Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 13 de 13
Filtrar
1.
Child Dev ; 92(3): 1083-1098, 2021 05.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32851656

RESUMEN

Phonological difficulties characterize children with developmental dyslexia across languages, but whether impaired auditory processing underlies these phonological difficulties is debated. Here the causal question is addressed by exploring whether individual differences in sensory processing predict the development of phonological awareness in 86 English-speaking lower- and middle-class children aged 8 years in 2005 who had dyslexia, or were age-matched typically developing children, some with exceptional reading/high IQ. The predictive relations between auditory processing and phonological development are robust for this sample even when phonological awareness at Time 1 (the autoregressor) is controlled. High reading/IQ does not much impact these relations. The data suggest that basic sensory abilities are significant longitudinal predictors of growth in phonological awareness in children.


Asunto(s)
Dislexia , Fonética , Percepción Auditiva , Niño , Humanos , Estudios Longitudinales , Lectura
2.
Neuroimage ; 143: 40-49, 2016 Dec.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27520749

RESUMEN

Phase entrainment of neuronal oscillations is thought to play a central role in encoding speech. Children with developmental dyslexia show impaired phonological processing of speech, proposed theoretically to be related to atypical phase entrainment to slower temporal modulations in speech (<10Hz). While studies of children with dyslexia have found atypical phase entrainment in the delta band (~2Hz), some studies of adults with developmental dyslexia have shown impaired entrainment in the low gamma band (~35-50Hz). Meanwhile, studies of neurotypical adults suggest asymmetric temporal sensitivity in auditory cortex, with preferential processing of slower modulations by right auditory cortex, and faster modulations processed bilaterally. Here we compared neural entrainment to slow (2Hz) versus faster (40Hz) amplitude-modulated noise using fNIRS to study possible hemispheric asymmetry effects in children with developmental dyslexia. We predicted atypical right hemisphere responding to 2Hz modulations for the children with dyslexia in comparison to control children, but equivalent responding to 40Hz modulations in both hemispheres. Analyses of HbO concentration revealed a right-lateralised region focused on the supra-marginal gyrus that was more active in children with dyslexia than in control children for 2Hz stimulation. We discuss possible links to linguistic prosodic processing, and interpret the data with respect to a neural 'temporal sampling' framework for conceptualizing the phonological deficits that characterise children with developmental dyslexia across languages.


Asunto(s)
Dislexia/diagnóstico por imagen , Dislexia/fisiopatología , Lateralidad Funcional/fisiología , Pruebas del Lenguaje , Espectroscopía Infrarroja Corta/métodos , Adolescente , Niño , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino
3.
Dyslexia ; 20(3): 261-79, 2014 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25044949

RESUMEN

The core cognitive difficulty in developmental dyslexia involves phonological processing, but adults and children with dyslexia also have sensory impairments. Impairments in basic auditory processing show particular links with phonological impairments, and recent studies with dyslexic children across languages reveal a relationship between auditory temporal processing and sensitivity to rhythmic timing and speech rhythm. As rhythm is explicit in music, musical training might have a beneficial effect on the auditory perception of acoustic cues to rhythm in dyslexia. Here we took advantage of the presence of musicians with and without dyslexia in musical conservatoires, comparing their auditory temporal processing abilities with those of dyslexic non-musicians matched for cognitive ability. Musicians with dyslexia showed equivalent auditory sensitivity to musicians without dyslexia and also showed equivalent rhythm perception. The data support the view that extensive rhythmic experience initiated during childhood (here in the form of music training) can affect basic auditory processing skills which are found to be deficient in individuals with dyslexia.


