Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 32
Filtrar
1.
J Child Psychol Psychiatry ; 62(8): 922-936, 2021 08.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33137209

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Evidence increasingly suggests that ASD manifests differently in females than males. Previous reviews investigating sex/gender differences in social interaction and social communication have focused at the level of broad constructs (e.g. comparing algorithm scores from pre-existing diagnostic instruments) and have typically reported no significant differences between males and females. However, a number of individual studies have found sex/gender differences in narrow construct domains. METHODS: We conducted a systematic review and random effects model meta-analyses (in January 2019 and updated January 2020) that investigated sex/gender differences in narrow construct measures of social communication and interaction in autistic and nonautistic children and adolescents, and adults. Study quality was appraised using the Appraisal Tool for Cross-Sectional Studies (AXIS, BMJ Open, 6, 2016, 1). RESULTS: Across 16 studies (including 2,730 participants), the analysis found that female (vs. male) individuals with ASD had significantly better social interaction and social communication skills (SMD = 0.39, p < .001), which was reflective of a similar sex/gender profile in nonautistic individuals (SMD = 0.35, p < .001). Nonautistic males had significantly better social interaction and communication than males with ASD (SMD = 0.77, p < .001). Nonautistic females also had significantly better social interaction and communication than females with ASD (SMD = 0.72, p  <.001). Nonautistic males had better social interaction and communication than females with ASD, though this difference was not significant (SMD = 0.30, p = .07). CONCLUSIONS: This systematic review and meta-analysis highlighted important sex/gender differences in social interaction and communication for individuals with ASD, likely not captured by pre-existing diagnostic instruments, which potentially contribute to the under recognition of autism in females, and may need to be reflected in the diagnostic process.


Asunto(s)
Trastorno del Espectro Autista , Trastorno Autístico , Adolescente , Adulto , Niño , Comunicación , Estudios Transversales , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Factores Sexuales , Interacción Social
2.
Exp Brain Res ; 237(4): 897-909, 2019 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30656352

RESUMEN

Anxiety has been associated with poor attentional control, as reflected in lowered performance on experimental measures of executive attention and inhibitory control. Recent conceptualisations of anxiety propose that individuals who report elevated anxiety symptoms worry about performance and will exert greater cognitive effort to complete tasks well, particularly when cognitive demands are high. Across two experiments, we examined the effect of anxiety on task performance and across two load conditions using (1) measures of inhibitory control (behavioural reaction times and eye-movement responses) and (2) task effort with pupillary and electrocortical markers of effort (CNV) and inhibitory control (N2). Experiment 1 used an oculomotor-delayed-response task that manipulated load by increasing delay duration to create a high load, relative to a low load, condition. Experiment 2 used a Go/No-Go task and load was manipulated by decreasing the No-Go probabilities (i.e., 20% No-Go in the high load condition and 50% No-Go in the low load condition). Experiment 1 showed individuals with high (vs. low) anxiety made more antisaccade errors across load conditions, and made more effort during the high load condition, as evidenced by greater frontal CNV and increased pupillary responses. In Experiment 2, individuals with high anxiety showed increased effort (irrespective of cognitive load), as characterised by larger pupillary responses. In addition, N2 amplitudes were sensitive to load only in individuals with low anxiety. Evidence of reduced performance effectiveness and efficiency across electrophysiological, pupillary, and oculomotor systems in anxiety provides some support for neurocognitive models of frontocortical attentional dysfunction in anxiety.


Asunto(s)
Corteza Cerebral/fisiología , Potenciales Evocados/fisiología , Función Ejecutiva/fisiología , Inhibición Psicológica , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Pupila/fisiología , Adulto , Electroencefalografía , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Adulto Joven
3.
Child Psychiatry Hum Dev ; 47(4): 554-62, 2016 08.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26424720

RESUMEN

This paper examined the reliability, convergent validity and factor structure of the self-report Screen for Child Anxiety Disorders (SCARED; Birmaher et al. in J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 36:545-553, 1997) in a large community sample of children and adolescents in Saudi Arabia. The questionnaire showed moderate to high internal consistency and satisfactory test-retest reliability over a 2 week period. In addition, there were significant positive correlations between reported anxiety symptoms with parent report behavioural difficulties. The five factor structure model of the SCARED also had a good model fit in this population. The results showed that self-report anxiety symptoms decreased with age (for boys and not girls) and were higher in adolescent girls. The results suggest that the SCARED could be useful in this population to identify individuals who are at risk of developing anxiety disorders in childhood with a view to implementing prevention and intervention methods to ensure positive developmental outcome over time.


