Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 5 de 5
Filtrar
1.
Health Commun ; 31(12): 1447-59, 2016 12.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27054822

RESUMEN

How we cope with the many stressors that we encounter throughout our lives has implications for our well-being. By affecting how individuals appraise stressful events, communication can prolong or ameliorate physiological and emotional responses to stress. This study investigated the short-term effects of hope-inducing and rumination-inducing messages on heart rate, state anxiety, and emotions after a standardized, social-evaluative stressor. Continuous heart rate was monitored for 127 college students (64 female, 63 male) throughout an experiment that included a performance stressor and messages designed to (a) cause feelings of hope, (b) evoke rumination, or (c) be a distraction (control). Heart rate varied by message, such that heart rate was lowest in the hope evocation condition. State anxiety was lower in the hope evocation and distraction control conditions than in the rumination condition. The rumination condition led to greater anger, greater guilt, and less happiness than did the other conditions. This study advances our knowledge about potential ways that communication messages can counter the psychological and biological effects of stressful life events. Overall, the study provides preliminary evidence that hope evocation messages may be a form of supportive communication and can ameliorate stress.


Asunto(s)
Ansiedad/psicología , Comunicación , Emociones , Frecuencia Cardíaca/fisiología , Esperanza , Estrés Psicológico , Pensamiento , Adolescente , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Análisis y Desempeño de Tareas , Adulto Joven
2.
Stress ; 18(5): 554-60, 2015.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26119263

RESUMEN

Ruminative thought about stressors has been linked to extended post-stressor cardiovascular activation, which in turn predicts negative long-term health outcomes. Past work indicates that the nature of thought (mental imagery or verbal thought) may shape cardiovascular responses. Some evidence suggests that individuals with rumination tendencies may be especially vulnerable to stress-related cardiovascular activation, although it is unclear to what extent type of thought (imagery or verbal thought) influences this relationship. This study included a laboratory stressor followed by a stressor recall task in which mentation type was manipulated. Healthy undergraduate students (N = 138; 47% female) underwent a speech stressor and then were randomly assigned to complete a stressor recall task using either mental imagery or verbal-linguistic mentation. Heart rate (HR) and blood pressure (BP) were measured throughout. Self-report trait rumination was measured at baseline. Results indicated that trait rumination and mentation type interacted to predict HR. Individuals with high trait rumination scores had significantly greater increases in HR during the verbal-linguistic conditions compared to the mental imagery conditions. There were no mentation type differences in the low trait ruminators, no differences in BP and no main effects of trait rumination. Results suggest that mentation type may be a key in understanding the relationship between rumination and cardiovascular activation, especially for trait ruminators.


Asunto(s)
Presión Sanguínea/fisiología , Frecuencia Cardíaca/fisiología , Imaginación/fisiología , Recuerdo Mental/fisiología , Personalidad , Estrés Psicológico/fisiopatología , Pensamiento , Adolescente , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Autoinforme , Habla , Adulto Joven
3.
J Pediatr Psychol ; 37(9): 990-8, 2012 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22753443

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVE: Given the inconsistent relationship between stress and metabolic control, the purpose of this study was to empirically derive patterns of perceived diabetes-related stress among youth with type 1 diabetes mellitus (T1DM) and determine if these patterns relate to overall diabetes-related stress levels and metabolic control. METHOD: A sample of 204 youth with T1DM completed the diabetes stress questionnaire, and their hemoglobin A(1c) (a long-term measure of metabolic control) was obtained from their medical record. RESULTS: Latent profile analyses revealed three perceived-stress profiles: "low stress" (LS), "interpersonal/peer" (IP), and "family stress" (FS). The FS and IP groups reported more overall stress than the LS group; however, only the FS group's HbA(1c) values were significantly higher than either the LS or IP groups. CONCLUSIONS: A global measure of stress may not accurately account for the association between perceived stress and metabolic control. FS, rather than IP stress seems to be a key stress domain linked to suboptimal metabolic control.


