Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 83
Filtrar
1.
Aggress Behav ; 48(5): 487-499, 2022 09.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35560230

RESUMEN

Despite its adaptive value for social life, the emergence and the development of the ability to detect agents that cause aversive interactions and distinguish them from potentially affiliative agents (approachers) has not been investigated. We presented infants with a simple interaction involving two agents: one of them (the "repulser") moved toward and pushed the other (the "approacher") which reacted by simply moving toward the repulser without contacting it. We found that 8-month-olds (N = 28) looked longer at the approacher than at the repulser (Experiment 1), whereas 4-month-olds (N = 30) exhibited no preference (Experiment 2). To control for low-level cues (such as the preference for the agent that moved after the contact), two new groups of 4- and 8-month-old infants were presented with a series of interactions in which the agents inverted their social roles. Older infants (N = 30) manifested no preference for either agent (Experiment 3), while younger infants (N = 30) looked longer at the first agent to move (Experiment 4). Our results indicated that 8-month-olds' preferences for the approacher over the repulser depended on social information and were finely tuned to agents that display prosocial rather than antisocial behavior. We discuss these findings in light of the development and adaptive value of the ability to negatively evaluate repulsers, to avoid choosing them as partners.


Asunto(s)
Trastorno de Personalidad Antisocial , Señales (Psicología) , Humanos , Lactante
2.
Biochem Biophys Res Commun ; 564: 37-42, 2021 07 30.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33280818

RESUMEN

Non-symbolic numerical abilities are widespread among vertebrates due to their important adaptive value. Moreover, these abilities were considered peculiar of vertebrate species as numerical competence is regarded as cognitively sophisticated. However, recent evidence convincingly showed that this is not the case: invertebrates, with their limited number of neurons, proved able to successfully discriminate different quantities (e.g., of prey), to use the ordinal property of numbers, to solve arithmetic operations as addition and subtraction and even to master the concept of zero numerosity. To date, though, the debate is still open on the presence and the nature of a «sense of number¼ in invertebrates. Whether this is peculiar for discrete countable quantities (numerosities) or whether this is part of a more general magnitude system dealing with both discrete and continuous quantities, as hypothesized for humans and other vertebrates. Here we reviewed the main studies on numerical abilities of invertebrates, discussing in particular the recent findings supporting the hypothesis of a general mechanism that allows for processing of both discrete (i.e., number) and continuous dimensions (e.g., space).


Asunto(s)
Cognición/fisiología , Invertebrados/fisiología , Animales
3.
Proc Biol Sci ; 288(1957): 20211570, 2021 08 25.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34428963

RESUMEN

Grouping sets of elements into smaller, equal-sized, subsets constitutes a perceptual strategy employed by humans and other animals to enhance cognitive performance. Here, we show that day-old chicks can solve extremely complex numerical discriminations (Exp.1), and that their performance can be enhanced by the presence of symmetrical/asymmetrical colour grouping (Exp.2 versus Exp.3). Newborn chicks were habituated for 1 h to even numerosities (sets of elements presented on a screen) and then tested for their spontaneous choice among what for humans would be considered a prime and a non-prime odd numerosity. Chicks discriminated and preferred the prime over the composite set of elements irrespective of its relative magnitude (i.e. 7 versus 9 and 11 versus 9). We discuss this result in terms of novelty preference. By employing a more complex contrast (i.e. 13 versus 15), we investigated the limits of such a mechanism and showed that induced grouping positively affects chicks' performance. Our results suggest the existence of a spontaneous mechanism that enables chicks to create symmetrical (i.e. same-sized) subgroups of sets of elements. Chicks preferentially inspected numerosities for which same-sized grouping is never possible (i.e. the prime numerosity) rather than numerosities allowing for symmetrical grouping (i.e. composite).


Asunto(s)
Pollos , Percepción Visual , Animales , Humanos
4.
Learn Behav ; 49(1): 54-66, 2021 03.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33025570

RESUMEN

When facing two sets of imprinting objects of different numerousness, domestic chicks prefer to approach the larger one. Given that choice for familiar and novel stimuli in imprinting situations is known to be affected by the sex of the animals, we investigated how male and female domestic chicks divide the time spent in the proximity of a familiar versus an unfamiliar number of objects, and how animals interact (by pecking) with these objects. We confirmed that chicks discriminate among the different numerousnesses, but we also showed that females and males behave differently, depending on the degree of familiarity of the objects. When objects in the testing sets were all familiar, females equally explored both sets and pecked at all objects individually. Males instead selectively approached the familiar numerousness and pecked more at it. When both testing sets comprised familiar as well as novel objects, both males and females approached the larger numerousness of familiar objects. However, chicks directed all their pecks toward the novel object within the set. Differences in the behavior of males and females can be accounted for in terms of sex difference in the motivation to reinstate social contact with the familiar objects and to explore novel ones, likely associated with the ecology and the social structure of the species before domestication.


