Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 28
Filtrar
1.
Neuroimage ; 69: 126-37, 2013 Apr 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23186917

RESUMEN

Functional networks are comprised of neuronal ensembles bound through synchronization across multiple intrinsic oscillatory frequencies. Various coupled interactions between brain oscillators have been described (e.g., phase-amplitude coupling), but with little evidence that these interactions actually influence perceptual sensitivity. Here, electroencephalographic (EEG) recordings were made during a sustained-attention task to demonstrate that cross-frequency coupling has significant consequences for perceptual outcomes (i.e., whether participants detect a near-threshold visual target). The data reveal that phase-detection relationships at higher frequencies are dependent on the phase of lower frequencies, such that higher frequencies alternate between periods when their phase is either strongly or weakly predictive of visual-target detection. Moreover, the specific higher frequencies and scalp topographies linked to visual-target detection also alternate as a function of lower-frequency phase. Cross-frequency coupling between lower (i.e., delta and theta) and higher frequencies (e.g., low- and high-beta) thus results in dramatic fluctuations of visual-target detection.


Asunto(s)
Encéfalo/fisiología , Vías Nerviosas/fisiología , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Adulto , Mapeo Encefálico , Electroencefalografía , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Estimulación Luminosa , Procesamiento de Señales Asistido por Computador
2.
Exp Brain Res ; 219(1): 1-11, 2012 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22434342

RESUMEN

The perception of self-motion is a product of the integration of information from both visual and non-visual cues, to which the vestibular system is a central contributor. It is well documented that vestibular dysfunction leads to impaired movement and balance, dizziness and falls, and yet our knowledge of the neuronal processing of vestibular signals remains relatively sparse. In this study, high-density electroencephalographic recordings were deployed to investigate the neural processes associated with vestibular detection of changes in heading. To this end, a self-motion oddball paradigm was designed. Participants were translated linearly 7.8 cm on a motion platform using a one second motion profile, at a 45° angle leftward or rightward of straight ahead. These headings were presented with a stimulus probability of 80-20 %. Participants responded when they detected the infrequent direction change via button-press. Event-related potentials (ERPs) were calculated in response to the standard (80 %) and target (20 %) movement directions. Statistical parametric mapping showed that ERPs to standard and target movements differed significantly from 490 to 950 ms post-stimulus. Topographic analysis showed that this difference had a typical P3 topography. Individual participant bootstrap analysis revealed that 93.3 % of participants exhibited a clear P3 component. These results indicate that a perceived change in vestibular heading can readily elicit a P3 response, wholly similar to that evoked by oddball stimuli presented in other sensory modalities. This vestibular-evoked P3 response may provide a readily and robustly detectable objective measure for the evaluation of vestibular integrity in various disease models.


Asunto(s)
Mapeo Encefálico , Potenciales Evocados/fisiología , Movimientos de la Cabeza/fisiología , Percepción de Movimiento/fisiología , Detección de Señal Psicológica/fisiología , Vestíbulo del Laberinto/fisiología , Estimulación Acústica , Adulto , Análisis de Varianza , Electroencefalografía , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Factores de Tiempo , Adulto Joven
3.
Drug Alcohol Depend ; 227: 108946, 2021 10 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34392051

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: The Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development ™ Study (ABCD Study®) is an open-science, multi-site, prospective, longitudinal study following over 11,800 9- and 10-year-old youth into early adulthood. The ABCD Study aims to prospectively examine the impact of substance use (SU) on neurocognitive and health outcomes. Although SU initiation typically occurs during teen years, relatively little is known about patterns of SU in children younger than 12. METHODS: This study aims to report the detailed ABCD Study® SU patterns at baseline (n = 11,875) in order to inform the greater scientific community about cohort's early SU. Along with a detailed description of SU, we ran mixed effects regression models to examine the association between early caffeine and alcohol sipping with demographic factors, externalizing symptoms and parental history of alcohol and substance use disorders (AUD/SUD). PRIMARY RESULTS: At baseline, the majority of youth had used caffeine (67.6 %) and 22.5 % reported sipping alcohol (22.5 %). There was little to no reported use of other drug categories (0.2 % full alcohol drink, 0.7 % used nicotine, <0.1 % used any other drug of abuse). Analyses revealed that total caffeine use and early alcohol sipping were associated with demographic variables (p's<.05), externalizing symptoms (caffeine p = 0002; sipping p = .0003), and parental history of AUD (sipping p = .03). CONCLUSIONS: ABCD Study participants aged 9-10 years old reported caffeine use and alcohol sipping experimentation, but very rare other SU. Variables linked with early childhood alcohol sipping and caffeine use should be examined as contributing factors in future longitudinal analyses examining escalating trajectories of SU in the ABCD Study cohort.


