Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 61
Filtrar
1.
PLoS Biol ; 14(10): e1002569, 2016 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27780207

RESUMEN

Retinal prosthesis technologies require that the visual system downstream of the retinal circuitry be capable of transmitting and elaborating visual signals. We studied the capability of plastic remodeling in late blind subjects implanted with the Argus II Retinal Prosthesis with psychophysics and functional MRI (fMRI). After surgery, six out of seven retinitis pigmentosa (RP) blind subjects were able to detect high-contrast stimuli using the prosthetic implant. However, direction discrimination to contrast modulated stimuli remained at chance level in all of them. No subject showed any improvement of contrast sensitivity in either eye when not using the Argus II. Before the implant, the Blood Oxygenation Level Dependent (BOLD) activity in V1 and the lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN) was very weak or absent. Surprisingly, after prolonged use of Argus II, BOLD responses to visual input were enhanced. This is, to our knowledge, the first study tracking the neural changes of visual areas in patients after retinal implant, revealing a capacity to respond to restored visual input even after years of deprivation.


Asunto(s)
Ceguera/fisiopatología , Agudeza Visual , Prótesis Visuales , Ceguera/diagnóstico por imagen , Ceguera/etiología , Cuerpos Geniculados/fisiopatología , Humanos , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Retinitis Pigmentosa/complicaciones
2.
Neuroimage ; 143: 364-377, 2016 Dec.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27622396

RESUMEN

Psychophysical studies have shown that numerosity is a sensory attribute susceptible to adaptation. Neuroimaging studies have reported that, at least for relatively low numbers, numerosity can be accurately discriminated in the intra-parietal sulcus. Here we developed a novel rapid adaptation paradigm where adapting and test stimuli are separated by pauses sufficient to dissociate their BOLD activity. We used multivariate pattern recognition to classify brain activity evoked by non-symbolic numbers over a wide range (20-80), both before and after psychophysical adaptation to the highest numerosity. Adaptation caused underestimation of all lower numerosities, and decreased slightly the average BOLD responses in V1 and IPS. Using support vector machine, we showed that the BOLD response of IPS, but not in V1, classified numerosity well, both when tested before and after adaptation. However, there was no transfer from training pre-adaptation responses to testing post-adaptation, and vice versa, indicating that adaptation changes the neuronal representation of the numerosity. Interestingly, decoding was more accurate after adaptation, and the amount of improvement correlated with the amount of perceptual underestimation of numerosity across subjects. These results suggest that numerosity adaptation acts directly on IPS, rather than indirectly via other low-level stimulus parameters analysis, and that adaptation improves the capacity to discriminate numerosity.


Asunto(s)
Adaptación Fisiológica/fisiología , Mapeo Encefálico/métodos , Conceptos Matemáticos , Lóbulo Parietal/fisiología , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología , Máquina de Vectores de Soporte , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Masculino , Lóbulo Parietal/diagnóstico por imagen , Reconocimiento de Normas Patrones Automatizadas/métodos , Psicofísica/métodos , Adulto Joven
3.
Behav Res Methods ; 48(2): 614-20, 2016 06.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26092392

RESUMEN

We have constructed and tested a custom-made magnetic-imaging-compatible visual projection system designed to project on a very wide visual field (~80°). A standard projector was modified with a coupling lens, projecting images into the termination of an image fiber. The other termination of the fiber was placed in the 3-T scanner room with a projection lens, which projected the images relayed by the fiber onto a screen over the head coil, viewed by a participant wearing magnifying goggles. To validate the system, wide-field stimuli were presented in order to identify retinotopic visual areas. The results showed that this low-cost and versatile optical system may be a valuable tool to map visual areas in the brain that process peripheral receptive fields.


