Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 38
Filtrar
1.
Brain Cogn ; 145: 105628, 2020 11.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33007685

RESUMEN

Our study was designed to test a recent proposal by Cayol and Nazir (2020), according to which language processing takes advantage of motor system "emulators". An emulator is a brain mechanism that learns the causal relationship between an action and its sensory consequences. Emulators predict the outcome of a motor command in terms of its sensory reafference and serve monitoring ongoing movements. For the purpose of motor planning/learning, emulators can "run offline", decoupled from sensory input and motor output. Such offline simulations are equivalent to mental imagery (Grush, 2004). If language processing can profit from the associative-memory network of emulators, mental-imagery-aptitude should predict language skills. However, this should hold only for language content that is imageable. We tested this assumption in typically developing adolescents using two motor-imagery paradigms. One that measured participant's error in estimating their motor ability, and another that measured the time to perform a mental simulation. When the time to perform a mental simulation is taken as measure, mental-imagery-aptitude does indeed selectively predict word-definition performance for high imageable words. These results provide an alternative position relative to the question of why language processes recruit modality-specific brain regions and support the often-hypothesized link between language and motor skills.


Asunto(s)
Aptitud , Lenguaje , Memoria , Adolescente , Encéfalo , Humanos , Imaginación , Destreza Motora
2.
Behav Res Methods ; 49(1): 61-73, 2017 02.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26705116

RESUMEN

Research in cognitive neuroscience has shown that brain structures serving perceptual, emotional, and motor processes are also recruited during the understanding of language when it refers to emotion, perception, and action. However, the exact linguistic and extralinguistic conditions under which such language-induced activity in modality-specific cortex is triggered are not yet well understood. The purpose of this study is to introduce a simple experimental technique that allows for the online measure of language-induced activity in motor structures of the brain. This technique consists in the use of a grip force sensor that captures subtle grip force variations while participants listen to words and sentences. Since grip force reflects activity in motor brain structures, the continuous monitoring of force fluctuations provides a fine-grained estimation of motor activity across time. In other terms, this method allows for both localization of the source of language-induced activity to motor brain structures and high temporal resolution of the recorded data. To facilitate comparison of the data to be collected with this tool, we present two experiments that describe in detail the technical setup, the nature of the recorded data, and the analyses (including justification about the data filtering and artifact rejection) that we applied. We also discuss how the tool could be used in other domains of behavioral research.


Asunto(s)
Recolección de Datos/instrumentación , Fuerza de la Mano/fisiología , Lenguaje , Adulto , Percepción Auditiva , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Adulto Joven
3.
J Cogn Neurosci ; 26(11): 2552-63, 2014 Nov.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24893746

RESUMEN

Growing evidence suggests that semantic knowledge is represented in distributed neural networks that include modality-specific structures. Here, we examined the processes underlying the acquisition of words from different semantic categories to determine whether the emergence of visual- and action-based categories could be tracked back to their acquisition. For this, we applied correspondence analysis (CA) to ERPs recorded at various moments during acquisition. CA is a multivariate statistical technique typically used to reveal distance relationships between words of a corpus. Applied to ERPs, it allows isolating factors that best explain variations in the data across time and electrodes. Participants were asked to learn new action and visual words by associating novel pseudowords with the execution of hand movements or the observation of visual images. Words were probed before and after training on two consecutive days. To capture processes that unfold during lexical access, CA was applied on the 100-400 msec post-word onset interval. CA isolated two factors that organized the data as a function of test sessions and word categories. Conventional ERP analyses further revealed a category-specific increase in the negativity of the ERPs to action and visual words at the frontal and occipital electrodes, respectively. The distinct neural processes underlying action and visual words can thus be tracked back to the acquisition of word-referent relationships and may have its origin in association learning. Given current evidence for the flexibility of language-induced sensory-motor activity, we argue that these associative links may serve functions beyond word understanding, that is, the elaboration of situation models.