Asunto(s)
Percepción Auditiva/fisiología , Cognición/fisiología , Dislexia/fisiopatología , Música , Fonética , Análisis y Desempeño de Tareas , Adulto , Niño , Femenino , Humanos , Lenguaje , Lingüística , Masculino , Desempeño Psicomotor , Lectura
4.
J Cogn Neurosci ; 23(2): 325-37, 2011 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20146613

RESUMEN

Studies in sensory neuroscience reveal the critical importance of accurate sensory perception for cognitive development. There is considerable debate concerning the possible sensory correlates of phonological processing, the primary cognitive risk factor for developmental dyslexia. Across languages, children with dyslexia have a specific difficulty with the neural representation of the phonological structure of speech. The identification of a robust sensory marker of phonological difficulties would enable early identification of risk for developmental dyslexia and early targeted intervention. Here, we explore whether phonological processing difficulties are associated with difficulties in processing acoustic cues to speech rhythm. Speech rhythm is used across languages by infants to segment the speech stream into words and syllables. Early difficulties in perceiving auditory sensory cues to speech rhythm and prosody could lead developmentally to impairments in phonology. We compared matched samples of children with and without dyslexia, learning three very different spoken and written languages, English, Spanish, and Chinese. The key sensory cue measured was rate of onset of the amplitude envelope (rise time), known to be critical for the rhythmic timing of speech. Despite phonological and orthographic differences, for each language, rise time sensitivity was a significant predictor of phonological awareness, and rise time was the only consistent predictor of reading acquisition. The data support a language-universal theory of the neural basis of developmental dyslexia on the basis of rhythmic perception and syllable segmentation. They also suggest that novel remediation strategies on the basis of rhythm and music may offer benefits for phonological and linguistic development.


Asunto(s)
Trastornos de la Percepción Auditiva/etiología , Comparación Transcultural , Dislexia/complicaciones , Fonética , Percepción del Habla/fisiología , Adolescente , Factores de Edad , Análisis de Varianza , Niño , Inglaterra , Humanos , Desarrollo del Lenguaje , Masculino , Nombres , Psicoacústica , Tiempo de Reacción , Lectura , Reconocimiento en Psicología/fisiología , Análisis de Regresión , España , Taiwán
5.
Neuroimage ; 57(3): 723-32, 2011 Aug 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21507346

RESUMEN

The core difficulty in developmental dyslexia across languages is a "phonological deficit", a specific difficulty with the neural representation of the sound structure of words. Recent data across languages suggest that this phonological deficit arises in part from inefficient auditory processing of the rate of change of the amplitude envelope at syllable onset (inefficient sensory processing of rise time). Rise time is a complex percept that also involves changes in duration and perceived intensity. Understanding the neural mechanisms that give rise to the phonological deficit in dyslexia is important for optimising educational interventions. In a three-deviant passive 'oddball' paradigm and a corresponding blocked 'deviant-alone' control condition we recorded ERPs to tones varying in rise time, duration and intensity in children with dyslexia and typically developing children longitudinally. We report here results from test Phases 1 and 2, when participants were aged 8-10 years. We found an MMN to duration, but not to rise time nor intensity deviants, at both time points for both groups. For rise time, duration and intensity we found group effects in both the Oddball and Blocked conditions. There was a slower fronto-central P1 response in the dyslexic group compared to controls. The amplitude of the P1 fronto-centrally to tones with slower rise times and lower intensity was smaller compared to tones with sharper rise times and higher intensity in the Oddball condition, for children with dyslexia only. The latency of this ERP component for all three stimuli was shorter on the right compared to the left hemisphere, only for the dyslexic group in the Blocked condition. Furthermore, we found decreased N1c amplitude to tones with slower rise times compared to tones with sharper rise times for children with dyslexia, only in the Oddball condition. Several other effects of stimulus type, age and laterality were also observed. Our data suggest that neuronal responses underlying some aspects of auditory sensory processing may be impaired in dyslexia.


Asunto(s)
Percepción Auditiva/fisiología , Encéfalo/fisiopatología , Dislexia/fisiopatología , Potenciales Evocados/fisiología , Niño , Electroencefalografía , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Procesamiento de Señales Asistido por Computador
6.
Dev Sci ; 14(1): 34-43, 2011 Jan.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21159086