Asunto(s)
Imagen por Resonancia Magnética/instrumentación , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética/métodos , Estimulación Luminosa/instrumentación , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Adulto , Mapeo Encefálico , Femenino , Humanos , Procesamiento de Imagen Asistido por Computador , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética/economía , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Reproducibilidad de los Resultados , Campos Visuales
4.
Eur J Neurosci ; 38(2): 2290-9, 2013 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23600977

RESUMEN

Animal physiological and human psychophysical studies suggest that an early step in visual processing involves the detection and identification of features such as lines and edges, by neural mechanisms with even- and odd-symmetric receptive fields. Functional imaging studies also demonstrate mechanisms with even- and odd-receptive fields in early visual areas, in response to luminance-modulated stimuli. In this study we measured fMRI BOLD responses to 2-D stimuli composed of only even or only odd symmetric features, and to an amplitude-matched random noise control, modulated in red-green equiluminant colour contrast. All these stimuli had identical power but different phase spectra, either highly congruent (even or odd symmetry stimuli) or random (noise). At equiluminance, V1 BOLD activity showed no preference between congruent- and random-phase stimuli, as well as no preference between even and odd symmetric stimuli. Areas higher in the visual hierarchy, both along the dorsal pathway (caudal part of the intraparietal sulcus, dorsal LO and V3A) and the ventral pathway (V4), responded preferentially to odd symmetry over even symmetry stimuli, and to congruent over random phase stimuli. Interestingly, V1 showed an equal increase in BOLD activity at each alternation between stimuli of different symmetry, suggesting the existence of specialised mechanisms for the detection of edges and lines such as even- and odd-chromatic receptive fields. Overall the results indicate a high selectivity of colour-selective neurons to spatial phase along both the dorsal and the ventral pathways in humans.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Color/fisiología , Vías Visuales/fisiología , Femenino , Humanos , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Masculino
5.
Neuropsychologia ; 128: 166-177, 2019 05.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29100949

RESUMEN

In adults, partial damage to V1 or optic radiations abolishes perception in the corresponding part of the visual field, causing a scotoma. However, it is widely accepted that the developing cortex has superior capacities to reorganize following an early lesion to endorse adaptive plasticity. Here we report a single patient case (G.S.) with near normal central field vision despite a massive unilateral lesion to the optic radiations acquired early in life. The patient underwent surgical removal of a right hemisphere parieto-temporal-occipital atypical choroid plexus papilloma of the right lateral ventricle at four months of age, which presumably altered the visual pathways during in utero development. Both the tumor and surgery severely compromised the optic radiations. Residual vision of G.S. was tested psychophysically when the patient was 7 years old. We found a close-to-normal visual acuity and contrast sensitivity within the central 25° and a great impairment in form and contrast vision in the far periphery (40-50°) of the left visual hemifield. BOLD response to full field luminance flicker was recorded from the primary visual cortex (V1) and in a region in the residual temporal-occipital region, presumably corresponding to the middle temporal complex (MT+), of the lesioned (right) hemisphere. A population receptive field analysis of the BOLD responses to contrast modulated stimuli revealed a retinotopic organization just for the MT+ region but not for the calcarine regions. Interestingly, consistent islands of ipsilateral activity were found in MT+ and in the parieto-occipital sulcus (POS) of the intact hemisphere. Probabilistic tractography revealed that optic radiations between LGN and V1 were very sparse in the lesioned hemisphere consistently with the post-surgery cerebral resection, while normal in the intact hemisphere. On the other hand, strong structural connections between MT+ and LGN were found in the lesioned hemisphere, while the equivalent tract in the spared hemisphere showed minimal structural connectivity. These results suggest that during development of the pathological brain, abnormal thalamic projections can lead to functional cortical changes, which may mediate functional recovery of vision.