Asunto(s)
Aprendizaje por Asociación/fisiología , Encéfalo/fisiología , Percepción de Movimiento/fisiología , Semántica , Percepción del Habla/fisiología , Estimulación Acústica , Electroencefalografía , Potenciales Evocados , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Estimulación Luminosa , Psicolingüística , Adulto Joven
4.
Exp Brain Res ; 227(3): 407-19, 2013 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23615976

RESUMEN

Action observation and execution share overlapping neural resonating mechanisms. In the present study, we sought to examine the effect of the activation of this system during concurrent movement observation and execution in a prehension task, when no a priori information about the requirements of grasping action was available. Although it is known that simultaneous activation by observation and execution influences motor performance, the importance of the delays of these two events and the specific effect of movement observation itself (and not the prediction of the to-be-observed movement) on action performance are poorly known. Fine-grained kinematic analysis of both the transport and grasp components of the movement should provide knowledge about the influence of movement observation on the precision and the performance of the executed movement. The experiment involved two real participants who were asked to grasp a different side of a single object that was composed of a large and a small part. In the first experiment, we measured how the transport component and the grasp component were affected by movement observation. We tested whether this influence was greater if the observed movement occurred just before the onset of movement (200 ms) or well before the onset of movement (1 s). In a second experiment, to reproduce the previous experiment and to verify the specificity of the grasping movements, we also included a condition consisting of pointing towards the object. Both experiments showed two main results. A general facilitation of the transport component was found when observing a simultaneous action, independent of its congruency. Moreover, a specific facilitation of the grasp component was present during the observation of a congruent action when movement execution and observation were nearly synchronised. While the general facilitation may arise from a competition between the two participants as they reached for the object, the specific facilitation of the grasp component seems to be directly related to mirror neuron system activity induced by action observation itself. Moreover, the time course of the events appears to be an essential factor for this modulation, implying the transitory activation of the mirror neuron system.


Asunto(s)
Brazo/fisiología , Fuerza de la Mano/fisiología , Movimiento/fisiología , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Adulto , Fenómenos Biomecánicos/fisiología , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Estimulación Luminosa , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología
5.
PLoS One ; 18(11): e0287507, 2023.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37976324

RESUMEN

The social robots market will grow considerably in the coming years. What the arrival of these new kind of social agents means for society, however, is largely unknown. Existing cases of robot abuse point to risks of introducing such artificial social agents (ASAs) without considerations about consequences (risks for the robots and the human witnesses to the abuse). We believe that humans react aggressively towards ASAs when they are enticed into establishing dominance hierarchies. This happens when there is a basis for skill comparison. We therefore presented pairs of robots on which we varied similarity and the degree of stimulatability of their mechanisms/functions with the human body (walking, jumping = simulatable; rolling, floating = non-simulatable). We asked which robot (i) resembled more a human, (ii) possessed more "essentialized human qualities" (e.g. creativity). To estimate social acceptability, participants had also (iii) to predict the outcome of a situation where a robot approached a group of humans. For robots with simulatable functions, rating of essentialized human qualities decreased as human resemblance decreased (jumper < walker). For robots with non-simulable functions, the reversed relation was seen: robots that least resembled humans (floater) scored highest in qualities. Critically, robot's acceptability followed ratings of essentialized human qualities. Humans respond socially to certain morphological (physical aspects) and behavioral cues. Therefore, unless ASAs perfectly mimic humans, it is safer to provide them with mechanisms/functions that cannot be simulated with the human body.


Asunto(s)
Robótica , Humanos , Interacción Social , Predominio Social , Caminata , Señales (Psicología)
6.
J Exp Psychol Gen ; 151(9): 2173-2194, 2022 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35157482

RESUMEN

It is well established that the processing of hand-, mouth-, and foot-related action terms can activate areas of the motor cortex that are involved in the planning and execution of the described actions. In the present study, the sensitivity of these motor structures to language processes was exploited to test linguistic theories on information layering. Human languages possess a variety of linguistic devices, so-called presupposition triggers, that allow us to convey background information without asserting it. A statement such as "Marie stopped smoking" presupposes, without asserting it, that Marie used to smoke. How such presupposed information is represented in the brain is not yet understood. Using a grip-force sensor that allows capturing motor brain activity during language processing, we investigated effects of information layering by comparing asserted information that is known to trigger motor activity ("In the living room, Peter irons his shirt") with information embedded under a presuppositional factive verb construction ("Louis knows that Peter irons his shirt"; Experiment 1) and a nonfactive verb construction ("Louis believes that Peter irons his shirt"; Experiment 2). Furthermore, we examined whether the projection behavior of a factive verb construction modulates grip force under negation ("Louis does not know that Peter irons his shirt"; Experiment 3). The data show that only the presupposed action verb in affirmative contexts (Experiment 1) triggers an increase in grip force comparable to the one of asserted action verbs, whereas the nonfactive complement and projection structure show a weaker response (Experiments 2 and 3). While the first two experiments seem to confirm the sensitivity of the grip-force response to the construction of a plausible situation or event model, in which the motor action is represented as taking place, the third one raises the question of how robust this hypothesis is and how it can take the specificity of projection into account. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).