Asunto(s)
Trastornos Relacionados con Sustancias , Adolescente , Adulto , Encéfalo , Niño , Preescolar , Cognición , Humanos , Estudios Longitudinales , Estudios Prospectivos , Trastornos Relacionados con Sustancias/epidemiología
4.
Eur J Neurosci ; 27(9): 2303-12, 2008 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18445221

RESUMEN

Previous event-related potential (ERP) studies have identified the superior parietal lobule (SPL) as actively multisensory. This study compares effective, or contextually active, connections to this region under unisensory and multisensory conditions. Effective connectivity, the influence of one brain region over another, during unisensory visual, unisensory auditory and multisensory audiovisual stimulation was investigated. ERPs were recorded from subdural electrodes placed over the parietal lobe of three patients while they conducted a rapid reaction-time task. A generative model of interacting neuronal ensembles for ERPs was inverted in a scheme allowing investigation of the connections from and to the SPL, a multisensory processing area. Important features of the ensemble model include inhibitory and excitatory feedback connections to pyramidal cells and extrinsic input to the stellate cell pool, with extrinsic forward and backward connections delineated by laminar connection differences between ensembles. The framework embeds the SPL in a plausible connection of distinct neuronal ensembles mirroring the integrated brain regions involved in the response task. Bayesian model comparison was used to test competing feed-forward and feed-backward models of how the electrophysiological data were generated. Comparisons were performed between multisensory and unisensory data. Findings from three patients show differences in summed unisensory and multisensory ERPs that can be accounted for by a mediation of both forward and backward connections to the SPL. In particular, a negative gain in all forward and backward connections to the SPL from other regions was observed during the period of multisensory integration, while a positive gain was observed for forward projections that arise from the SPL.


Asunto(s)
Estimulación Acústica , Mapeo Encefálico , Potenciales Evocados/fisiología , Vías Nerviosas/fisiología , Lóbulo Parietal/fisiología , Estimulación Luminosa , Adulto , Electroencefalografía , Epilepsia/fisiopatología , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Modelos Teóricos , Tiempo de Reacción
5.
Transl Psychiatry ; 6: e804, 2016 05 10.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27163205

RESUMEN

Neurophysiological investigations in patients with schizophrenia consistently show early sensory processing deficits in the visual system. Importantly, comparable sensory deficits have also been established in healthy first-degree biological relatives of patients with schizophrenia and in first-episode drug-naive patients. The clear implication is that these measures are endophenotypic, related to the underlying genetic liability for schizophrenia. However, there is significant overlap between patient response distributions and those of healthy individuals without affected first-degree relatives. Here we sought to develop more sensitive measures of sensory dysfunction in this population, with an eye to establishing endophenotypic markers with better predictive capabilities. We used a sensory adaptation paradigm in which electrophysiological responses to basic visual and somatosensory stimuli presented at different rates (ranging from 250 to 2550 ms interstimulus intervals, in blocked presentations) were compared. Our main hypothesis was that adaptation would be substantially diminished in schizophrenia, and that this would be especially prevalent in the visual system. High-density event-related potential recordings showed amplitude reductions in sensory adaptation in patients with schizophrenia (N=15 Experiment 1, N=12 Experiment 2) compared with age-matched healthy controls (N=15 Experiment 1, N=12 Experiment 2), and this was seen for both sensory modalities. At the individual participant level, reduced adaptation was more robust for visual compared with somatosensory stimulation. These results point to significant impairments in short-term sensory plasticity across sensory modalities in schizophrenia. These simple-to-execute measures may prove valuable as candidate endophenotypes and will bear follow-up in future work.