Asunto(s)
Trastornos de Ansiedad/diagnóstico , Ansiedad/diagnóstico , Encuestas y Cuestionarios , Adolescente , Factores de Edad , Ansiedad/psicología , Trastornos de Ansiedad/psicología , Niño , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Psicometría/métodos , Reproducibilidad de los Resultados , Arabia Saudita , Autoinforme , Factores Sexuales
4.
Cogn Emot ; 26(5): 934-42, 2012.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22375840

RESUMEN

The current study explored the proposition that anxiety is associated with impaired inhibition of threat. Using a modified version of the remote distractor paradigm, we considered whether this impairment is related to attentional capture by threat, difficulties disengaging from threat presented within foveal vision, or difficulties orienting to task-relevant stimuli when threat is present in central, parafoveal and peripheral locations in the visual field. Participants were asked to direct their eyes towards and identify a target in the presence and absence of a distractor (an angry, happy or neutral face). Trait anxiety was associated with a delay in initiating eye movements to the target in the presence of central, parafoveal and peripheral threatening distractors. These findings suggest that elevated anxiety is linked to difficulties inhibiting task-irrelevant threat presented across a broad region of the visual field.


Asunto(s)
Ansiedad/psicología , Atención/fisiología , Inhibición Psicológica , Adolescente , Adulto , Ansiedad/fisiopatología , Movimientos Oculares/fisiología , Expresión Facial , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Campos Visuales/fisiología , Percepción Visual/fisiología
5.
JCPP Adv ; 2(1): e12059, 2022 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37431493

RESUMEN

Neurodevelopmental and mental health disorders are common in children and young people, and frequently continue to be impairing into adulthood. Biological sex and gender identity are important factors in the likelihood of the development, referral and diagnosis of these disorders, as well as sources of clinical heterogeneity. In this editorial, we highlight that the aetiology of neurodevelopmental and mental health disorders in young people is complex and multi-faceted. Emerging evidence has implicated sex and gender as playing an important role in understanding the onset and course of psychopathology in childhood and adolescence. Here, we underline the significance of continuing to actively study the roles of sex and gender in the context of outlining potential future research directions and understanding how best to support children and young people.

6.
J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol ; 40(6): 897-903, 2011.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22023281

RESUMEN

Aggression in young people has been associated with a bias toward attributing hostile intent to others. However, little is known about the origin of biased social information processing. The current study explored the potential role of peer contagion in the emergence of hostile attribution in adolescents. One hundred thirty-four adolescents (M age = 13.8 years) were assigned to one of two manipulated "chat-room" conditions, where they believed they were communicating with online peers (e-confederates) who endorsed either hostile or benign intent attributions. Adolescents showed increased hostile attributions following exposure to hostile e-confederates and reduced hostility in the benign condition. Further analyses demonstrated that social anxiety was associated with a reduced tendency to take on hostile peer attitudes. Neither gender nor levels of aggression influenced individual susceptibility to peer influence, but aggressive adolescents reported greater affinity with hostile e-confederates.


Asunto(s)
Conducta del Adolescente/psicología , Hostilidad , Relaciones Interpersonales , Grupo Paritario , Percepción Social , Adolescente , Agresión/psicología , Ansiedad/psicología , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Trastornos Fóbicos/psicología , Encuestas y Cuestionarios
7.
J Autism Dev Disord ; 51(4): 1353-1364, 2021 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32691191

RESUMEN

This study investigated sex/gender differences in camouflaging with children and adolescents (N = 84) with and without an autism diagnosis/increased levels of autistic traits using two conceptualisations/operationalisations of camouflaging. A significant group-by-gender interaction using ANCOVA, with the covariate of verbal IQ, reflected similar levels of social reciprocity in autistic and neurotypical females, whereas autistic males had lower reciprocity than neurotypical males. Autistic females also had higher reciprocity than autistic males, despite similar levels of autistic traits (behavioural camouflaging). Additionally, autistic males and females had similar theory of mind skills, despite females having increased reciprocity (compensatory camouflaging). These findings provide evidence of increased camouflaging in autistic females, which may contribute to delay in the recognition of difficulties and provision of support.