Asunto(s)
Encéfalo , Lenguaje , Encéfalo/fisiología , Gemifloxacina , Mano , Fuerza de la Mano , Humanos
7.
Cereb Cortex ; 20(5): 1153-63, 2010 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19684250

RESUMEN

The sensitivity of the left ventral occipito-temporal (vOT) cortex to visual word processing has triggered a considerable debate about the role of this region in reading. One popular view is that the left vOT underlies the perceptual expertise needed for rapid skilled reading. Because skilled reading breaks down when words are presented in a visually unfamiliar format, we tested this hypothesis by analyzing vOT responses to horizontally presented words (familiar format) and vertically presented words (unfamiliar format). In addition, we compared the activity in participants with left and right cerebral dominance for language generation. Our results revealed 1) that the vOT activity during reading is lateralized to the same side as the inferior frontal activity during word generation, 2) that vertically and horizontally presented words triggered the same amount of activity in the vOT of the dominant hemisphere, but 3) that there was significantly more activity for vertically presented words in the vOT of the nondominant hemisphere. We suggest that the reading-related activity in vOT reflects the integration of general perceptual processes with language processing in the anterior brain regions and is not limited to skilled reading in the familiar horizontal format.


Asunto(s)
Lateralidad Funcional/fisiología , Lenguaje , Lóbulo Occipital/fisiología , Reconocimiento en Psicología/fisiología , Lóbulo Temporal/fisiología , Vocabulario , Adulto , Análisis de Varianza , Mapeo Encefálico , Toma de Decisiones/fisiología , Electroencefalografía/métodos , Femenino , Humanos , Procesamiento de Imagen Asistido por Computador/métodos , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética/métodos , Masculino , Lóbulo Occipital/irrigación sanguínea , Oxígeno/sangre , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Lectura , Lóbulo Temporal/irrigación sanguínea , Adulto Joven
8.
J Cogn ; 3(1): 35, 2020 Sep 30.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33043245

RESUMEN

Whether language comprehension requires the participation of brain structures that evolved for perception and action has been a subject of intense debate. While brain-imaging evidence for the involvement of such modality-specific regions has grown, the fact that lesions to these structures do not necessarily erase word knowledge has invited the conclusion that language-induced activity in these structures might not be essential for word recognition. Why language processing recruits these structures remains unanswered, however. Here, we examine the original findings from a slightly different perspective. We first consider the 'original' function of structures in modality-specific brain regions that are recruited by language activity. We propose that these structures help elaborate 'internal forward models' in motor control (c.f. emulators). Emulators are brain systems that capture the relationship between an action and its sensory consequences. During language processing emulators could thus allow accessing associative memories. We further postulate the existence of a linguistic system that exploits, in a rule-based manner, emulators and other nonlinguistic brain systems, to gain complementary (and redundant) information during language processing. Emulators are therefore just one of several sources of information. We emphasize that whether a given word-form triggers activity in modality-specific brain regions depends on the linguistic context and not on the word-form as such. The role of modality-specific systems in language processing is thus not to help understanding words but to model the verbally depicted situation by supplying memorized context information. We present a model derived from these assumptions and provide predictions and perspectives for future research.

9.
Neuropsychologia ; 46(2): 743-56, 2008 Jan 31.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18037143

RESUMEN

Recent studies have demonstrated that processing of action words recruits cortical motor regions that are also involved in the planning and execution of the actions words refer to. The functional role of these regions in word understanding remains, however, to be clarified. The present study investigates this issue by examining the impact of Parkinson's disease (PD) on lexical decision performance for action words, relative to concrete nouns, in a masked priming paradigm. Priming effects for the two word categories were measured in non-demented PD patients off and on dopaminergic treatment, and in healthy participants. Our results revealed that although overall performances did not differ between verbs and nouns, priming effects showed a clear dissociation between word categories. While priming for concrete nouns was not affected by Levodopa intake, it dissociated as a function of treatment for action verbs. No priming was actually obtained for action verbs in PD patients off dopaminergic treatment. Following Levodopa intake, this deficit recovered, however, because priming effects for verbs became comparable to those for concrete nouns and similar to performance of healthy participants. Overall, this study thus brings compelling evidence that processing lexico-semantic information about action words depends on the integrity of the motor system.