Asunto(s)
Potenciales Evocados Somatosensoriales/fisiología , Potenciales Evocados Visuales/fisiología , Trastornos Psicóticos/fisiopatología , Esquizofrenia/fisiopatología , Adulto , Estudios de Casos y Controles , Cognición , Endofenotipos , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Estimulación Luminosa , Estimulación Física , Adulto Joven
6.
J Neurosci ; 20(6): RC63, 2000 Mar 15.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10704517

RESUMEN

Alpha-band (8-14 Hz) oscillatory EEG activity was examined with high-density scalp electrical recording during the cue-stimulus interval of an endogenous spatial cueing paradigm. In different blocks, cued spatial locations (left or right) were in either the upper or lower visual field, and attended stimuli were either oriented Ts or moving dots. Distractor stimuli were equally likely in the uncued hemifield. Sustained focal increases of alpha-band activity were seen over occipital cortex contralateral to the direction of the to-be-ignored location (ipsilateral to the cued direction of attention) before onset of the to-be-attended stimulus. The focus of alpha-band activity also moved depending on whether cued locations were in the upper or lower field. Results are consistent with active gating of uncued spatial locations.


Asunto(s)
Atención/fisiología , Lóbulo Occipital/fisiología , Retina/fisiología , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Adulto , Ritmo alfa , Mapeo Encefálico , Señales (Psicología) , Potenciales Evocados , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Estimulación Luminosa , Campos Visuales
7.
Neuropsychologia ; 39(8): 828-44, 2001.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11369406

RESUMEN

The timecourse and scalp topography of interactions between neural responses to stimuli in different visual quadrants, straddling either the vertical or horizontal meridian, were studied in 15 subjects. Visual evoked potentials (VEPs) were recorded from 64 electrodes during a simple reaction time (RT) task. VEPs to single stimuli displayed in different quadrants were summed ('sum') and compared to the VEP response from simultaneous stimulation of the same two quadrants ('pair'). These responses would be equivalent if the neural responses to the single stimuli were independent. Divergence between the 'pair' and 'sum' VEPs indicates a neural response interaction. In each visual field, interactions occurred within 72-86 ms post-stimulus over parieto-occipital brain regions. Independent of visual quadrant, RTs were faster for stimulus pairs than single stimuli. This replicates the redundant target effect (RTE) observed for bilateral stimulus pairs and generalizes the RTE to unilateral stimulus pairs. Using Miller's 'race' model inequality (Miller J. Divided attention: evidence for coactivation with redundant signals, Cognitive Psychology 1982;14:247-79), we found that probability summation could fully account for the RTE in each visual field. Although measurements from voltage waveforms replicated the observation of earlier peak P1 latencies for the 'pair' versus 'sum' comparison (Miniussi C, Girelli M, Marzi CA. Neural site of the redundant target effect: electrophysiological evidence. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 1998;10:216-30), this did not hold with measurements taken from second derivative (scalp current density) waveforms. Since interaction effects for bilateral stimulus pairs occurred within 86 ms and require interhemispheric transfer, transcallosal volleys must arrive within 86 ms, which is earlier than previously calculated. Interaction effects for bilateral conditions were delayed by approximately 10 ms versus unilateral conditions, consistent with current estimates of interhemispheric transmission time. Interaction effects place an upper limit on the time required for neuronal ensembles to combine inputs from different quadrants of visual space ( approximately 72 ms for unilateral and approximately 82 ms for bilateral conditions).