Asunto(s)
Diabetes Mellitus Tipo 1/psicología , Familia/psicología , Hemoglobina Glucada/metabolismo , Relaciones Interpersonales , Estrés Psicológico/psicología , Adolescente , Glucemia/metabolismo , Niño , Diabetes Mellitus Tipo 1/sangre , Diabetes Mellitus Tipo 1/tratamiento farmacológico , Femenino , Humanos , Hipoglucemiantes/uso terapéutico , Masculino , Grupo Paritario , Encuestas y Cuestionarios
4.
Stress Health ; 30(3): 188-97, 2014 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25100270

RESUMEN

Psychological detachment from work is important for facilitating recovery. This can be threatened by rumination, or thinking about the day's stressors. Rumination may lead to distress, fatigue and extended activation of stress-related systems, but findings are not unequivocal. Level of construal (abstract or concrete) and type of mentation (imagery or verbal thought) used during stressor-focused rumination may shape physiological and affective responses and impact recovery. This study tested whether blood pressure (BP) and anxiety responses to stressor-focused rumination differ by mentation type and construal level. Healthy undergraduates (n = 136) performed a speech stressor and then completed a rumination task in one of four randomly assigned conditions: concrete imagery, abstract imagery, concrete verbal thought or abstract verbal thought. Anxiety and continuous BP were assessed. Concrete rumination led to greater BP, whereas rumination with abstract construals led to lower BP. Furthermore, participants in the abstract conditions had greater increases in anxiety following stressor-focused rumination than in the concrete conditions. Results suggest that the immediate physiological and psychological consequences of stressor-focused rumination depend upon mode of thought.


Asunto(s)
Afecto/fisiología , Presión Sanguínea/fisiología , Estrés Psicológico/psicología , Pensamiento/fisiología , Análisis de Varianza , Ansiedad/etiología , Ansiedad/fisiopatología , Femenino , Humanos , Imaginación/fisiología , Masculino , Recuerdo Mental/fisiología , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Autoeficacia , Estrés Psicológico/fisiopatología , Adulto Joven
5.
Health Psychol ; 33(12): 1606-9, 2014 Dec.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24467256

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVE: Stress-related physiological activation may last longer for those who ruminate, or dwell, on past stressors. Correlational and quasi-experimental research has linked rumination to immune activity and elevated cortisol. This study's aim was to experimentally test whether rumination (relative to distraction) can sustain stress-induced increases in inflammation and cortisol. Concentrations of poststressor cortisol and inflammatory markers were hypothesized to be greater for those who ruminated compared with those who were distracted. METHOD: Thirty-four healthy young women completed a laboratory speech stressor and were then randomly assigned to either ruminate on the stressor or engage in distraction for 5 minutes. Salivary cortisol and circulating plasma concentrations of C-reactive protein (CRP), interleukin-6 (IL-6), and tumor necrosis factor-alpha (TNF-α) were assessed throughout the 2-hr visit. RESULTS: As predicted, CRP and cortisol responses differed for the rumination and distraction groups. In the distraction group, participants' CRP concentrations increased poststressor and then returned to prestressor levels by the end of the visit. In contrast, participants in the rumination condition demonstrated increases in CRP that did not return to prestressor levels by the end of the visit. Similarly, poststressor cortisol was higher for those who ruminated compared with those who were distracted. Plasma IL-6 and TNF-α concentrations increased over the visit, but did not differ by experimental group. CONCLUSIONS: RESULTS suggest that ruminating on stressors may sustain CRP and cortisol responses, whereas distraction may diminish them. Findings have implications for understanding potential risk and protective factors for stress-related activation.


Asunto(s)
Proteína C-Reactiva/metabolismo , Hidrocortisona/metabolismo , Estrés Psicológico/metabolismo , Estrés Psicológico/psicología , Pensamiento/fisiología , Proteína C-Reactiva/análisis , Femenino , Estudios de Seguimiento , Humanos , Hidrocortisona/análisis , Interleucina-6/sangre , Saliva/química , Factor de Necrosis Tumoral alfa/sangre , Adulto Joven
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
Detalles de la búsqueda