RESUMEN

Across languages, children with developmental dyslexia have a specific difficulty with the neural representation of the sound structure (phonological structure) of speech. One likely cause of their difficulties with phonology is a perceptual difficulty in auditory temporal processing (Tallal, 1980). Tallal (1980) proposed that basic auditory processing of brief, rapidly successive acoustic changes is compromised in dyslexia, thereby affecting phonetic discrimination (e.g. discriminating /b/ from /d/) via impaired discrimination of formant transitions (rapid acoustic changes in frequency and intensity). However, an alternative auditory temporal hypothesis is that the basic auditory processing of the slower amplitude modulation cues in speech is compromised (Goswami et al., 2002). Here, we contrast children's perception of a synthetic speech contrast (ba/wa) when it is based on the speed of the rate of change of frequency information (formant transition duration) versus the speed of the rate of change of amplitude modulation (rise time). We show that children with dyslexia have excellent phonetic discrimination based on formant transition duration, but poor phonetic discrimination based on envelope cues. The results explain why phonetic discrimination may be allophonic in developmental dyslexia (Serniclaes et al., 2004), and suggest new avenues for the remediation of developmental dyslexia.


Asunto(s)
Dislexia , Fonética , Percepción del Habla , Trastornos de la Articulación , Niño , Señales (Psicología) , Humanos , Lenguaje , Lingüística , Psicoacústica
7.
Front Psychol ; 7: 791, 2016.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27303348

RESUMEN

Here we use two filtered speech tasks to investigate children's processing of slow (<4 Hz) versus faster (∼33 Hz) temporal modulations in speech. We compare groups of children with either developmental dyslexia (Experiment 1) or speech and language impairments (SLIs, Experiment 2) to groups of typically-developing (TD) children age-matched to each disorder group. Ten nursery rhymes were filtered so that their modulation frequencies were either low-pass filtered (<4 Hz) or band-pass filtered (22 - 40 Hz). Recognition of the filtered nursery rhymes was tested in a picture recognition multiple choice paradigm. Children with dyslexia aged 10 years showed equivalent recognition overall to TD controls for both the low-pass and band-pass filtered stimuli, but showed significantly impaired acoustic learning during the experiment from low-pass filtered targets. Children with oral SLIs aged 9 years showed significantly poorer recognition of band pass filtered targets compared to their TD controls, and showed comparable acoustic learning effects to TD children during the experiment. The SLI samples were also divided into children with and without phonological difficulties. The children with both SLI and phonological difficulties were impaired in recognizing both kinds of filtered speech. These data are suggestive of impaired temporal sampling of the speech signal at different modulation rates by children with different kinds of developmental language disorder. Both SLI and dyslexic samples showed impaired discrimination of amplitude rise times. Implications of these findings for a temporal sampling framework for understanding developmental language disorders are discussed.

8.
Cortex ; 49(5): 1363-76, 2013 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22726605

RESUMEN

INTRODUCTION: In a recent study, we reported that the accurate perception of beat structure in music ('perception of musical meter') accounted for over 40% of the variance in single word reading in children with and without dyslexia (Huss et al., 2011). Performance in the musical task was most strongly associated with the auditory processing of rise time, even though beat structure was varied by manipulating the duration of the musical notes. METHODS: Here we administered the same musical task a year later to 88 children with and without dyslexia, and used new auditory processing measures to provide a more comprehensive picture of the auditory correlates of the beat structure task. We also measured reading comprehension and nonword reading in addition to single word reading. RESULTS: One year later, the children with dyslexia performed more poorly in the musical task than younger children reading at the same level, indicating a severe perceptual deficit for musical beat patterns. They now also had significantly poorer perception of sound rise time than younger children. Longitudinal analyses showed that the musical beat structure task was a significant longitudinal predictor of development in reading, accounting for over half of the variance in reading comprehension along with a linguistic measure of phonological awareness. CONCLUSIONS: The non-linguistic musical beat structure task is an important independent longitudinal and concurrent predictor of variance in reading attainment by children. The different longitudinal versus concurrent associations between musical beat perception and auditory processing suggest that individual differences in the perception of rhythmic timing are an important shared neural basis for individual differences in children in linguistic and musical processing.