Asunto(s)
Electroencefalografía , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Percepción Espacial/fisiología , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Adulto , Mapeo Encefálico , Electrofisiología , Femenino , Lateralidad Funcional/fisiología , Humanos , Masculino , Estimulación Luminosa , Retina/fisiología , Campos Visuales/fisiología
8.
Front Biosci ; 6: D672-84, 2001 May 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11333209

RESUMEN

This review contrasts the most-studied variety of attention, visuospatial attention, with several types of nonspatial visual attention. We: 1) discuss the manner in which spatial and nonspatial varieties of attention are experimentally defined, and the ecological validity of the paradigms in which they are studied, 2) review and compare differing effects of spatial and nonspatial attention on neural processing, 3) discuss the manner in which attention operates within the framework of an anatomical visual hierarchy, as well as 4) how attention relates to the temporal dynamics of visual processing, 5) describe cellular circuits and physiological processes that appear to be involved in attention effects, 6) discuss the relationship of attentional physiology to the perceptual and cognitive effects of attention, and 7) consider the strengths and limitations of several current models of selective attention. Throughout, we attempt to integrate the findings of monkey and human studies whenever possible. We have three main conclusions. First, two models, the Neural Specificity Model of Harter and colleagues and the Feature Similarity Gain Model of Treue and colleagues best incorporate findings in relation to both spatial and nonspatial varieties of attention. Significantly, these models explicitly note that the specific neuronal components used in attentional modulation of processing are flexible and determined by task demands. Second, current evidence also provides strong bases for deriving testable hypotheses about the specific brain mechanisms utilized by attention. Cellular processes, brain circuits and neurotransmitter components can and should be incorporated into our models of attention. Finally, it is increasingly evident that we can and should analyze temporal patterns of attentional modulation, both within and across brain areas. These patterns provide critical information on the dynamics of attention.


Asunto(s)
Atención/fisiología , Vías Nerviosas/fisiología , Animales , Cognición/fisiología , Humanos , Corteza Visual/fisiología , Vías Visuales/fisiología , Percepción Visual/fisiología
9.
Brain Res Cogn Brain Res ; 12(1): 75-88, 2001 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11489611

RESUMEN

Studies in primates have found visually responsive neurons that are distributed beyond cortical areas typically described as directly involved in vision. Among these areas are premotor cortex, supplementary motor area, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and frontal eye fields. Given these findings, visual stimulation would be expected to result in activation of human frontal cortex. However, few human studies have described sensory activations in frontal regions in response to simple visual stimulation. Such studies have classically described event-related potential (ERP) components over occipital regions. The present study sought to further characterize the spatiotemporal dynamics of visually-evoked electrocortical responses elicited by simple visual stimuli using scalp current density measures derived from high-density ERP recordings, with particular emphasis on the distribution of stimulus-related activity over frontal cortex. Hemiretinal stimuli were viewed passively and during a simple ipsi- or contramanual (RT) task. The motor requirement was included to investigate the effects of response preparation on premovement frontal activations. The results indicate early frontocentral activation, particularly over the right hemisphere (peak magnitude 124-148 ms) that is independent of input visual field or motor response requirement, and that is clearly separate in timecourse from the posterior responses elicited by visual input. These findings are in accord with the multiplicity of visual inputs to frontal cortex and are discussed in terms of frontal lobe functions as may be required in these tasks.


Asunto(s)
Mapeo Encefálico , Corteza Cerebral/fisiología , Lóbulo Frontal/fisiología , Lóbulo Occipital/fisiología , Adulto , Electrodos , Electroencefalografía , Lateralidad Funcional/fisiología , Humanos , Individualidad , Masculino , Estimulación Luminosa , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología
10.
Brain Res Cogn Brain Res ; 10(1-2): 77-83, 2000 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10978694

RESUMEN

We investigated the time-course and scalp topography of multisensory interactions between simultaneous auditory and somatosensory stimulation in humans. Event-related potentials (ERPs) were recorded from 64 scalp electrodes while subjects were presented with auditory-alone stimulation (1000-Hz tones), somatosensory-alone stimulation (median nerve electrical pulses), and simultaneous auditory-somatosensory (AS) combined stimulation. Interaction effects were assessed by comparing the responses to combined stimulation with the algebraic sum of responses to the constituent auditory and somatosensory stimuli when they were presented alone. Spatiotemporal analysis of ERPs and scalp current density (SCD) topographies revealed AS interaction over the central/postcentral scalp which onset at approximately 50 ms post-stimulus presentation. Both the topography and timing of these interactions are consistent with multisensory integration early in the cortical processing hierarchy, in brain regions traditionally held to be unisensory.