Asunto(s)
Adaptación Psicológica/fisiología , Trastorno Autístico/psicología , Caracteres Sexuales , Teoría de la Mente/fisiología , Adolescente , Trastorno Autístico/diagnóstico , Niño , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Reconocimiento en Psicología/fisiología , Factores Sexuales
8.
J Atten Disord ; 25(13): 1919-1930, 2021 11.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32513052

RESUMEN

Objective: This study examined the synergistic effects of ADHD and anxiety symptoms on attention and inhibitory control depending on the emotional content of the stimuli. Method: Fifty-four typically developing individuals (27 children/adolescents and 27 adults) completed an eye-movement based emotional Go/No-Go task, using centrally presented (happy, angry) faces and neutral/symbolic stimuli. Sustained attention was measured through saccade latencies and saccadic omission errors (Go trials), and inhibitory control through saccadic commission errors (No-Go trials). ADHD and anxiety were assessed dimensionally. Results: Elevated ADHD symptoms were associated with more commission errors and slower saccade latencies for angry (vs. happy) faces. In contrast, angry faces were linked to faster saccade onsets when anxiety symptoms were high, and this effect prevailed when both anxiety and ADHD symptoms were high. Conclusion: Social threat impacted performance in individuals with sub-clinical anxiety and ADHD differently. The effects of anxiety on threat processing prevailed when both symptoms were high.


Asunto(s)
Trastorno por Déficit de Atención con Hiperactividad , Adolescente , Adulto , Ansiedad , Atención , Niño , Emociones , Expresión Facial , Humanos , Tiempo de Reacción
9.
J Autism Dev Disord ; 50(5): 1470-1478, 2020 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30051187

RESUMEN

This study explored the social-cognitive profile of 173 adults referred for an autism assessment. We considered key dimensional traits (autism, empathy and systemising) to understand social cognition in adults diagnosed with an autism spectrum condition compared with those who were referred for, but did not receive a diagnosis. There were no significant social cognitive differences between groups on measures of emotion recognition and social inference. Adults with a confirmed diagnosis, however, reported fewer empathising traits which were positively associated with social-cognitive understanding. Empathising partially mediated the relationship between diagnostic group and social-cognition. Lower empathising traits in individuals diagnosed in adulthood may be important in understanding challenges with social adaptability. The findings have implications for assessment and highlight the role of empathy in developing social understanding in autism.


Asunto(s)
Trastorno del Espectro Autista/diagnóstico , Trastorno del Espectro Autista/psicología , Cognición , Empatía , Conducta Social , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Adulto Joven
10.
Br J Dev Psychol ; 27(Pt 2): 487-94, 2009 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19998543

RESUMEN

This study used an emotional face stroop task to investigate the effects of self-report trait anxiety, social concern (SC), and chronological age (CA) on reaction time to match coloured outlines of angry, happy, and neutral faces (and control faces with scrambled features) with coloured buttons in a community sample of 74 children aged 6-12 years. The results showed an interference of colour matching for angry (relative to neutral) faces in children with elevated SC. The same effect was not found for happy or control faces. In addition, the results suggest that selective attention to angry faces in children with social concern (SC) was not significantly moderated by age.


Asunto(s)
Ansiedad/fisiopatología , Atención/fisiología , Emociones/fisiología , Enmascaramiento Perceptual/fisiología , Niño , Conducta de Elección/fisiología , Conflicto Psicológico , Expresión Facial , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Estimulación Luminosa , Escalas de Valoración Psiquiátrica , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Percepción Visual/fisiología
11.
Evid Based Ment Health ; 22(3): 118-124, 2019 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31253603