Asunto(s)
Trastornos del Lenguaje/complicaciones , Corteza Motora/fisiología , Destreza Motora , Enfermedad de Parkinson/complicaciones , Semántica , Anciano , Análisis de Varianza , Antiparkinsonianos/uso terapéutico , Estudios de Casos y Controles , Señales (Psicología) , Femenino , Humanos , Trastornos del Lenguaje/diagnóstico , Trastornos del Lenguaje/tratamiento farmacológico , Levodopa/uso terapéutico , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Corteza Motora/efectos de los fármacos , Corteza Motora/fisiopatología , Enfermedad de Parkinson/tratamiento farmacológico , Enfermedad de Parkinson/fisiopatología , Tiempo de Reacción/efectos de los fármacos , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Valores de Referencia , Terminología como Asunto
10.
J Physiol Paris ; 102(1-3): 130-6, 2008.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18485678

RESUMEN

Recent evidence has shown that processing action-related language and motor action share common neural representations to a point that the two processes can interfere when performed concurrently. To support the assumption that language-induced motor activity contributes to action word understanding, the present study aimed at ruling out that this activity results from mental imagery of the movements depicted by the words. For this purpose, we examined cross-talk between action word processing and an arm reaching movement, using words that were presented too fast to be consciously perceived (subliminally). Encephalogram (EEG) and movement kinematics were recorded. EEG recordings of the "Readiness potential" ("RP", indicator of motor preparation) revealed that subliminal displays of action verbs during movement preparation reduced the RP and affected the subsequent reaching movement. The finding that motor processes were modulated by language processes despite the fact that words were not consciously perceived, suggests that cortical structures that serve the preparation and execution of motor actions are indeed part of the (action) language processing network.


Asunto(s)
Electroencefalografía , Lenguaje , Procesos Mentales/fisiología , Movimiento/fisiología , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Adulto , Fenómenos Biomecánicos , Variación Contingente Negativa , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Semántica , Factores de Tiempo
11.
Neuroimage Clin ; 19: 454-465, 2018.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29984154

RESUMEN

The ARX (Aristaless Related homeoboX) gene was identified in 2002 as responsible for XLAG syndrome, a lissencephaly characterized by an almost complete absence of cortical GABAergic interneurons, and for milder forms of X-linked Intellectual Disability (ID) without apparent brain abnormalities. The most frequent mutation found in the ARX gene, a duplication of 24 base pairs (c.429_452dup24) in exon 2, results in a recognizable syndrome in which patients present ID without primary motor impairment, but with a very specific upper limb distal motor apraxia associated with a pathognomonic hand-grip, described as developmental Limb Kinetic Apraxia (LKA). In this study, we first present ARX expression during human fetal brain development showing that it is strongly expressed in GABAergic neuronal progenitors during the second and third trimester of pregnancy. We show that although ARX expression strongly decreases towards the end of gestation, it is still present after birth in some neurons of the basal ganglia, thalamus and cerebral cortex, suggesting that ARX also plays a role in more mature neuron functioning. Then, using morphometric brain MRI in 13 ARX patients carrying c.429_452dup24 mutation and in 13 sex- and age-matched healthy controls, we show that ARX patients have a significantly decreased volume of several brain structures including the striatum (and more specifically the caudate nucleus), hippocampus and thalamus as well as decreased precentral gyrus cortical thickness. We observe a significant correlation between caudate nucleus volume reduction and motor impairment severity quantified by kinematic parameter of precision grip. As basal ganglia are known to regulate sensorimotor processing and are involved in the control of precision gripping, the combined decrease in cortical thickness of primary motor cortex and basal ganglia volume in ARX dup24 patients is very likely the anatomical substrate of this developmental form of LKA.