Asunto(s)
Corteza Auditiva/fisiología , Mapeo Encefálico/métodos , Potenciales Evocados Auditivos/fisiología , Potenciales Evocados Somatosensoriales/fisiología , Corteza Somatosensorial/fisiología , Estimulación Acústica , Adulto , Estimulación Eléctrica , Electroencefalografía , Electrofisiología , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Nervio Mediano/fisiología , Cuero Cabelludo/fisiología , Factores de Tiempo
11.
Brain Res Cogn Brain Res ; 12(1): 145-52, 2001 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11489617

RESUMEN

Recent studies show that in addition to enhancing neural processing for attentionally relevant stimuli, selective attention also operates by suppressing the processing of distracter stimuli. When subjects are pre-cued to selectively deploy attention during voluntary (endogenous) attentional tasks, these mechanisms can be set up in advance of actual stimulus processing. That is, the brain can be placed in a biased attentional state. Two recent cueing studies have provided evidence for the deployment of such biased attentional states [J.J. Foxe, G.V. Simpson, S.P. Ahlfors, Neuroreport 9 (1998) 3929-3933; M.S. Worden, J.J. Foxe, N. Wang, G.V. Simpson, J. Neurosci. 20:RC63 (2000) 1-6]. Specifically, these studies implicated oscillatory activity in the alpha frequency-band (8-14 Hz) as an anticipatory mechanism for suppressing distracter visual stimulation. The current study extends these findings by showing that this alpha-suppressive effect is also invoked by cross-modal cues. Auditory symbolic cues were used in an intermodal attention task, to direct subjects' attention to a subsequent task in either the visual or auditory modality. Cueing attention to the auditory features of the imminent task stimuli resulted in significantly higher parieto-occipital alpha amplitude in the period preceding onset of this stimulus than when attention was cued to the visual features. Topographic mapping suggests that this effect is generated in regions of the inferior parietal cortex, areas that have been repeatedly implicated in the engagement and maintenance of visual attention. Taken together, the results of this series of studies suggest that these parietal regions are capable of integrating sensory cues from multiple sensory modalities in order to program the subsequent deployment of visual attention.


Asunto(s)
Ritmo alfa , Atención/fisiología , Lóbulo Occipital/fisiología , Lóbulo Parietal/fisiología , Estimulación Acústica , Adulto , Señales (Psicología) , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Estimulación Luminosa
12.
Brain Res Cogn Brain Res ; 20(2): 132-43, 2004 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15183386

RESUMEN

We used fMRI to study the brain processes involved in the executive control of behavior. The Sustained Attention to Response Task (SART), which allows unpredictable and predictable NOGO events to be contrasted, was imaged using a mixed (block and event-related) fMRI design to examine tonic and phasic processes involved in response inhibition, error detection, conflict monitoring and sustained attention. A network of regions, including right ventral prefrontal cortex (PFC), left dorsolateral PFC (DLPFC) and right inferior parietal cortex, was activated for successful unpredictable inhibitions, while rostral anterior cingulate was implicated in error processing and the pre-SMA in conflict monitoring. Furthermore, the pattern of correlations between left dorsolateral PFC, implicated in task-set maintenance, and the pre-SMA were indicative of a tight coupling between prefrontally mediated control and conflict levels monitored more posteriorly. The results reveal that the executive control of behavior can be separated into distinct functions performed by discrete cortical regions.