Asunto(s)
Plasticidad Neuronal , Corteza Visual/lesiones , Adolescente , Mapeo Encefálico , Neoplasias del Plexo Coroideo/cirugía , Sensibilidad de Contraste , Imagen de Difusión Tensora , Femenino , Humanos , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Papiloma del Plexo Coroideo/cirugía , Complicaciones Posoperatorias/diagnóstico por imagen , Complicaciones Posoperatorias/psicología , Lóbulo Temporal/diagnóstico por imagen , Lóbulo Temporal/lesiones , Corteza Visual/diagnóstico por imagen , Pruebas del Campo Visual , Vías Visuales/diagnóstico por imagen , Vías Visuales/lesiones
6.
Nat Neurosci ; 3(12): 1322-8, 2000 Dec.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11100154

RESUMEN

The continuously changing optic flow on the retina provides information about direction of heading and about the three-dimensional structure of the environment. Here we use functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to demonstrate that an area in human cortex responds selectively to components of optic flow, such as circular and radial motion. This area is within the region commonly referrred to as V5/MT complex, but is distinct from the part of this region that responds to translation. The functional properties of these two areas of the V5/MT complex are also different; the response to optic flow was obtained only with changing flow stimuli, whereas response to translation occurred during exposure to continuous motion.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Movimiento/fisiología , Lóbulo Temporal/fisiología , Corteza Visual/fisiología , Adulto , Mapeo Encefálico , Femenino , Humanos , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Masculino , Estimulación Luminosa , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Rotación/efectos adversos , Lóbulo Temporal/anatomía & histología , Corteza Visual/anatomía & histología
7.
Sci Rep ; 8(1): 320, 2018 01 10.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29321562

RESUMEN

How the visual system achieves perceptual stability across saccadic eye movements is a long-standing question in neuroscience. It has been proposed that an efference copy informs vision about upcoming saccades, and this might lead to shifting spatial coordinates and suppressing image motion. Here we ask whether these two aspects of visual stability are interdependent or may be dissociated under special conditions. We study a memory-guided double-step saccade task, where two saccades are executed in quick succession. Previous studies have led to the hypothesis that in this paradigm the two saccades are planned in parallel, with a single efference copy signal generated at the start of the double-step sequence, i.e. before the first saccade. In line with this hypothesis, we find that visual stability is impaired during the second saccade, which is consistent with (accurate) efference copy information being unavailable during the second saccade. However, we find that saccadic suppression is normal during the second saccade. Thus, the second saccade of a double-step sequence instantiates a dissociation between visual stability and saccadic suppression: stability is impaired even though suppression is strong.


Asunto(s)
Movimientos Sacádicos/fisiología , Percepción Visual , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Memoria
8.
Curr Biol ; 9(20): 1207-9, 1999 Oct 21.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10531034

RESUMEN

There is now good evidence that perception of motion is strongly suppressed during saccades (rapid shifts of gaze), presumably to blunt the disturbing sense of motion that saccades would otherwise elicit. Other aspects of vision, such as contrast detection of high-frequency or equiluminant gratings, are virtually unaffected by saccades [1] [2] [3] [4] [5]. This has led to the suggestion that saccades may suppress selectively the magnocellular pathway (which is strongly implicated in motion perception), leaving the parvocellular pathway unaffected [5] [6]. Here, we investigate the neural level at which perception of motion is suppressed. We used a simple technique in which an impression of motion is generated from only two frames, allowing precise control over the stimulus [7] [8]. One frame has a certain fixed contrast, whereas the contrast of the other (the test frame) is varied to determine the threshold for motion discrimination (that is, the lowest test-frame contrast level at which the direction of motion can be correctly guessed). Contrast thresholds of the test depended strongly and non-monotonically on the contrast of the fixed-contrast frame, with a minimum at medium contrast. To study the effect of saccadic suppression, we triggered the two-frame sequence by a voluntary saccade. Thresholds during saccades increased in a way that suggested that saccadic suppression precedes motion analysis: when the test frame was first in the motion sequence there was a general depression of sensitivity, whereas when it was second, the contrast response curve was shifted to a higher contrast range, sometimes even resulting in higher sensitivity than without a saccade. The dependence on presentation order suggests that saccadic suppression occurs at an early stage of visual processing, on the single frames themselves rather than on the combined motion signal. As motion detection itself is thought to occur at an early stage, saccadic suppression must take place at a very early phenomenon.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Movimiento/fisiología , Movimientos Sacádicos/fisiología , Humanos , Estimulación Luminosa , Vías Visuales/fisiología
9.
Curr Biol ; 11(10): 798-802, 2001 May 15.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11378393