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: The cortisol awakening response (CAR) is characterised by an increase in cortisol in the 30 to 60 min after waking. Research has found significant associations between an atypical CAR and symptoms of stress and anxiety in typically developing (TD) children and adolescents. A number of studies have explored the CAR in autism spectrum disorder (ASD), but no evidence synthesis is available to date. OBJECTIVE AND METHODS: Based on a preregistered protocol (PROSPERO: CRD42017051187), we carried out a systematic review (SR) and meta-analysis (MA) of CAR studies to explore potential significant differences between children and adolescents with ASD and TD controls. Web of Science, PubMed and PsychInfo were searched until January 2019. A random-effects model was used to pool studies and we used the Newcastle-Ottawa scale (NOS) to assess study quality and risk of bias. FINDINGS: The SR retrieved a total of nine studies, with mixed findings on the comparison of the CAR between children and adolescents with ASD and TD controls. The MA, based on four studies (ASD; n=117 and TD n=118), suggested no differences between the CAR in ASD and TD populations (SMD: -0.21, 95% CI -0.49 to 0.08). In terms of NOS items, no study specified Representativeness of the cases and Non-response rate. DISCUSSION AND CLINICAL IMPLICATIONS: Given the relatively few studies and lack of appropriately matched TD controls, additional research is needed to further understand and recommend the utility of the CAR as a reliable marker to differentiate ASD and TD.


Asunto(s)
Trastorno del Espectro Autista/metabolismo , Hidrocortisona/metabolismo , Adolescente , Niño , Humanos
12.
Psychol Assess ; 20(3): 305-9, 2008 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18778167

RESUMEN

In this study, we investigated a new instrument: the Southampton Test of Empathy for Preschoolers (STEP). The test incorporated 8 video vignettes of children in emotional scenarios, assessing a child's ability to understand (STEP-UND) and share (STEP-SHA) in the emotional experience of a story protagonist. Each vignette included 4 emotions (angry, happy, fearful, sad) that reflected emotion judgments based on the protagonist's facial expression, situation, verbal cues, and desire. The STEP was administered to 39 preschool children, and internal reliability, concurrent validity, and construct validity were addressed. The results showed good internal consistency. They also highlighted moderate concurrent validity with parent-rated empathy, a measure of facial indices, and construct validity with teacher-rated prosocial behavior.


Asunto(s)
Empatía , Pruebas Psicológicas , Preescolar , Humanos , Psicometría
13.
J Exp Psychol Appl ; 24(4): 564-577, 2018 Dec.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29389162

RESUMEN

Many real-world tasks now involve monitoring visual representations of data that change dynamically over time. Monitoring dynamically changing displays for the onset of targets can be done in two ways: detecting targets directly, post-onset, or predicting their onset from the prior state of distractors. In the present study, participants' eye movements were measured as they monitored arrays of 108 colored squares whose colors changed systematically over time. Across three experiments, the data show that participants detected the onset of targets both directly and predictively. Experiments 1 and 2 showed that predictive detection was only possible when supported by sequential color changes that followed a scale ordered in color space. Experiment 3 included measures of individual differences in working memory capacity (WMC) and anxious affect and a manipulation of target prevalence in the search task. It found that predictive monitoring for targets, and decisions about target onsets, were influenced by interactions between individual differences in verbal and spatial WMC and intolerance of uncertainty, a characteristic that reflects worry about uncertain future events. The results have implications for the selection of individuals tasked with monitoring dynamic visual displays for target onsets. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2018 APA, all rights reserved).


Asunto(s)
Atención/fisiología , Movimientos Oculares/fisiología , Individualidad , Memoria a Corto Plazo/fisiología , Incertidumbre , Adulto , Ansiedad/psicología , Percepción de Color/fisiología , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Estimulación Luminosa , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Adulto Joven
14.
Front Psychol ; 9: 125, 2018.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29515476

RESUMEN

Introduction: The emergence of anxiety during childhood is accompanied by the development of attentional biases to threat. However, the neural mechanisms underlying these biases are poorly understood. In addition, previous research has not examined whether state and trait anxiety are independently associated with threat-related biases. Methods: We compared ERP waveforms during the processing of emotional faces in a population sample of 58 6-11-year-olds who completed self-reported measures of trait and state anxiety and depression. Results: The results showed that the P1 was larger to angry than neutral faces in the left hemisphere, though early components (P1, N170) were not strongly associated with child anxiety or depression. In contrast, Late Positive Potential (LPP) amplitudes to angry (vs. neutral) faces were significantly and positively associated with symptoms of anxiety/depression. In addition, the difference between LPPs for angry (vs. neutral) faces was independently associated with state and trait anxiety symptoms. Discussion: The results showed that neural responses to facial emotion in children with elevated symptoms of anxiety and depression were most evident at later processing stages characterized as evaluative and effortful. The findings support cognitive models of threat perception in anxiety and indicate that trait elements of anxiety and more transitory fluctuations in anxious affect are important in understanding individual variation in the neural response to threat in late childhood.