Asunto(s)
Atención/fisiología , Mapeo Encefálico , Giro del Cíngulo/fisiología , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Corteza Prefrontal/fisiología , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Corteza Motora/fisiología , Inhibición Neural/fisiología , Lóbulo Parietal/fisiología , Corteza Visual/fisiología
13.
Neuroreport ; 12(17): 3815-20, 2001 Dec 04.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11726801

RESUMEN

Integrity of early visual sensory processing in schizophrenia was assessed using the well characterized P1 and N1 components of the visual evoked potential (VEP) as our dependent measures. VEPs were recorded in response to successively less fragmented line drawings of common objects. P1 amplitudes were significantly reduced across all stimulus conditions for patients versus controls. Further, this decrement was relatively greater at parieto-occipital than occipito-temporal electrode sites. No differences in N1 amplitude were found. The finding of P1 deficits in patients, particularly over dorsal scalp, supports the view that schizophrenia is associated with impairment of early dorsal visual stream processing. On the other hand, the finding of normal N1 amplitudes in patients suggests that early stages of ventral stream processing may be relatively more intact. These results imply that the cognitive impairment seen in schizophrenia is not just due to deficits in higher order aspects of cognition but also encompasses significant deficits in early sensory processing.


Asunto(s)
Mapeo Encefálico/métodos , Electroencefalografía/métodos , Potenciales Evocados Visuales/fisiología , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología , Esquizofrenia/fisiopatología , Corteza Visual/fisiopatología , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Estimulación Luminosa , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Esquizofrenia/patología , Corteza Visual/patología
14.
Neuroreport ; 15(6): 1079-87, 2004 Apr 29.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15076739

RESUMEN

Task-switching paradigms, which are regularly used to assay 'executive control' processes in humans, almost invariably reveal a decrement in subjects' performance on the first trial following a switch of task. That is, subjects are slower to respond and more error prone on the switch trial, a difference in performance that has been termed the 'switch-cost'. This switch cost has then been taken to reflect the time taken by neural control processes. Previous studies have shown that while performance improves as more time is provided to prepare for the switch, switch costs persist, even over very long intervals. In the present study, however, we find that changing the response regimen (choice reaction time vs go-no-go) has profound effects on the switch cost. A task switching paradigm was used in which subjects randomly switched between two tasks, based on a cue that was presented at varying intervals prior to the presentation of the imperative stimulus. While switch costs were found in all conditions in the choice reaction time blocks, they were completely abolished in the go-no-go blocks when sufficient preparation time was provided (500 or 800 ms). This is important because the only difference between the choice reaction time and go-no-go conditions was the response requirement: these conditions did not differ in the stimuli used, in the tasks performed or in the preparation time provided. These data call into question models of executive control that interpret switch costs as reflecting the time taken by neural processes to switch the system from a readiness to perform one task to a readiness to perform another.


Asunto(s)
Conducta de Elección/fisiología , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Adulto , Análisis de Varianza , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino
15.
Neuroreport ; 9(17): 3929-33, 1998 Dec 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9875731

RESUMEN

High-density eeg recordings revealed sensory specific modulation of anticipatory parieto-occipital approximately 10 Hz oscillatory activity when visually presented word cues instructed subjects in an intermodal selective attention paradigm. Cueing attention to the auditory features of imminent compound audio-visual stimuli resulted in significantly higher approximately 10 Hz amplitude in the period preceding onset of this stimulus than when attention was cued to the visual features. We propose that this parieto-occipital approximately 10 Hz activity reflects a disengaged visual attentional system in preparation for anticipated auditory input that is attentionally more relevant. Conversely, lower approximately 10 Hz activity during the attend-visual condition may reflect active engagement of parieto-occipital areas in the anticipatory period. These results support models implicating parieto-occipital areas in the directing and maintenance of visual attention.