RESUMEN

Some 30 years ago, Trevarthen [1] introduced the idea of two separate visual systems, a focal system for fine motor acts and an ambient system for gross body movements such as ambulation. More recent developments indicating anatomically and physiologically separate pathways in primate vision [2] have led to a different idea of separate visual systems, one for conscious perception and one for action [3]. It has received empirical support from several studies showing that pointing, reaching, and grasping can remain accurate while the perceived position or size of objects is subject to illusory distortion [4-6]. However, much of this evidence has been challenged on the grounds of methodological flaws, particularly failure to match perfectly the conditions for verbal and motor tasks and failure to replicate results [7-10]. Here we take advantage of the strong compression of perceived position that occurs around the time of saccadic eye movements [11, 12]. Under normal lighting conditions, stimuli flashed briefly over a wide range of spatial positions just before saccadic onset are neither seen nor reached for in their veridical positions, but are compressed toward the saccadic target. We validate the idea of separate systems by showing that, in the dark, subjects are able to point accurately to the correct target position, even though their verbal reports are still subject to compression.


Asunto(s)
Movimientos Sacádicos , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Humanos
10.
Curr Biol ; 9(14): 763-6, 1999 Jul 15.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10421583

RESUMEN

As we move through our environment, the flow of deforming images on the retinae provides a rich source of information about the three-dimensional structure of the external world and how to navigate through it. Recent evidence from psychophysical [1] [2] [3] [4], electrophysiological [5] [6] [7] [8] [9] and imaging [10] [11] studies suggests that there are neurons in the primate visual system - in the medial superior temporal cortex - that are specialised to respond to this type of complex 'optic flow' motion. In principle, optic flow could be encoded by a small number of neural mechanisms tuned to 'cardinal directions', including radial and circular motion [12] [13]. There is little support for this idea at present, however, from either physiological [6] [7] or psychophysical [14] research. We have measured the sensitivity of human subjects for detection of motion and for discrimination of motion direction over a wide and densely sampled range of complex motions. Average sensitivity was higher for inward and outward radial movement and for both directions of rotation, consistent with the existence of detectors tuned to these four types of motion. Principle component analysis revealed two clear components, one for radial stimuli (outward and inward) and the other for circular stimuli (clockwise and counter-clock-wise). The results imply that the mechanisms that analyse optic flow in humans tend to be tuned to the cardinal axes of radial and rotational motion.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Movimiento/fisiología , Adulto , Humanos , Modelos Estadísticos , Sensibilidad y Especificidad , Umbral Sensorial/fisiología , Percepción Visual/fisiología
11.
Vision Res ; 47(10): 1282-91, 2007 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17395237

RESUMEN

This study investigates the role played by individual spatial scales in determining the apparent brightness of greyscale patterns. We measured the perceived difference in brightness across an edge in the presence of notch filtering and high-pass filtering for two stimulus configurations, one that elicits the perception of transparency and one that appears opaque. For both stimulus configurations, the apparent brightness of the surfaces delimited by the border decreased monotonically with progressive (ideal) high-pass filtering, with a critical cut-off at 1 c/deg. Using two octave ideal notch filtering, the maximum detrimental effect on apparent brightness was observed at about 1c/deg. Critical frequencies for apparent brightness did not vary with contrast, viewing distance, or surface size, suggesting that apparent brightness is determined by the channel tuned at 1 c/deg. Modelling the data with the local energy model [Morrone, M. C., & Burr, D. C. (1988). Feature detection in human vision: a phase dependent energy model. Proceedings of the Royal Society (London), B235, 221-245] at 1c/deg confirmed the suggestion that this channel mediates apparent brightness for both opaque and transparent borders, with no need for pooling or integration across spatial channels.