Asunto(s)
Ganglios Basales/metabolismo , Genes Homeobox/genética , Proteínas de Homeodominio/genética , Mutación/genética , Factores de Transcripción/genética , Apraxia Ideomotora/genética , Proteína Doblecortina , Femenino , Fuerza de la Mano/fisiología , Humanos , Interneuronas/metabolismo , Neuronas/metabolismo , Embarazo , Ácido gamma-Aminobutírico/metabolismo
12.
Cognition ; 103(1): 131-46, 2007 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16616074

RESUMEN

There is growing evidence that words that are acquired early in life are processed faster and more accurately than words acquired later, even by adults. As neuropsychological and neuroimaging studies have implicated different brain networks in the processing of action verbs and concrete nouns, the present study was aimed at contrasting reaction times to early and later-acquired action verbs and concrete nouns, in order to determine whether effects of word learning age express differently for the two types of words. Our results show that while word frequency affected both types of words in the same way, distinct learning age effects were observed for action verbs and concrete nouns. A further experiment specified that this difference was observed for verbs describing actions belonging to the human motor repertoire, but not for verbs denoting actions past this repertoire (e.g., to neigh). We interpret these data within a recently emerging framework according to which language processing is associated with sensory motor programs.


Asunto(s)
Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Aprendizaje Verbal , Vocabulario , Adolescente , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Lingüística , Masculino , Red Nerviosa/fisiología
13.
Brain Res ; 1185: 212-20, 2007 Dec 14.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17956755

RESUMEN

According to the 'bilateral representation theory', a complete copy of the words presented foveally is received simultaneously in the left and right visual cortices. However, a growing body of observations, which has led to the 'split fovea theory', proposes a functional split of the foveal area between the two hemispheres. In the present study we tested these two accounts using an adapted version of the Reicher-Wheeler paradigm. Ten control participants and ten participants with developmental dyslexia undergoing electroencephalographic recordings were asked to identify one of five letters in a string. The target letter was systematically presented at fixation but the horizontal positioning of the letter string was varied such that the stimulus fluctuated in both the visual hemifields over the experiment. ERP results showed that letter strings encompassing the foveal field were not sent to both cerebral hemispheres simultaneously when fixation coincided with extreme letter positions (i.e., first or last). Indeed, the P1 peak was delayed in this case, which was interpreted as the result of a transfer of visual information from the contralateral hemisphere via the splenium of the corpus callosum. Consistent with the 'split fovea theory', this result suggests that a minimal amount of graphic input is necessary to induce a P1 event. The interhemispheric transfer time (IHTT) deducted from peak-to-peak P1 latency delays ranged from 26 to 42 ms. As previously observed, the IHTT was significantly faster for right-to-left than left-to-right transfer in the control group. IHTT was marginally shorter in control participants as compared to participants with developmental dyslexia, and the faster transfer to the left hemisphere seen in the former was not found in the latter.


Asunto(s)
Dislexia/fisiopatología , Potenciales Evocados Visuales/fisiología , Fóvea Central/fisiología , Lateralidad Funcional/fisiología , Corteza Visual/fisiología , Campos Visuales/fisiología , Adulto , Análisis de Varianza , Mapeo Encefálico , Conducta de Elección/fisiología , Dislexia/patología , Femenino , Fijación Ocular , Humanos , Masculino , Modelos Biológicos , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología
14.
J Vis ; 7(2): 18.1-12, 2007 Aug 16.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18217833

RESUMEN

Processes underlying crowding in visual letter recognition were examined by investigating effects of training. Experiment 1 revealed that training reduces crowding mainly for trained strings. This was corroborated in Experiment 2, where no training effects were obvious after 3 days of training when strings changed from trial to trial. Experiment 3 specified that after a short amount of training, learning effects remained specific to trained strings and also to the trained retinal eccentricity and the interletter spacing used in training. Transfer to other than trained conditions was observed only after further training. Experiment 4 showed that transfer occurred earlier when words were used as stimuli. These results thus demonstrate that part of crowding results from the absence of higher level representations of the stimulus. Such representations can be acquired through learning visual properties of the stimulus.


Asunto(s)
Aprendizaje/fisiología , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología , Enmascaramiento Perceptual/fisiología , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Percepción Espacial/fisiología , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Adulto , Humanos , Campos Visuales
15.
Brain Res ; 1098(1): 153-60, 2006 Jul 07.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16774747

RESUMEN

Most classical models of visual word recognition are based on sequentially organized levels of representation and involve feedback mechanisms to various extents. In this study, we aim at clarifying which of the early processing stages of visual word recognition are modulated by top-down lexical effects. We studied the identification of letters embedded in briefly presented words (e.g., TABLE) and illegal nonwords (e.g., GTFRS) using event-related potentials (ERPs). Participants were involved in the Reicher-Wheeler paradigm: they were asked to indicate which of two letters displayed above and below a string of hashes was flashed immediately before at fixation within a letter string, which was either a word or a nonword. Event-related potentials were significantly modulated by the lexical status of stimuli around 200 ms after stimulus onset, i.e., in the peaking window of the N1 component. In light of our results, we propose that visual word form representations can constrain letter identification at a prelexical stage i.e., during the extraction of letter-shape information. In addition, we show that this facilitatory top-down effect is sensitive to stimulus exposure duration.