Asunto(s)
Atención/fisiología , Percepción Auditiva/fisiología , Señales (Psicología) , Electroencefalografía , Lóbulo Occipital/fisiología , Lóbulo Parietal/fisiología , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Modelos Lineales , Masculino
16.
J Clin Neurophysiol ; 12(5): 432-49, 1995 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8576389

RESUMEN

To fully characterize the brain processes underlying sensorimotor and cognitive function, the spatial distribution of active regions, their interconnected regions must be measured. We describe methods for imaging brain sources from surface-recorded EEG and magnetoencephalographic data, called electromagnetic source imaging (EMSI). EMSI provides brain source locations within the common framework of magnetic resonance (MR) images of brain anatomy. This allows integration of data from other functional brain imaging methods, like positron emission tomography and functional MR imaging, which can improve the accuracy of EMSI localization. EMSI also provides submillisecond temporal resolution of the dynamic processes within brain systems. Examples are given of applications to visual perceptual and attentional studies.


Asunto(s)
Nivel de Alerta/fisiología , Atención/fisiología , Mapeo Encefálico/métodos , Encéfalo/fisiología , Diagnóstico por Imagen/métodos , Corteza Cerebral/fisiología , Humanos , Procesamiento de Imagen Asistido por Computador , Percepción Visual/fisiología
17.
Neuroscience ; 273: 100-17, 2014 Jul 25.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24836852

RESUMEN

Task execution almost always occurs in the context of reward-seeking or punishment-avoiding behavior. As such, ongoing task-monitoring systems are influenced by reward anticipation systems. In turn, when a task has been executed either successfully or unsuccessfully, future iterations of that task will be re-titrated on the basis of the task outcome. Here, we examined the neural underpinnings of the task-monitoring and reward-evaluation systems to better understand how they govern reward-seeking behavior. Twenty-three healthy adult participants performed a task where they accrued points that equated to real world value (gift cards) by responding as rapidly as possible within an allotted timeframe, while success rate was titrated online by changing the duration of the timeframe dependent on participant performance. Informative cues initiated each trial, indicating the probability of potential reward or loss (four levels from very low to very high). We manipulated feedback by first informing participants of task success/failure, after which a second feedback signal indicated actual magnitude of reward/loss. High-density electroencephalography (EEG) recordings allowed for examination of event-related potentials (ERPs) to the informative cues and in turn, to both feedback signals. Distinct ERP components associated with reward cues, task-preparatory and task-monitoring processes, and reward feedback processes were identified. Unsurprisingly, participants displayed increased ERP amplitudes associated with task-preparatory processes following cues that predicted higher chances of reward. They also rapidly updated reward and loss prediction information dependent on task performance after the first feedback signal. Finally, upon reward receipt, initial reward probability was no longer taken into account. Rather, ERP measures suggested that only the magnitude of actual reward or loss was now processed. Reward and task-monitoring processes are clearly dissociable, but interact across very fast timescales to update reward predictions as information about task success or failure is accrued. Careful delineation of these processes will be useful in future investigations in clinical groups where such processes are suspected of having gone awry.


Asunto(s)
Anticipación Psicológica/fisiología , Encéfalo/fisiología , Función Ejecutiva/fisiología , Retroalimentación Psicológica/fisiología , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Recompensa , Adulto , Mapeo Encefálico , Señales (Psicología) , Electroencefalografía , Potenciales Evocados , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Probabilidad , Esquema de Refuerzo
18.
Transl Psychiatry ; 4: e345, 2014 Jan 14.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24424391

RESUMEN

The common variant rs1344706 within the zinc-finger protein gene ZNF804A has been strongly implicated in schizophrenia (SZ) susceptibility by a series of recent genetic association studies. Although associated with a pattern of altered neural connectivity, evidence that increased risk is mediated by an effect on cognitive deficits associated with the disorder has been equivocal. This study investigated whether the same ZNF804A risk allele was associated with variation in the P300 auditory-evoked response, a cognitively relevant putative endophenotype for SZ. We compared P300 responses in carriers and noncarriers of the ZNF804A risk allele genotype groups in Irish patients and controls (n=97). P300 response was observed to vary according to genotype in this sample, such that risk allele carriers showed relatively higher P300 response compared with noncarriers. This finding accords with behavioural data reported by our group and others. It is also consistent with the idea that ZNF804A may have an impact on cortical efficiency, reflected in the higher levels of activations required to achieve comparable behavioural accuracy on the task used.