Asunto(s)
Pollos , Reconocimiento en Psicología , Animales , Femenino , Masculino
5.
Laterality ; 26(1-2): 1-18, 2021.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32698726

RESUMEN

In order to face a constantly changing environment, animals need to be able to update their knowledge of the world on the basis of new information. Often, this means to inhibit a previously acquired response and flexibly change their behaviour to produce a new response. Here, we measured such abilities in young domestic chicks, employing a Colour Reversal Learning Task. During the acquisition phase, 17 one-week-old male chicks had to learn to peck on one of two coloured boxes to obtain a food reward. After reaching criterion, chicks underwent a reversal phase in which the previously learned colour-reward contingency was swapped. As expected from the literature, chicks performed better in the acquisition phase with respect to the reversal phase. Results moreover highlighted the presence of a lateralized bias selectively during reversal: chicks performed better if the stimulus rewarded was located in the left hemispace (processed by the right hemisphere). Interestingly, the bias correlated with the individual difficulty, i.e., it was stronger in those chicks which needed more trials to complete the reversal session. The present study contributes evidence in support of behavioural flexibility in young chicks, along with a novel perspective on lateralized mechanisms that might underlie such ability.


Asunto(s)
Lateralidad Funcional , Aprendizaje Inverso , Animales , Pollos , Masculino , Recompensa
6.
Anim Cogn ; 23(6): 1119-1127, 2020 Nov.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32076920

RESUMEN

In a constantly changing environment, it is advantageous for animals to encode a location (such as a food source) relying on more than one single cue. A certain position might, in fact, be signalled by the presence of information acquired through different sensory modalities which may be integrated into cohesive memories. Here, we aimed to investigate multi-sensory learning capabilities and multi-modal information integration in Lasius niger ants. Individual ants were placed in a Y-maze where odour information always led to a food reward; moreover, arm and wall colour were also predictive but only when co-occurring with odour in a specific combination. At test, the odour cue was made uninformative (it was present in both arms). Ants were still able to correctly locate the reward by integrating odour with the right colour and side combination. In a second experiment, we tested whether multi-modal cue integration can take place in a single trial. To this end, ants were exposed to a rewarded odour in a single-arm maze and could experience the Y-maze (with all available cues) only once. At test (which was identical to that of Experiment 1), ants showed a slight preference for the correct colour-side combination, although not significantly different from chance level. Our results showed the capability of black garden ants to code apparently redundant contextual information and to create and rely on conditional relationships between the information available. We argue that future studies should deepen the inquiry on the timing and progression of multi-modal cue learning.


Asunto(s)
Hormigas , Animales , Color , Señales (Psicología) , Aprendizaje , Memoria
7.
Dev Sci ; 22(6): e12801, 2019 11.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30676679

RESUMEN

Humans represent numbers on a mental number line with smaller numbers on the left and larger numbers on the right side. A left-to-right oriented spatial-numerical association, (SNA), has been demonstrated in animals and infants. However, the possibility that SNA is learnt by early exposure to caregivers' directional biases is still open. We conducted two experiments: in Experiment 1, we tested whether SNA is present at birth and in Experiment 2, we studied whether it depends on the relative rather than the absolute magnitude of numerousness. Fifty-five-hour-old newborns, once habituated to a number (12), spontaneously associated a smaller number (4) with the left and a larger number (36) with the right side (Experiment 1). SNA in neonates is not absolute but relative. The same number (12) was associated with the left side rather than the right side whenever the previously experienced number was larger (36) rather than smaller (4) (Experiment 2). Control on continuous physical variables showed that the effect is specific of discrete magnitudes. These results constitute strong evidence that in our species SNA originates from pre-linguistic and biological precursors in the brain.