Asunto(s)
Potenciales Evocados/fisiología , Lectura , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Adulto , Electroencefalografía , Electrofisiología , Femenino , Lateralidad Funcional/fisiología , Humanos , Masculino , Lóbulo Parietal/fisiología , Psicolingüística , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Tiempo de Reacción , Reconocimiento en Psicología/fisiología , Lóbulo Temporal/fisiología
16.
Front Psychol ; 7: 2016, 2016.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28082939

RESUMEN

Non-verbal social interaction between humans requires accurate understanding of the others' actions. The cognitivist approach suggests that successful interaction depends on the creation of a shared representation of the task, where the pairing of perceptive and motor systems of partners allows inclusion of the other's goal into the overarching representation. Activity of the Mirror Neurons System (MNS) is thought to be a crucial mechanism linking two individuals during a joint action through action observation. The construction of a shared representation of an interaction (i.e., joint action) depends upon sensorimotor cognitive processes that modulate the ability to adapt in time and space. We attempted to detect individuals' behavioral/kinematic change resulting in a global amelioration of performance for both subjects when a common representation of the action is built using a repetitive joint action. We asked pairs of subjects to carry out a simple task where one puts a base in the middle of a table and the other places a parallelepiped fitting into the base, the crucial manipulation being that participants switched roles during the experiment. We aimed to show that a full comprehension of a joint action is not an automatic process. We found that, before switching the interactional role, the participant initially placing the base orientated it in a way that led to an uncomfortable action for participants placing the parallelepiped. However, after switching roles, the action's kinematics by the participant who places the base changed in order to facilitate the action of the other. More precisely, our data shows significant modulation of the base angle in order to ease the completion of the joint action, highlighting the fact that a shared knowledge of the complete action facilitates the generation of a common representation. This evidence suggests the ability to establish an efficient shared representation of a joint action benefits from physically taking our partner's perspective because simply observing the actions of others may not be enough.

17.
Front Psychol ; 7: 1500, 2016.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27826259

RESUMEN

Scalar implicatures, the phenomena where a sentence like "The pianist played some Mozart sonatas" is interpreted, as "The pianist did not play all Mozart sonatas" have been given two different analyses. Neo-Griceans (NG) claim that this interpretation is based on lexical scales (e.g., ), where the stronger term (e.g., all) implies the weaker term (e.g., some), but the weaker term (e.g., some) implicates the negation of the stronger term (i.e., some = not all). Post-Griceans (PG) deny that this is the case and offer a context-based inferential account for scalar implicatures. While scalar implicatures have been extensively investigated, with results apparently in favor of PG accounts, the psychological reality of lexical scales has not been put to the test. This is what we have done in the present experiment, with a lexical decision task using lexical scales in a masked priming paradigm. While PG accounts do not attribute any role for lexical scales in the computation of scalar implicatures, NG accounts suggest that lexical scales are the core mechanism behind the computation of scalar implicatures, and predict that weaker terms in a scale should prime stronger terms more than the reverse because stronger words are necessary to the interpretation of weaker words, while stronger words can be interpreted independently of weaker words. Our results provided evidence in favor of the psychological existence of scales, leading to the first clear experimental support for the NG account.