Asunto(s)
Luz , Percepción Visual , Sensibilidad de Contraste , Discriminación en Psicología , Humanos , Ilusiones , Modelos Psicológicos , Enmascaramiento Perceptual , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Psicofísica
12.
Trends Neurosci ; 24(2): 113-21, 2001 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11164942

RESUMEN

We frequently reposition our gaze by making rapid ballistic eye movements that are called saccades. Saccades pose problems for the visual system, because they generate rapid, large-field motion on the retina and change the relationship between the object position in external space and the image position on the retina. The brain must ignore the one and compensate for the other. Much progress has been made in recent years in understanding the effects of saccades on visual function and elucidating the mechanisms responsible for them. Evidence suggests that saccades trigger two distinct neural processes: (1) a suppression of visual sensitivity, specific to the magnocellular pathway, that dampens the sensation of motion and (2) a gross perceptual distortion of visual space in anticipation of the repositioning of gaze. Neurophysiological findings from several laboratories are beginning to identify the neural substrates involved in these effects.


Asunto(s)
Sensibilidad de Contraste/fisiología , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Movimientos Sacádicos/fisiología , Campos Visuales/fisiología , Animales , Humanos , Percepción Visual/fisiología
13.
J Neurosci ; 20(9): 3449-55, 2000 May 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10777808

RESUMEN

We measured the time course of saccadic suppression and tested whether suppression results entirely from retinal image motion or has an extraretinal source. We measured contrast thresholds for low-frequency gratings modulated either in luminance, at 17 cd/m(2) and 0.17 cd/m(2), or color at 17 cd/m(2). Gratings were flashed on a uniform background before, during, or after voluntary 12 degrees saccades and, additionally in the case of luminance modulated gratings, saccades simulated by mirror motion. A 10-fold decrease in contrast sensitivity was found for luminance-modulated gratings with saccades, but little suppression was found with simulated saccades. Adding high-contrast noise to the display increased the magnitude and the duration of the suppression during simulated saccades but had little effect on suppression produced by real saccades. Suppression anticipates saccades by 50 msec, is maximal at the moment of saccadic onset, and outlasts saccades by approximately 50 msec. At lower luminance, suppression is reduced, and its course is shallower than at higher luminance. Simulated saccades produce shallower suppression over a longer time course at both luminances. No suppression was found for chromatically modulated gratings. Differences between real and simulated saccades in the magnitude and time course of sensitivity loss suggest that saccadic suppression has an extraretinal component. We model the effects of saccades by adding a signal to the visual input, so as to saturate the nonlinear stage of visual processing and make detection of a test stimulus more difficult.


Asunto(s)
Sensibilidad de Contraste/fisiología , Iluminación , Movimientos Sacádicos/fisiología , Adulto , Anciano , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Modelos Biológicos
14.
Proc Biol Sci ; 242(1304): 134-9, 1990 Nov 22.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-1981617

RESUMEN

We have monitored the development of infant colour vision by measuring chromatic contrast sensitivity and acuity in eight young infants over a period of 6 months. Steady-state visual evoked potentials (VEPS) were recorded in response to both chromatic (red-green) and luminance (red-black or green-black) patterns that were reversed in contrast over time. For most infants, no response could be obtained to chromatic stimuli of any size or contrast before 5 weeks of age, although luminance stimuli of 20% contrast gave reliable responses at that age. When responses to chromatic stimuli first appeared, they could be obtained only with stimuli of very low spatial frequency, 20 times lower than the acuity for luminance stimuli. Both contrast sensitivity and acuity for chromatic stimuli increased steadily, more rapidly than for luminance stimuli. As the spectral selectivities of infant cones are similar to those of adults, the difference in rate of development of luminance and chromatic contrast sensitivity and acuity stimuli probably reflects neural development of the infant colour system.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Color/fisiología , Sensibilidad de Contraste/fisiología , Agudeza Visual/fisiología , Factores de Edad , Potenciales Evocados Visuales , Humanos , Lactante
15.
Neuroreport ; 10(13): 2723-9, 1999 Sep 09.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10511430