Asunto(s)
Conceptos Matemáticos , Percepción Espacial/fisiología , Femenino , Humanos , Recién Nacido , Aprendizaje , Lingüística , Masculino
9.
Neurobiol Learn Mem ; 133: 13-18, 2016 09.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27246250

RESUMEN

Pre-verbal infants and non-human animals associate small numbers with the left space and large numbers with the right space. Birds and primates, trained to identify a given position in a sagittal series of identical positions, whenever required to respond on a left/right oriented series, referred the given position starting from the left end. Here, we extended this evidence by selectively investigating the role of either cerebral hemisphere, using the temporary monocular occlusion technique. In birds, lacking the corpus callosum, visual input is fed mainly to the contralateral hemisphere. We trained 4-day-old chicks to identify the 4th element in a sagittal series of 10 identical elements. At test, the series was identical but left/right oriented. Test was conducted in right monocular, left monocular or binocular condition of vision. Right monocular chicks pecked at the 4th right element; left monocular and binocular chicks pecked at the 4th left element. Data on monocular chicks demonstrate that both hemispheres deal with an ordinal (sequential) task. Data on binocular chicks indicate that the left bias is linked to a right hemisphere dominance, that allocates the attention toward the left hemispace. This constitutes a first step towards understanding the neural basis of number space mapping.


Asunto(s)
Conducta Animal/fisiología , Pollos/fisiología , Lateralidad Funcional/fisiología , Conceptos Matemáticos , Percepción Espacial/fisiología , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Animales , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Visión Binocular/fisiología , Visión Monocular/fisiología
10.
Anim Cogn ; 19(5): 1007-17, 2016 09.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27287627

RESUMEN

Evidence of learning and generalization of visual regularities in a newborn organism is provided in the present research. Domestic chicks have been trained to discriminate visual triplets of simultaneously presented shapes, implementing AAB versus ABA (Experiment 1), AAB versus ABB and AAB versus BAA (Experiment 2). Chicks distinguished pattern-following and pattern-violating novel test triplets in all comparisons, showing no preference for repetition-based patterns. The animals generalized to novel instances even when the patterns compared were not discriminable by the presence or absence of reduplicated elements or by symmetry (e.g., AAB vs. ABB). These findings represent the first evidence of learning and generalization of regularities at the onset of life in an animal model, revealing intriguing differences with respect to human newborns and infants. Extensive prior experience seems to be unnecessary to drive the process, suggesting that chicks are predisposed to detect patterns characterizing the visual world.


Asunto(s)
Pollos , Aprendizaje , Percepción Visual , Animales , Humanos
11.
Anim Cogn ; 18(3): 605-16, 2015 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25539771

RESUMEN

We investigated whether 4-day-old domestic chicks can discriminate proportions. Chicks were trained to respond, via food reinforcement, to one of the two stimuli, each characterized by different proportions of red and green areas (» vs. ¾). In Experiment 1, chicks approached the proportion associated with food, even if at test the spatial dispositions of the two areas were novel. In Experiment 2, chicks responded on the basis of proportion even when the testing stimuli were of enlarged dimensions, creating a conflict between the absolute positive area experienced during training and the relative proportion of the two areas. However, chicks could have responded on the basis of the overall colour (red or green) of the figures rather than proportion per se. To control for this objection, in Experiment 3, we used new pairs of testing stimuli, each depicting a different number of small squares on a white background (i.e. 1 green and 3 red vs. 3 green and 1 red or 5 green and 15 red vs. 5 red and 15 green). Chicks were again able to respond to the correct proportion, showing they discriminated on the basis of proportion of continuous quantities and not on the basis of the prevalent colour or on the absolute amount of it. Data indicate that chicks can track continuous quantities through various manipulations, suggesting that proportions are information that can be processed by very young animals.


Asunto(s)
Pollos/fisiología , Aprendizaje Discriminativo , Percepción Espacial , Animales , Conducta Animal/fisiología , Color , Generalización del Estimulo
12.
Anim Cogn ; 17(4): 925-35, 2014 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24368707

RESUMEN

In this paper, we studied the ability of newborn chicks to use kind information (sortal objects) provided by social and food attractors to determine the number of distinct objects present in an event (object individuation). Newly hatched chicks were reared with five imprinting objects and were fed mealworms. Chicks' spontaneous tendency to approach the larger group of items was exploited. At test, on day 2 post-hatching, chicks observed two events in which objects, differing in kind, were each hidden behind one of two identical screens. Approaching either screen was considered a preferential choice. In Experiment 1, chicks presented with two social versus two food attractors did not exhibit any preference. In contrast, in Experiment 2, when chicks saw two different attractors (one social and one food) hidden behind a screen and one attractor hidden twice (i.e. moved back and forth two times) behind the other screen, they spontaneously approached the two different attractors rather than the single one seen twice. An explanation based on the preference for the more varied set was ruled out in Experiment 3: chicks did not preferentially choose between two different versus two identical objects when both groups were simultaneously presented. Results suggest for the first time that a non-human species uses kind information for individuating objects in a cross-basic-level contrast (i.e. food and social items) with minimal experience. As social and food stimuli differ in property as well as in kind information, the alternative explanation accounting for use of property information alone is also discussed.