Asunto(s)
Concienciación/fisiología , Comprensión/fisiología , Dislexia/fisiopatología , Música , Fonética , Lectura , Estimulación Acústica/métodos , Adolescente , Análisis de Varianza , Niño , Dislexia/psicología , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Percepción del Habla/fisiología
9.
Cortex ; 47(6): 674-89, 2011 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20843509

RESUMEN

INTRODUCTION: Rhythm organises musical events into patterns and forms, and rhythm perception in music is usually studied by using metrical tasks. Metrical structure also plays an organisational function in the phonology of language, via speech prosody, and there is evidence for rhythmic perceptual difficulties in developmental dyslexia. Here we investigate the hypothesis that the accurate perception of musical metrical structure is related to basic auditory perception of rise time, and also to phonological and literacy development in children. METHODS: A battery of behavioural tasks was devised to explore relations between musical metrical perception, auditory perception of amplitude envelope structure, phonological awareness (PA) and reading in a sample of 64 typically-developing children and children with developmental dyslexia. RESULTS: We show that individual differences in the perception of amplitude envelope rise time are linked to musical metrical sensitivity, and that musical metrical sensitivity predicts PA and reading development, accounting for over 60% of variance in reading along with age and I.Q. Even the simplest metrical task, based on a duple metrical structure, was performed significantly more poorly by the children with dyslexia. CONCLUSIONS: The accurate perception of metrical structure may be critical for phonological development and consequently for the development of literacy. Difficulties in metrical processing are associated with basic auditory rise time processing difficulties, suggesting a primary sensory impairment in developmental dyslexia in tracking the lower-frequency modulations in the speech envelope.


Asunto(s)
Percepción Auditiva/fisiología , Dislexia/fisiopatología , Música , Lectura , Percepción del Tiempo/fisiología , Estimulación Acústica , Adolescente , Análisis de Varianza , Niño , Femenino , Humanos , Lenguaje , Masculino , Memoria/fisiología , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas
10.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 103(20): 7865-70, 2006 May 16.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16682637

RESUMEN

The processing of spoken language has been attributed to areas in the superior temporal lobe, where speech stimuli elicit the greatest activation. However, neurobiological and psycholinguistic models have long postulated that knowledge about the articulatory features of individual phonemes has an important role in their perception and in speech comprehension. To probe the possible involvement of specific motor circuits in the speech-perception process, we used event-related functional MRI and presented experimental subjects with spoken syllables, including [p] and [t] sounds, which are produced by movements of the lips or tongue, respectively. Physically similar nonlinguistic signal-correlated noise patterns were used as control stimuli. In localizer experiments, subjects had to silently articulate the same syllables and, in a second task, move their lips or tongue. Speech perception most strongly activated superior temporal cortex. Crucially, however, distinct motor regions in the precentral gyrus sparked by articulatory movements of the lips and tongue were also differentially activated in a somatotopic manner when subjects listened to the lip- or tongue-related phonemes. This sound-related somatotopic activation in precentral gyrus shows that, during speech perception, specific motor circuits are recruited that reflect phonetic distinctive features of the speech sounds encountered, thus providing direct neuroimaging support for specific links between the phonological mechanisms for speech perception and production.


Asunto(s)
Mapeo Encefálico , Lenguaje , Corteza Motora , Percepción del Habla/fisiología , Adulto , Encéfalo/anatomía & histología , Encéfalo/fisiología , Electromiografía , Femenino , Humanos , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Masculino , Corteza Motora/anatomía & histología , Corteza Motora/fisiología , Habla , Conducta Verbal/fisiología
11.
J Acoust Soc Am ; 117(6): 3841-52, 2005 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16018486

RESUMEN

The perception of pitch for pure tones with frequencies falling inside low- or high-frequency dead regions (DRs) was examined. Subjects adjusted a variable-frequency tone to match the pitch of a fixed tone. Matches within one ear were often erratic for tones falling in a DR, indicating unclear pitch percepts. Matches across ears of subjects with asymmetric hearing loss, and octave matches within ears, indicated that tones falling within a DR were perceived with an unclear pitch and/or a pitch different from "normal" whenever the tones fell more than 0.5 octave within a low- or high-frequency DR. One unilaterally impaired subject, with only a small surviving region between 3 and 4 kHz, matched a fixed 0.5-kHz tone in his impaired ear with, on average, a 3.75-kHz tone in his better ear. When asked to match the 0.5-kHz tone with an amplitude-modulated tone, he adjusted the carrier and modulation frequencies to about 3.8 and 0.5 kHz, respectively, suggesting that some temporal information was still available. Overall, the results indicate that the pitch of low-frequency tones is not conveyed solely by a temporal code. Possibly, there needs to be a correspondence between place and temporal information for a normal pitch to be perceived.