RESUMEN

Animal models suggest that the asymmetry of monocular optokinetic nystagmus (OKN) in young infants can be explained by a direct pathway from retina to the midbrain nucleus of the optic tract. However, earlier studies with hemispherectomized infants showed no evidence for OKN responses towards the damaged cortex that could be ascribed to this subcortical pathway. In longitudinal testing of two infants with very extensive unilateral cortical damage, we have now shown that OKN responses in both directions do occur before 10 months of age. OKN towards the damaged cortex, indicating functioning of the direct pathways in the absence of cortical control, drops out in the later development. The neural circuitry responsible for OKN in humans appears to undergo a plastic reorganization.


Asunto(s)
Corteza Cerebral/fisiología , Desarrollo Infantil , Dominancia Cerebral/fisiología , Nistagmo Optoquinético/fisiología , Encéfalo/anomalías , Encéfalo/patología , Encéfalo/cirugía , Encefalopatías/patología , Encefalopatías/cirugía , Corteza Cerebral/patología , Femenino , Humanos , Lactante , Estudios Longitudinales , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Masculino , Plasticidad Neuronal , Estimulación Luminosa
16.
Brain Res ; 331(2): 382-5, 1985 Apr 08.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-3986577

RESUMEN

The spatial acuity was measured for cells of the posteromedial lateral suprasylvian area (PMLS) of the cat. Acuities were found to be 2 cycles/degree (15 mins arc) at best, and 1 cycle/degree (30 mins arc) on average. Both best acuity and average acuity remained constant with receptive field eccentricity within 20 degrees of the area centralis, and then fell gradually with eccentricity. Acuity was good, given receptive field size, and was not correlated with receptive field size. Comparisons are drawn with other visual structures.


Asunto(s)
Corteza Cerebral/fisiología , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Animales , Mapeo Encefálico , Gatos , Agudeza Visual , Campos Visuales
17.
Vision Res ; 36(14): 2069-78, 1996 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8776473

RESUMEN

Previous work has shown that during saccadic eye movements, contrast sensitivity for low spatial frequency patterns modulated in luminance is selectively reduced by up to one logarithmic unit, while high spatial frequency patterns, and equiluminant patterns of all spatial frequencies are not suppressed at all [Burr et al. (1994). Nature, 371, 511-513]. Here we study the temporal characteristics for sensitivity to luminance and chromatic patterns during saccades, using the two-pulse summation technique. Sensitivity was measured for detecting two successive pulses as a function of stimulus-onset asynchrony, during normal viewing and during saccades. Impulse response functions were estimated from the summation data, for all conditions. For equiluminance, the functions were monophasic during normal viewing and saccades. For luminance modulation, the impulse response functions were di-phasic in both normal viewing and saccades. However, during saccades the impulse responses were faster in normal viewing. This result is consistent with the suggestion that saccadic suppression is mediated by contrast gain control mechanisms, known to occur in M-cells but not P-cells.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Color/fisiología , Sensibilidad de Contraste/fisiología , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología , Movimientos Sacádicos/fisiología , Discriminación en Psicología/fisiología , Humanos , Iluminación , Masculino , Matemática , Vías Nerviosas/fisiología , Umbral Sensorial/fisiología , Factores de Tiempo
18.
Vision Res ; 37(18): 2609-29, 1997 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9373692