15.
J Autism Dev Disord ; 47(12): 3896-3908, 2017 Dec.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27440250

RESUMEN

This study evaluated the effectiveness of a school-based Cognitive Behavioural Therapy (CBT) on symptoms of anxiety, social worry and social responsiveness, and indices of attentional control and attentional biases to threat in adolescents diagnosed with Autism Spectrum Disorder. Thirty-five young people (11-14 years; IQ > 70) with ASD and elevated teacher or parent reported anxiety were randomly assigned to 6 sessions of the Exploring Feelings CBT intervention (Attwood in Exploring feelings (anxiety). Future Horizons, Arlington, 2004) (n = 18) or a wait-list control group (n = 17). The intervention (compared to the wait-list control) group showed positive change for parent, teacher and self-reported anxiety symptoms, and more marginal effects of increased teacher-reported social responsiveness. The discussion highlights the potential value and limitations of school-based CBT for young people with ASD.


Asunto(s)
Ansiedad/terapia , Trastorno del Espectro Autista/terapia , Terapia Cognitivo-Conductual , Servicios de Salud Escolar , Adolescente , Ansiedad/complicaciones , Atención , Trastorno del Espectro Autista/complicaciones , Niño , Emociones , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Autoinforme
16.
Biol Psychol ; 127: 173-179, 2017 07.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28578187

RESUMEN

Anxious individuals report hyper-arousal and sensitivity to environmental stimuli, difficulties concentrating, performing tasks efficiently and inhibiting unwanted thoughts and distraction. We used pupillometry and eye-movement measures to compare high vs. low anxious individuals hyper-reactivity to emotional stimuli (facial expressions) and subsequent attentional biases in a memory-guided pro- and antisaccade task during conditions of low and high cognitive load (short vs. long delay). High anxious individuals produced larger and slower pupillary responses to face stimuli, and more erroneous eye-movements, particularly following long delay. Low anxious individuals' pupillary responses were sensitive to task demand (reduced during short delay), whereas high anxious individuals' were not. These findings provide evidence in anxiety of enhanced, sustained and inflexible patterns of pupil responding during affective stimulus processing and cognitive load that precede deficits in task performance.


Asunto(s)
Afecto/fisiología , Ansiedad/fisiopatología , Función Ejecutiva/fisiología , Pupila/fisiología , Movimientos Sacádicos/fisiología , Ansiedad/psicología , Expresión Facial , Reconocimiento Facial/fisiología , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Análisis y Desempeño de Tareas , Adulto Joven
17.
Clin Psychol Rev ; 26(7): 876-94, 2006 Nov.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16524655

RESUMEN

The aim of this paper is to explore parenting as one potential route through which information processing biases for threat develop in children. It reviews information processing biases in childhood anxiety in the context of theoretical models and empirical research in the adult anxiety literature. Specifically, it considers how adult models have been used and adapted to develop a theoretical framework with which to investigate information processing biases in children. The paper then considers research which specifically aims to understand the relationship between parenting and the development of information processing biases in children. It concludes that a clearer theoretical framework is required to understand the significance of information biases in childhood anxiety, as well as their origins in parenting.