Asunto(s)
Potenciales Relacionados con Evento P300/genética , Potenciales Evocados Auditivos/genética , Factores de Transcripción de Tipo Kruppel/genética , Esquizofrenia/genética , Adulto , Electroencefalografía , Endofenotipos , Potenciales Relacionados con Evento P300/fisiología , Potenciales Evocados Auditivos/fisiología , Femenino , Genotipo , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Polimorfismo de Nucleótido Simple , Esquizofrenia/fisiopatología
19.
Neuroscience ; 221: 1-11, 2012 Sep 27.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22521825

RESUMEN

To reveal the fundamental processes underlying the different stages of visual object perception, most studies have manipulated relatively complex images, such as photographs, line drawings of natural objects, or perceptual illusions. Here, rather than starting from complex images and working backward to infer simpler processes, we investigated how the visual system parses and integrates information contained in stimuli of the most basic variety. Simple scatterings of a few points of light were manipulated in terms of their numerosity, spatial extent, and organization, and high-density electrophysiological recordings were made from healthy adults engaged in an unrelated task. We reasoned that this approach permitted an uncontaminated view of the spatio-temporal dynamics of the related neural processes. We were guided in our predictions by the "frame-and-fill" model for object perception, whereby fast inputs to the dorsal stream of the visual "where" system first frame the spatial extent of visual objects, which are subsequently "filled-in" by the slower activation of the ventral stream of the visual "what" system. Our findings were consistent with this view, showing a rapidly-onsetting effect of spatial extent in dorsal stream sources, and later-onsetting effects due to dot number and symmetry, which were deemed to be more closely tied to the details of object identity, from ventral stream sources. This collection of observations provides an important baseline from which to understand the spatio-temporal properties of basic visual object perception, and from which to test dysfunction of this system in clinical populations.


Asunto(s)
Mapeo Encefálico , Corteza Cerebral/fisiología , Potenciales Evocados Visuales/fisiología , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología , Campos Visuales/fisiología , Adulto , Análisis de Varianza , Electroencefalografía , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Estimulación Luminosa , Tiempo de Reacción , Vías Visuales/fisiología , Adulto Joven
20.
Neuroscience ; 218: 226-34, 2012 Aug 30.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22683721

RESUMEN

The VESPA (visual-evoked spread spectrum analysis) method derives an impulse response function of the visual system from scalp electroencephalographic (EEG) data using the controlled modulation of some feature of a visual stimulus. Recent research using VESPA responses to modulations of stimulus contrast has provided new insights into both early visual attention mechanisms and the specificity of visual-processing deficits in schizophrenia. To allow a fuller interpretation of these and future findings, it is necessary to further characterize the VESPA in terms of its underlying cortical generators. To that end, we here examine spatio-temporal variations in the components of the VESPA as a function of stimulus location. We found that the first two VESPA components (C1/P1) each have a posterior dorsal midline focus and reverse in polarity across the horizontal meridian, consistent with retinotopic projections to calcarine cortex (V1) for the stimulus locations tested. Furthermore, the focal scalp topography of the VESPA was strikingly constant across the entire C1-P1 timeframe (50-120 ms) for each stimulus location, with negligible global scalp activity visible at the zero-crossing dividing the two. This indicates a common focal source underpinning both components, which was further supported by a significant correlation between C1 and P1 amplitudes across subjects (r=0.54; p<0.05). These results, along with factors implicit in the method of derivation of the contrast-VESPA, lead us to conclude that these responses are dominated by activity from striate cortex. We discuss the implications of this finding for previous and future research using the VESPA.


Asunto(s)
Mapeo Encefálico/métodos , Potenciales Evocados Visuales/fisiología , Corteza Visual/fisiología , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Adolescente , Adulto , Electroencefalografía , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Estimulación Luminosa , Vías Visuales/fisiología , Adulto Joven
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
Detalles de la búsqueda