Asunto(s)
Animales Recién Nacidos/psicología , Pollos , Formación de Concepto , Animales , Conducta Apetitiva , Femenino , Aprendizaje , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos , Estimulación Luminosa , Reconocimiento en Psicología , Conducta Social
13.
Anim Cogn ; 16(4): 557-64, 2013 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23334508

RESUMEN

Human adults master sophisticated, abstract numerical calculations that are mostly based on symbolic language and thus inimitably human. Humans may nonetheless share a subset of non-verbal numerical skills, available soon after birth and considered the evolutionary foundation of more complex numerical reasoning, with other animals. These skills are thought to be based on the two systems: the object file system which processes small values (<3) and the analogue magnitude system which processes large magnitudes (>4). Infants' ability to discriminate 1 vs. 2, 1 vs. 3, 2 vs. 3, but not 1 vs. 4, seems to indicate that the two systems are independent, implying that the conception of a continuous number processing system is based on precursors that appear to be interrupted at or about the number four. The findings from the study being presented here indicating that chicks are able to make a series of discriminations regarding that borderline number (1 vs. 4, 1 vs. 5, 2 vs. 4) support the hypothesis that there is continuity in the number system which processes both small and large numerousness.


Asunto(s)
Animales Recién Nacidos/psicología , Pollos , Formación de Concepto , Discriminación en Psicología , Animales , Pollos/fisiología , Conducta de Elección , Femenino
14.
Dev Sci ; 16(3): 327-35, 2013 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23587033

RESUMEN

Perception of mechanical (i.e. physical) causality, in terms of a cause-effect relationship between two motion events, appears to be a powerful mechanism in our daily experience. In spite of a growing interest in the earliest causal representations, the role of experience in the origin of this sensitivity is still a matter of dispute. Here, we asked the question about the innate origin of causal perception, never tested before at birth. Three experiments were carried out to investigate sensitivity at birth to some visual spatiotemporal cues present in a launching event. Newborn babies, only a few hours old, showed that they significantly preferred a physical causality event (i.e. Michotte's Launching effect) when matched to a delay event (i.e. a delayed launching; Experiment 1) or to a non-causal event completely identical to the causal one except for the order of the displacements of the two objects involved which was swapped temporally (Experiment 3). This preference for the launching event, moreover, also depended on the continuity of the trajectory between the objects involved in the event (Experiment 2). These results support the hypothesis that the human system possesses an early available, possibly innate basic mechanism to compute causality, such a mechanism being sensitive to the additive effect of certain well-defined spatiotemporal cues present in the causal event independently of any prior visual experience.


Asunto(s)
Cognición , Recién Nacido/psicología , Percepción de Movimiento , Percepción Espacial , Señales (Psicología) , Humanos
15.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 107(9): 4483-5, 2010 Mar 02.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20160095

RESUMEN

The idea that sensitivity to self-produced motion could lie at the foundations of the clear-cut divide that the brain operates between the two basic domains of inanimate and animate objects dates back to Aristotle. Sensitivity to self-propelled objects is apparent in human infants from around the fifth month of age, which leaves undetermined whether it is acquired by experience with animate objects or whether it is innately predisposed in the brain. Here, we report that newly hatched, visually naïve domestic chicks presented with objects exhibiting motion either self-produced or caused by physical contact prefer to associate with self-propelled objects. This finding supports the idea of an evolutionarily ancient, predisposed neural mechanism in the vertebrate brain for the detection of animacy.


Asunto(s)
Conducta Animal , Pollos/fisiología , Locomoción , Animales , Animales Recién Nacidos
16.
iScience ; 26(1): 105863, 2023 Jan 20.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36632061

RESUMEN

Newborn domestic chicks shortly exposed to a conspecific learn to recognize and prefer it over unfamiliar individuals. We assessed whether lack of physical contact or social feedback during familiarization affects affiliative preference, hypothesizing a crucial role of social responsiveness. Four-day-old chicks were tested for their preference between a familiar and an unfamiliar chick. In Exp. 1, we replicated the well-known preference for the familiar individual, even when (Exp. 2) a transparent glass prevented haptic interaction during familiarization. No preference was scored in Exp. 3, using a one-way glass, i.e., the subject could never be seen by its cagemate. The development of preferences toward a familiar but socially unresponsive cagemate was assessed by testing chicks twice (Exp. 4). While behaving at chance on day 2, birds showed a preference for the unfamiliar individual on day 4 of life. Our results highlight the importance of social interaction already in the first stages of life, irrespective of familiarity.

17.
Animals (Basel) ; 13(22)2023 Nov 08.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38003062

RESUMEN

Over the past 20 years, the scientific community has witnessed a growing interest in the comparative study of mental capabilities [...].

18.
Animals (Basel) ; 13(14)2023 Jul 17.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37508103

RESUMEN

Spiders possess a unique visual system, split into eight different eyes and divided into two fully independent visual pathways. This peculiar organization begs the question of how visual information is processed, and whether the classically recognized Gestalt rules of perception hold true. In a previous experiment, we tested the ability of jumping spiders to associate a geometrical shape with a reward (sucrose solution), and then to generalize the learned association to a partially occluded version of the shape. The occluded shape was presented together with a broken version of the same shape. The former should be perceived as a whole shape only in the case the animals, like humans, are able to amodally complete an object partly hidden by an occluder; otherwise, the two shapes would be perceived as identical. There, the spiders learned the association but failed to generalize. Here, we present a replication of the experiment, with an increased number of subjects, a DeepLabCut-based scoring procedure, and an improved statistical analysis. The results of the experiment follow closely the direction of the effects observed in the previous work but fail to rise to significance. We discuss the importance of study replication, and we especially highlight the use of automated scoring procedures to maximize objectivity in behavioral studies.

19.
Poult Sci ; 102(12): 103148, 2023 Dec.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37890387

RESUMEN

Domestic chickens (Gallus gallus) are among those species subject to intensive selection for production. Among the most widely used broiler strains are the Ross308 and the Hybro. From the perspective of animal production, Ross308 were superior to Hybro in weight gain, final body mass, and feed conversion. Intensive selection is thought to also cause behavioral changes and to negatively affect cognitive abilities. Up to date, though, no evidence has been provided on broiler breeds. The aim of this study was to explore cognitive differences among Hybro and Ross308 chickens by assessing their ordinal-numerical abilities. Chicks learned learnt to find a food reward in the 4th container in a series of 10 identical and sagittally aligned containers. We designed a standard training procedure ensuring that all chicks received the same amount of training. The chicks underwent 2 tests: a sagittal and a fronto-parallel one. In the former test, the series was identical to that experienced during training. In the fronto-parallel test, the series was rotated by 90°, thus left-to-right oriented, to assess the capability of transferring the learnt rule with a novel spatial orientation. In the sagittal test, both chicken hybrids selected the 4th item above chance; interestingly the Hybro outperformed the Ross308 chicks. In the fronto-parallel test, both strains selected the 4th left and the 4th right container above chance; nevertheless, the Hybro chicks were more accurate. Our results support the hypothesis that intense selection for production can influence animal cognition and behavior, with implications on animal husbandry and welfare.


Asunto(s)
Pollos , Percepción Espacial , Animales , Cognición , Aprendizaje , Crianza de Animales Domésticos
20.
Proc Biol Sci ; 279(1730): 841-6, 2012 Mar 07.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21920978

RESUMEN

Bilateral symmetry is visually salient to diverse animals including birds, but whereas experimental studies typically use bilaterally symmetrical two-dimensional patterns that are viewed approximately fronto-parallel; in nature, animals observe three-dimensional objects from all angles. Many animals and plant structures have a plane of bilateral symmetry. Here, we first (experiment I) give evidence that young poultry chicks readily generalize bilateral symmetry as a feature of two-dimensional patterns in fronto-parallel view. We then test the ability of chicks to recognize symmetry in images that would be produced by the transformed view produced by a 40° horizontal combined with a 20° vertical rotation of a pattern on a spherical surface. Experiment II gives evidence that chicks trained to distinguish symmetrical from asymmetrical patterns treat rotated views of symmetrical 'objects' as symmetrical. Experiment III gives evidence that chicks trained to discriminate rotated views of symmetrical 'objects' from asymmetrical patterns generalize to novel symmetrical objects either in fronto-parallel or rotated view. These findings emphasize the importance of bilateral symmetry for three-dimensional object recognition and raise questions about the underlying mechanisms of symmetry perception.


Asunto(s)
Pollos/fisiología , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos , Animales , Masculino , Estimulación Luminosa
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
Detalles de la búsqueda