Asunto(s)
Trastornos de Ansiedad/psicología , Desarrollo Infantil , Procesos Mentales , Responsabilidad Parental/psicología , Adolescente , Atención , Niño , Humanos , Memoria , Modelos Psicológicos , Psicología del Adolescente/métodos , Psicología del Adolescente/tendencias , Psicología Infantil/métodos , Psicología Infantil/tendencias , Estrés Psicológico/psicología
18.
Front Psychol ; 7: 47, 2016.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26869956

RESUMEN

Research indicates that cognitive processes linked to the detection of threat stimuli are associated with poor attentional control, placing children and adolescents at increased risk for the development of anxious affect. The current study aimed to provide preliminary data to assess whether an intervention designed to improve attentional control (via working memory; WM) would lead to better performance in tests of WM and would be associated with positive changes in symptoms of trait and test anxiety, increased inhibitory control and reduced attention to threat. Forty adolescents aged 11-14 years who reported elevated anxiety and low attentional control were randomly allocated to a WM training or an active cognitive behavioural therapy (CBT) control group. Post intervention, WM training was associated with greater improvements (versus. CBT) in trained WM tasks. Both groups, however, reported fewer anxiety symptoms, demonstrated increased inhibitory control and a reduction in attentional biases to threat post intervention and these results were maintained at follow up. The study provides indicative evidence which suggests that WM training has similar benefits to a more traditional CBT intervention on reduced anxiety and attentional biases for threat. Future research should aim to replicate the findings in a large sample size and explore the broader impact of training on day-to-day functioning. In addition, further research is needed to identify which participants benefit most from different interventions (using baseline characteristics) on treatment compliance and outcome.

19.
Br J Dev Psychol ; 33(2): 218-36, 2015 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25492258

RESUMEN

Sensitivity to facial and vocal emotion is fundamental to children's social competence. Previous research has focused on children's facial emotion recognition, and few studies have investigated non-linguistic vocal emotion processing in childhood. We compared facial and vocal emotion recognition and processing biases in 4- to 11-year-olds and adults. Eighty-eight 4- to 11-year-olds and 21 adults participated. Participants viewed/listened to faces and voices (angry, happy, and sad) at three intensity levels (50%, 75%, and 100%). Non-linguistic tones were used. For each modality, participants completed an emotion identification task. Accuracy and bias for each emotion and modality were compared across 4- to 5-, 6- to 9- and 10- to 11-year-olds and adults. The results showed that children's emotion recognition improved with age; preschoolers were less accurate than other groups. Facial emotion recognition reached adult levels by 11 years, whereas vocal emotion recognition continued to develop in late childhood. Response bias decreased with age. For both modalities, sadness recognition was delayed across development relative to anger and happiness. The results demonstrate that developmental trajectories of emotion processing differ as a function of emotion type and stimulus modality. In addition, vocal emotion processing showed a more protracted developmental trajectory, compared to facial emotion processing. The results have important implications for programmes aiming to improve children's socio-emotional competence.


Asunto(s)
Desarrollo Infantil/fisiología , Emociones/fisiología , Expresión Facial , Comunicación no Verbal/fisiología , Adulto , Niño , Preescolar , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Reconocimiento en Psicología , Adulto Joven
20.
Child Neuropsychol ; 21(1): 25-40, 2015.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24344768

RESUMEN

Facial emotion-recognition difficulties have been reported in school-aged children with behavior problems; little is known, however, about either this association in preschool children or with regard to vocal emotion recognition. The current study explored the association between facial and vocal emotion recognition and behavior problems in a sample of 3 to 6-year-old children. A sample of 57 children enriched for risk of behavior problems (41 were recruited from the general population while 16 had been referred for behavior problems to local clinics) were each presented with a series of vocal and facial stimuli expressing different emotions (i.e., angry, happy, and sad) of low and high intensity. Parents rated children's externalizing and internalizing behavior problems. Vocal and facial emotion recognition accuracy was negatively correlated with externalizing but not internalizing behavior problems independent of emotion type. The effects with the externalizing domain were independently associated with hyperactivity rather than conduct problems. The results highlight the importance of using vocal as well as facial stimuli when studying the relationship between emotion-recognition and behavior problems. Future studies should test the hypothesis that difficulties in responding to adult instructions and commands seen in children with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) may be due to deficits in the processing of vocal emotions.


Asunto(s)
Trastorno por Déficit de Atención con Hiperactividad/psicología , Emociones , Expresión Facial , Reconocimiento en Psicología , Voz , Adulto , Análisis de Varianza , Trastorno por Déficit de Atención con Hiperactividad/diagnóstico , Niño , Preescolar , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Encuestas y Cuestionarios
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
Detalles de la búsqueda