Asunto(s)
Muerte Celular/fisiología , Cóclea/fisiología , Percepción de la Altura Tonal/fisiología , Estimulación Acústica , Audiometría de Tonos Puros , Umbral Auditivo/fisiología , Membrana Basilar/fisiología , Nervio Coclear/fisiología , Sordera , Humanos , Discriminación de la Altura Tonal/fisiología , Espectrografía del Sonido , Acústica del Lenguaje
12.
Int J Audiol ; 44(10): 599-611, 2005 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16315451

RESUMEN

Some hearing-impaired subjects report pure tones as sounding highly distorted and noise-like. We assessed whether such reports indicate that the tone frequency falls inside a dead region (DR). Nine hearing-impaired and four normally hearing subjects rated pure tones on a scale from 1 to 7, where 1 indicates clear tone and 7 indicates noise. A white noise was presented as a reference for a sound that should be rated as 7. Stimuli covered the whole audible range of frequencies and levels. The noisiness ratings were, on average, higher for hearing-impaired subjects than for normally hearing subjects. For the former, the ratings were not markedly different for tones with frequencies just outside or inside a DR. However, ratings always exceeded 3 for tones falling more than 1.5 octaves inside a DR. The results indicate that judgement of a tone as sounding noise-like does not reliably indicate that the tone frequency falls in a DR. Both normally hearing and hearing-impaired subjects rated 0.125 kHz and 12 kHz tones as somewhat noise-like, independently of the existence of a DR.


Asunto(s)
Audiometría de Tonos Puros , Cóclea/fisiopatología , Pérdida Auditiva Sensorineural/fisiopatología , Ruido , Adulto , Anciano , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Umbral Auditivo/fisiología , Estudios de Casos y Controles , Células Ciliadas Auditivas Internas/fisiopatología , Humanos , Persona de Mediana Edad , Psicoacústica
13.
J Acoust Soc Am ; 114(6 Pt 1): 3283-94, 2003 Dec.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-14714809

RESUMEN

For people with normal hearing, a sustained tone with a frequency within the standard audiometric range remains audible when presented at a level well above threshold. However, for a pure tone with frequency close to the upper limit of hearing (well above 8 kHz), the loudness may decrease within seconds and the tone may decay to inaudibility, even when presented at a level between 20 and 40 dB SL. Scharf [in Hearing Research and Theory, edited by J. V. Tobias and E. D. Schubert (Academic, New York, 1983), Vol. 2, pp. 1-53] suggested that marked loudness adaptation only occurs when the excitation pattern evoked by a tone is spatially limited. The upper limit of hearing may be comparable to the boundary of a "dead region," which is a region with a complete loss of inner hair cell (IHC) and/or neural function. The present study investigated the perceived decay of pure tones for 9 normal-hearing subjects and 12 subjects with moderate to severe sensorineural hearing loss, using a wide range of frequencies (0.125-12 kHz). A dead region was diagnosed for 8 of the 12 subjects. No consistent association was found between the degree of tone decay and the presence of a dead region. Subjects with dead regions did not experience significantly more tone decay than subjects with comparable absolute thresholds but without a dead region, even when the frequency of the tone fell within or close to the edge of a dead region. For severely hearing-impaired subjects, spatial restriction of the excitation pattern was neither necessary nor sufficient to lead to tone decay. The prevalence of tone decay was not well predicted by the audiometric threshold at the test frequency. It is proposed that tone decay depends on the physiological condition of the place in the cochlea where the tone is detected, which, in a case involving a dead region, is the place adjacent to the dead region. The prevalence of tone decay increased when the audiometric threshold was above 50 dB HL in the frequency region where the tone was detected.


Asunto(s)
Umbral Auditivo/fisiología , Células Ciliadas Auditivas Internas/fisiopatología , Pérdida Auditiva Sensorineural/diagnóstico , Percepción Sonora/fisiología , Discriminación de la Altura Tonal , Adulto , Anciano , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Audiometría de Tonos Puros , Femenino , Pérdida Auditiva Sensorineural/etiología , Pérdida Auditiva Sensorineural/fisiopatología , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Valores de Referencia
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
Detalles de la búsqueda