18.
PLoS One ; 11(2): e0149717, 2016.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26918704

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Intellectual Disability (ID) is characterized by deficits in intellectual functions such as reasoning, problem-solving, planning, abstract thinking, judgment, and learning. As new avenues are emerging for treatment of genetically determined ID (such as Down's syndrome or Fragile X syndrome), it is necessary to identify objective reliable and sensitive outcome measures for use in clinical trials. OBJECTIVE: We developed a novel visual analogical reasoning paradigm, inspired by the Progressive Raven's Matrices, but appropriate for Intellectually Disabled patients. This new paradigm assesses reasoning and inhibition abilities in ID patients. METHODS: We performed behavioural analyses for this task (with a reaction time and error rate analysis, Study 1) in 96 healthy controls (adults and typically developed children older than 4) and 41 genetically determined ID patients (Fragile X syndrome, Down syndrome and ARX mutated patients). In order to establish and quantify the cognitive strategies used to solve the task, we also performed an eye-tracking analysis (Study 2). RESULTS: Down syndrome, ARX and Fragile X patients were significantly slower and made significantly more errors than chronological age-matched healthy controls. The effect of inhibition on error rate was greater than the matrix complexity effect in ID patients, opposite to findings in adult healthy controls. Interestingly, ID patients were more impaired by inhibition than mental age-matched healthy controls, but not by the matrix complexity. Eye-tracking analysis made it possible to identify the strategy used by the participants to solve the task. Adult healthy controls used a matrix-based strategy, whereas ID patients used a response-based strategy. Furthermore, etiologic-specific reasoning differences were evidenced between ID patients groups. CONCLUSION: We suggest that this paradigm, appropriate for ID patients and developmental populations as well as adult healthy controls, provides an objective and quantitative assessment of visual analogical reasoning and cognitive inhibition, enabling testing for the effect of pharmacological or behavioural intervention in these specific populations.


Asunto(s)
Discapacidad Intelectual/psicología , Pensamiento , Adolescente , Adulto , Estudios de Casos y Controles , Cognición , Síndrome de Down/fisiopatología , Síndrome de Down/psicología , Femenino , Síndrome del Cromosoma X Frágil/fisiopatología , Síndrome del Cromosoma X Frágil/psicología , Proteínas de Homeodominio/genética , Humanos , Discapacidad Intelectual/genética , Discapacidad Intelectual/fisiopatología , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Mutación , Factores de Transcripción/genética , Adulto Joven
19.
Cogn Neuropsychol ; 15(1-2): 93-140, 1998 Mar 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28657522

RESUMEN

In the present article, we investigated the reading ability of CP, a pure alexic patient, using an experimental paradigm that is known to elicit the viewing position effect in norm al readers. The viewing position effect consists of a systematic variation of word recognition performance as a function of fixation location w ithin a word: Word recognition is best when the eyes fixate slightly left from the word centre and decreases when the eyes deviate from this optimal viewing position. A mathematical model (Nazir, O'Regan, & Jacobs, 1991), which provides a good description and quantification of the prototypical shape of the viewing position effect, served to interpret CP's reading performance. The results show ed that, like normal readers, CP was able to process all letters of a w ord in one fixation. However, in contrast to normal readers, reading performance was optimal when CP w as fixating the right half of the word. This somewhat abnormal pattern of performance was due to (1) poor perceptual processing in the right visual field, and (2) poor processing of letters situated towards the end of the word, independent of visual field presentation. A similar pattern of perform ance w as obtained with normal readers under experimental conditions in which lexical know ledge was of restricted use. We suggest that CP's reading impairment stems from a dysfunction in the coupling between incoming visual information and stored lexical information. This dysfunction is thought to uncover a prelexical level of word processing, where letter information is weighted differently as a function of letter position in a word-centred space.

20.
Brain Lang ; 88(3): 294-311, 2004 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-14967213

RESUMEN

The present work aims at demonstrating that visual training associated with the act of reading modifies the way we perceive printed words. As reading does not train all parts of the retina in the same way but favors regions on the side in the direction of scanning, visual word recognition should be better at retinal locations that are frequently used during reading. In two studies that probed word and letter discriminations we provided evidence for a correlation between eye fixation pattern during reading and performance. We showed that effects of reading-related visual training were stimulus-specific in the sense that it affected the perception of words but not that of visually unfamiliar non-words. This stimulus specificity was also evident in the legibility of individual characters of the Roman and the Hebrew scripts-two scripts that are read in opposing directions. When displayed within a sequence of homogenous letters (e.g., xxexx) the legibility of a target character varied with the location of the sequence in the visual field and with the serial position of the target within the sequence. This retinal location- and context-dependency differed between Roman and Hebrew characters. These results seem to indicate that reading modifies the functional structure of early stages in the visual pathway. The cortical network that supports reading seems to comprise components of the visual cortex of both hemispheres before it lateralizes to the left hemisphere. Expanding the reading network to include these visual regions will shed a different light on the potential role of the visual word form area (e.g., ) in word recognition and on the organization of the reading system in general.


Asunto(s)
Hábitos , Aprendizaje , Percepción , Lectura , Reconocimiento en Psicología , Vocabulario , Análisis de Varianza , Fijación Ocular/fisiología , Humanos , Campos Visuales/fisiología , Percepción Visual
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
Detalles de la búsqueda