequences. Haptic robotic interventions, based on the principles of sensorimotor learning, have been shown previously to help children with motor problems learn new movements. We therefore examined whether the training benefits of a robotic system would generalise to a standardised test of 'pen-skills', assessed using objective kinematic measures [via the Clinical Kinematic Assessment Tool, CKAT]. A counterbalanced, cross-over design was used in a group of 51 children (37 male, aged 5-11 years) with manual control difficulties. Improved performance on a novel task using the robotic device could be attributed to the intervention but there was no evidence of generalisation to any of the CKAT tasks. The robotic system appears to have the potential to support motor learning, with the technology affording numerous advantages. However, the training regime may need to target particular manual skills (e.g. letter formation) in order to obtain clinically significant improvements in specific skills such as handwriting.


Assuntos
Escrita Manual , Destreza Motora , Robótica , Fatores Etários , Fenômenos Biomecânicos , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Desempenho Psicomotor
17.
Hum Mov Sci ; 43: 90-9, 2015 Oct.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26241334

RESUMO

Many children have difficulty producing movements well enough to improve in perceptuo-motor learning. We have developed a training method that supports active movement generation to allow improvement in a 3D tracing task requiring good compliance control. We previously tested 7-8 year old children who exhibited poor performance and performance differences before training. After training, performance was significantly improved and performance differences were eliminated. According to the Dynamic Systems Theory of development, appropriate support can enable younger children to acquire the ability to perform like older children. In the present study, we compared 7-8 and 10-12 year old school children and predicted that younger children would show reduced performance that was nonetheless amenable to training. Indeed, the pre-training performance of the 7-8 year olds was worse than that of the 10-12 year olds, but post-training performance was equally good for both groups. This was similar to previous results found using this training method for children with DCD and age-matched typically developing children. We also found in a previous study of 7-8 year old school children that training in the 3D tracing task transferred to a 2D drawing task. We now found similar transfer for the 10-12 year olds.


Assuntos
Desenvolvimento Infantil , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos , Prática Psicológica , Desempenho Psicomotor , Fatores Etários , Fenômenos Biomecânicos , Criança , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Transferência de Experiência
18.
PLoS One ; 10(4): e0121708, 2015.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25874880

RESUMO

Previously, we measured perceptuo-motor learning rates across the lifespan and found a sudden drop in learning rates between ages 50 and 60, called the "50s cliff." The task was a unimanual visual rhythmic coordination task in which participants used a joystick to oscillate one dot in a display in coordination with another dot oscillated by a computer. Participants learned to produce a coordination with a 90° relative phase relation between the dots. Learning rates for participants over 60 were half those of younger participants. Given existing evidence for visual motion perception deficits in people over 60 and the role of visual motion perception in the coordination task, it remained unclear whether the 50s cliff reflected onset of this deficit or a genuine decline in perceptuo-motor learning. The current work addressed this question. Two groups of 12 participants in each of four age ranges (20s, 50s, 60s, 70s) learned to perform a bimanual coordination of 90° relative phase. One group trained with only haptic information and the other group with both haptic and visual information about relative phase. Both groups were tested in both information conditions at baseline and post-test. If the 50s cliff was caused by an age dependent deficit in visual motion perception, then older participants in the visual group should have exhibited less learning than those in the haptic group, which should not exhibit the 50s cliff, and older participants in both groups should have performed less well when tested with visual information. Neither of these expectations was confirmed by the results, so we concluded that the 50s cliff reflects a genuine decline in perceptuo-motor learning with aging, not the onset of a deficit in visual motion perception.


Assuntos
Envelhecimento , Aprendizagem , Percepção de Movimento , Percepção Visual , Adulto , Idoso , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Desempenho Psicomotor , Adulto Jovem
19.
PLoS One ; 9(3): e92464, 2014.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24651280

RESUMO

Many children have difficulty producing movements well enough to improve in sensori-motor learning. Previously, we developed a training method that supports active movement generation to allow improvement at a 3D tracing task requiring good compliance control. Here, we tested 7-8 year old children from several 2nd grade classrooms to determine whether 3D tracing performance could be predicted using the Beery VMI. We also examined whether 3D tracing training lead to improvements in drawing. Baseline testing included Beery, a drawing task on a tablet computer, and 3D tracing. We found that baseline performance in 3D tracing and drawing co-varied with the visual perception (VP) component of the Beery. Differences in 3D tracing between children scoring low versus high on the Beery VP replicated differences previously found between children with and without motor impairments, as did post-training performance that eliminated these differences. Drawing improved as a result of training in the 3D tracing task. The training method improved drawing and reduced differences predicted by Beery scores.


Assuntos
Aprendizagem Baseada em Problemas , Desempenho Psicomotor , Criança , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino
20.
J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform ; 40(4): 1542-50, 2014 Aug.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24865157

RESUMO

Mon-Williams and Bingham (2011) developed an affordance model of the spatial structure of reaches-to-grasp. With a single free parameter (P), the model predicted the safety margins (SMs) exhibited in maximum grasp apertures (MGAs), during the approach of a hand to a target object, as a function of an affordance measure of object size and a functional measure of hand size. An affordance analysis revealed that object size is determined by a diagonal through the object, called the maximum object extent. Mon-Williams and Bingham provided no theoretical account for the empirically determined values of P. We now address this question. Snapp-Childs and Bingham (2009) augmented Warren's (1984) geometric affordance scaling model with a dynamical component determined by the stability of the motor performance. Because P was found to vary with the speeds of reaches, we incorporated a measure of the variability of performance into the model to yield predictions of P. We also found that P varied with gender. In respect to the size of safety margins, women were more conservative in taking risks then men. Finally, following Warren (1984), the classic paradigm for testing affordance models is to test the scaling relations with both small and large participants. We tested small- and large-handed men and small- and large-handed women and found that the new parameter free model successfully accounted for the spatial structure of reaches-to-grasp.


Assuntos
Mãos , Atividade Motora/fisiologia , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Assunção de Riscos , Percepção de Tamanho/fisiologia , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Fatores Sexuais
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA