Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 10 de 10
Filtrar
1.
Sci Eng Ethics ; 21(5): 1085-93, 2015 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25256408

RESUMEN

Teaching of responsible conduct of research is largely predicated on the assumption that there are accepted standards of conduct that can be taught. However there is little evidence of consensus in the scientific community about such standards, at least for the practices of authorship, collaboration, and data management. To assess whether such differences in standards are based on disciplinary differences, a survey, described previously, addressing standards, practices, and perceptions about teaching and learning was distributed in November 2010 to US faculty from 50 graduate programs for the biomedical disciplines of microbiology, neuroscience, nursing, and psychology. Despite evidence of statistically significant differences across the four disciplines, actual differences were quite small. Stricter measures of effect size indicated practically significant disciplinary differences for fewer than 10% of the questions. This suggests that the variation in individual standards of practice within each discipline is at least as great as variation due to differences among disciplines. Therefore, the need for discipline-specific training may not be as important as sometimes thought.


Asunto(s)
Investigación Biomédica/ética , Consenso , Ética en Investigación/educación , Microbiología/ética , Neurociencias/ética , Psicología/ética , Autoria , Investigación Biomédica/educación , Conducta Cooperativa , Humanos , Microbiología/educación , Neurociencias/educación , Investigación en Enfermería/educación , Investigación en Enfermería/ética , Psicología/educación , Encuestas y Cuestionarios , Estados Unidos
2.
Nurs Outlook ; 63(2): 171-80, 2015.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25771191

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Team science is advocated to speed the pace of scientific discovery, yet the goals of collaborative practice in nursing science and the responsibilities of nurse stakeholders are sparse and inconclusive. The purpose of this study was to examine nurse scientists' views on collaborative research as part of a larger study on standards of scientific conduct. METHODS: Web-based descriptive survey of nurse scientists randomly selected from 50 doctoral graduate programs in the United States. RESULTS: Nearly forty percent of nurse respondents were not able to identify good collaborative practices for the discipline; more than three quarters did not know of any published guidelines available to them. Successful research collaborations were challenged by different expectations of authorship and data ownership, lack of timeliness and communication, poorly defined roles and responsibilities, language barriers, and when they involve junior and senior faculty working together on a project. CONCLUSION: Individual and organizational standards, practices, and policies for collaborative research needs clarification within the discipline.


Asunto(s)
Conducta Cooperativa , Educación de Postgrado en Enfermería , Investigación en Enfermería/organización & administración , Actitud del Personal de Salud , Códigos de Ética , Femenino , Humanos , Relaciones Interprofesionales , Masculino , Profesionalismo , Encuestas y Cuestionarios
3.
Sci Eng Ethics ; 20(4): 885-96, 2014 Dec.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24337932

RESUMEN

The practice of research is full of ethical challenges, many of which might be addressed through the teaching of responsible conduct of research (RCR). Although such training is increasingly required, there is no clear consensus about either the goals or content of an RCR curriculum. The present study was designed to assess community standards in three domains of research practice: authorship, collaboration, and data management. A survey, developed through advice from content matter experts, focus groups, and interviews, was distributed in November 2010 to U.S. faculty from 50 graduate programs for each of four different disciplines: microbiology, neuroscience, nursing, and psychology. The survey addressed practices and perceived standards, as well as perceptions about teaching and learning. Over 1,300 responses (response rate of 21 %) yielded statistically significant differences in responses to nearly all questions. However the magnitude of these differences was typically small, leaving little reason to argue for community consensus on standards. For nearly all questions asked, the clear finding was that there was nothing approaching consensus. These results may be useful not so much to teach what the standards are, but to increase student awareness of the diversity of those standards in reported practice.


Asunto(s)
Autoria , Conducta Cooperativa , Curriculum/normas , Educación de Postgrado/normas , Ética en Investigación/educación , Proyectos de Investigación , Ciencia/ética , Recolección de Datos , Educación de Postgrado en Enfermería , Docentes , Grupos Focales , Humanos , Entrevistas como Asunto , Aprendizaje , Microbiología/educación , Neurociencias/educación , Psicología/educación , Características de la Residencia , Ciencia/educación , Ciencia/normas , Encuestas y Cuestionarios , Enseñanza , Estados Unidos
4.
Infant Child Dev ; 19(4): 422-442, 2010 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20953286

RESUMEN

This study explored the effects of collectivism on lying to conceal a group transgression. Seven-, 9-, and 11-year-old US and Chinese children (N = 374) were asked to evaluate stories in which protagonists either lied or told the truth about their group's transgression and were then asked about either the protagonist's motivations or justification for their own evaluations. Previous research suggests that children in collectivist societies such as China find lying for one's group to be more acceptable than do children from individualistic societies such as the United States. The current study provides evidence that this is not always the case: Chinese children in this study viewed lies told to conceal a group's transgressions less favourably than did US children. An examination of children's reasoning about protagonists' motivations for lying indicated that children in both countries focused on an impact to self when discussing motivations for protagonists to lie for their group. Overall, results suggest that children living in collectivist societies do not always focus on the needs of the group.

5.
Br J Dev Psychol ; 27(Pt 4): 875-90, 2009 Nov.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19994484

RESUMEN

Children's reasoning about the willingness of peers to convey accurate positive and negative performance feedback to others was investigated among a total of 179 6- to 11-year-olds from the USA and China. In Study 1, which was conducted in the USA only, participants responded that peers would be more likely to provide positive feedback than negative feedback, and this tendency was strongest among the younger children. In Study 2, the expectation that peers would preferentially disclose positive feedback was replicated among children from the USA, and was also seen among younger but not older children from China. Participants in all groups took the relationship between communication partners into account when predicting whether peers would express evaluative feedback. Results of open-ended responses suggested cross-cultural differences, including a greater emphasis by Chinese children on the implications of evaluative feedback for future performance, and reference by some older Chinese children to the possibility that positive feedback might make the recipient 'too proud'.


Asunto(s)
Comparación Transcultural , Cultura , Retroalimentación Psicológica , Grupo Paritario , Afecto , Factores de Edad , Concienciación , Niño , China , Femenino , Humanos , Juicio , Masculino , Refuerzo en Psicología , Revelación de la Verdad , Estados Unidos
6.
Appl Psycholinguist ; 34(6): 1219-1243, 2013 Nov 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24489415

RESUMEN

We investigated the factors that may help understand the differential rates of language development in the home language (i.e., Spanish) of Latino preschoolers with specific language impairment (SLI). Children were randomly assigned to either bilingual or English-only small group interventions and followed from preschool to kindergarten. Predictors of Spanish growth included the language of intervention, the child's level of language development or severity, the child's socio-emotional skills, and the child's level of English use. Spanish performance outcomes were assessed over time using a series of longitudinal models with baseline and post-treatment measures nested within child. Children demonstrated growth on Spanish outcomes over time. The language of instruction and the child's level of vocabulary and socio-emotional development at baseline were significant predictors of differences in rates of growth in the home language. Clinicians may need to take into consideration these factors when making clinical recommendations.

7.
Am J Speech Lang Pathol ; 21(1): 64-77, 2012 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22230174

RESUMEN

PURPOSE: This study evaluated the extent to which the language of intervention, the child's development in Spanish, and the effects of English vocabulary, use, proficiency, and exposure predict differences in the rates of acquisition of English in Latino children with specific language impairment (SLI). METHOD: In this randomized controlled trial, 188 Latino preschoolers with SLI participated in a small-group academic enrichment program for 12 weeks and were followed up 3 and 5 months later. Children were randomly assigned to either a bilingual or an English-only program. Predictors of English growth included measures of Spanish language skills and English vocabulary, use, proficiency, and exposure. Performance on English outcomes (i.e., picture description and narrative sample) was assessed over time. A series of longitudinal models were tested via multilevel modeling with baseline and posttreatment measures nested within child. RESULTS: Children demonstrated growth on the English outcomes over time. The language of intervention, Spanish skills, English vocabulary, and English use significantly predicted differences in rates of growth across children for specific measures of English development. CONCLUSIONS: This study underscores the role of the child's first language skills, the child's level of English vocabulary development, and level of English use for predicting differences in English acquisition in Latino preschoolers with SLI. These factors should be carefully considered in making clinical decisions.


Asunto(s)
Lenguaje Infantil , Trastornos del Desarrollo del Lenguaje/diagnóstico , Trastornos del Desarrollo del Lenguaje/rehabilitación , Desarrollo del Lenguaje , Multilingüismo , Escuelas de Párvulos/organización & administración , Preescolar , Femenino , Hispánicos o Latinos , Humanos , Lenguaje , Trastornos del Desarrollo del Lenguaje/etnología , Pruebas del Lenguaje , Masculino , Valor Predictivo de las Pruebas , Evaluación de Programas y Proyectos de Salud , Vocabulario
8.
Dev Psychol ; 46(3): 669-80, 2010 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20438178

RESUMEN

Event-related potentials (ERPs) were recorded from 6-month-olds (N = 30) as they looked at pictures of their mother's face and a stranger's face. Negative component (Nc) and P400 component responses from the ERP portion of the study were correlated with behavioral responses of the infants during a separation from their mothers. We measured the mother-directed infant behaviors of distress and visual search for mother during separation in order to determine if they were predictive of infants' brain responses to pictures of the mother's face versus a stranger's face. These behavioral measures are important because they likely reflect the functioning of the emerging mother-child relationship and inform debates about interactions between social experience and face processing. Infant distress and visual search for mother during separation were predictive of face processing ERPs, and this relationship differed across mother and stranger face presentations. In particular, distress was associated with larger amplitude P400 and Nc responses to the mother's face, and visual search for mother was associated with longer P400 and Nc latencies to the stranger's face. Implications for the developing mother-child relationship and face processing system are discussed.


Asunto(s)
Corteza Cerebral/fisiología , Conducta del Lactante , Privación Materna , Relaciones Madre-Hijo , Reconocimiento en Psicología/fisiología , Atención/fisiología , Mapeo Encefálico , Electroencefalografía , Potenciales Evocados/fisiología , Cara , Humanos , Lactante , Madres , Apego a Objetos , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología , Estimulación Luminosa , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Procesamiento de Señales Asistido por Computador
9.
Soc Dev ; 18(3): 728-746, 2009 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21072132

RESUMEN

Children's reasoning about lying and truth-telling was examined among participants ages 7-11 (total N = 181) with reference to conflicts between being honest and protecting the feelings of others. In Study 1, participants showed different patterns of evaluation and motivational inference in politeness contexts vs. transgression contexts: in politeness contexts, they rated lie-telling more favorably and were far more likely to assume that motives were prosocial. In Study 2, participants evaluated lie-telling more positively and truth-telling more negatively in politeness contexts, especially when they focused on the implications of the statements for others instead of whether the statements were true or false.

10.
Child Dev ; 75(5): 1435-56, 2004.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15369524

RESUMEN

Home visiting programs for families with young children have been in effect for many years; however, this is the first comprehensive meta-analytic effort to quantify the usefulness of home visits as a strategy for helping families across a range of outcomes. Sixty home visiting programs contributed data to analysis within 5 child and 5 parent outcome groups. Standardized effect sizes were computed for each end-of-treatment outcome measure, for each treatment versus control contrast. Weighted mean standardized effect sizes ranged from -.043 to.318; 6 of the 10 significantly differed from 0. No one program characteristic consistently affected effect sizes across outcome groups. The extent to which these findings have practical use for the field is discussed.


Asunto(s)
Familia , Servicios de Salud/provisión & distribución , Visita Domiciliaria , Desarrollo de Programa , Niño , Maltrato a los Niños/prevención & control , Desarrollo Infantil , Preescolar , Servicios de Salud/estadística & datos numéricos , Humanos , Lactante , Recién Nacido , Atención Prenatal , Relaciones Profesional-Familia , Factores de Riesgo , Población Urbana/estadística & datos numéricos
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
Detalles de la búsqueda