RESUMEN

This study examines the effect of coarse quantization (blocking) on image recognition, and explores possible mechanisms. Thresholds for noise corruption showed that coarse quantization reduces drastically the recognizability of both faces and letters, well beyond the levels expected by equivalent blurring. Phase-shifting the spurious high frequencies introduced by the blocking (with an operation designed to leave both overall and local contrast unaffected, and feature localization) greatly improved recognizability of both faces and letters. For large phase shifts, the low spatial frequencies appear in transparency behind a grid structure of checks or lines. We also studied a more simple example of blocking, the checkerboard, that can be considered as a coarse quantized diagonal sinusoidal plaid. When one component of the plaid was contrast-inverted, it was seen in transparency against the checkerboard, while the other remained "captured" within the block structure. If the higher harmonics are then phase-shifted by pi, the contrast-reversed fundamental becomes captured and the other seen in transparency. Intermediate phase shifts of the higher harmonics cause intermediate effects, which we measured by adjusting the relative contrast of the fundamentals until neither orientation dominated. The contrast match varied considerably with the phase of the higher harmonics, over a range of about 1.5 log units. Simulations with the local energy model predicted qualitatively the results of the recognizability of both faces and letters, and quantitatively the apparent orientation of the modified checkerboard pattern. More generally, the model predicts the conditions under which an image will be "captured" by coarse quantization, or seen in transparency.


Asunto(s)
Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología , Sensibilidad de Contraste/fisiología , Señales (Psicología) , Humanos , Matemática , Modelos Biológicos , Psicofísica , Rotación , Umbral Sensorial/fisiología
19.
Vision Res ; 33(17): 2535-52, 1993 Dec.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8249333

RESUMEN

We have monitored the development of contrast sensitivity to equiluminant red-green chromatic patterns by monitoring visual evoked potentials (VEPs) in 13 infants. The results confirm our previous report [Morrone, Burr and Fiorentini, Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 242 (1990a)] that, before 7-8 weeks of age, there was no response to purely chromatic stimuli, while at the same age luminance stimuli of 20% contrast produced reliable responses. At all ages (even before the onset of a chromatic response) the colour mixture to yield equiluminance was similar to that of adults, suggesting that the relative proportion and efficacy of medium- and long-wave cones is similar for infants as for adults. For both luminance and chromatic stimuli, amplitude increased roughly linearly with log-contrast, so sensitivity thresholds could be predicted by linear extrapolation to the abscissa. Detailed contrast sensitivity curves were measured for four infants at various ages. The results show that luminance and chromatic contrast sensitivity develop independently at different rates, probably reflecting differential development of postreceptoral neural mechanisms.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Color/fisiología , Sensibilidad de Contraste/fisiología , Factores de Edad , Desarrollo Infantil , Potenciales Evocados Visuales/fisiología , Femenino , Humanos , Lactante , Recién Nacido , Masculino , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología , Umbral Sensorial/fisiología , Espectrofotometría , Factores de Tiempo
20.
Vision Res ; 44(12): 1389-401, 2004 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15066398

RESUMEN

We used an interference paradigm to investigate whether attention is attribute-specific at early levels of visual processing. We show that the peripheral increment thresholds for luminance contrast deteriorate when the observer is currently performing another luminance (form or contrast) discrimination task in central view, but not when he or she is performing a color discrimination task. Similar results were obtained for color increment thresholds, indicating that the interference is specific to contrast modality. The effects are strong and robust over primary task difficulties and perceptual learning levels. Modeling suggests that attention improves contrast sensitivity by modulating the gain of the neuronal response to contrast. These results suggest that attention is parceled in independent resources for luminance and color contrast.


Asunto(s)
Atención/fisiología , Percepción de Color/fisiología , Sensibilidad de Contraste/fisiología , Discriminación en Psicología , Percepción de Forma/fisiología , Humanos , Aprendizaje/fisiología , Luz , Modelos Psicológicos , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Umbral Sensorial
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
Detalles de la búsqueda