Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 1.252
Filtrar
1.
Perception ; 53(8): 544-562, 2024 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38826086

RESUMEN

The way that attention affects the processing of visual information is one of the most intriguing fields in the study of visual perception. One way to examine this interaction is by studying the way perceptual aftereffects are modulated by attention. In the present study, we have manipulated attention during adaptation to translational motion generated by coherently moving random dots, in order to investigate the effect of the distraction of attention on the strength of the peripheral dynamic motion aftereffect (MAE). A foveal rapid serial visual presentation task (RSVP) of varying difficulty was introduced during the adaptation period while the adaptation and test stimuli were presented peripherally. Furthermore, to examine the interaction between the physical characteristics of the stimulus and attention, we have manipulated the motion coherence level of the adaptation stimuli. Our results suggested that the removal of attention through an irrelevant task modulated the MAE's magnitude moderately and that such an effect depends on the stimulus strength. We also showed that the MAE still persists with subthreshold and unattended stimuli, suggesting that perhaps attention is not required for the complete development of the MAE.


Asunto(s)
Atención , Efecto Tardío Figurativo , Percepción de Movimiento , Humanos , Atención/fisiología , Percepción de Movimiento/fisiología , Efecto Tardío Figurativo/fisiología , Adulto , Adulto Joven , Femenino , Masculino , Adaptación Fisiológica/fisiología
2.
J Vis ; 24(7): 4, 2024 Jul 02.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38975947

RESUMEN

To dissociate aftereffects of size and density in the perception of relative numerosity, large or small adapter sizes were crossed with high or low adapter densities. A total of 48 participants were included in this preregistered design. To adapt the same retinotopic region as the large adapters, the small adapters were flashed in a sequence so as to "paint" the adapting density across the large region. Perceived numerosities and sizes in the adapted region were then compared to those in an unadapted region in separate blocks of trials, so that changes in density could be inferred. These density changes were found to be bidirectional and roughly symmetric, whereas the aftereffects of size and number were not symmetric. A simple account of these findings is that local adaptations to retinotopic density as well as global adaptations to size combine in producing numerosity aftereffects measured by assessing perceived relative number. Accounts based on number adaptation are contraindicated, in particular, by the result of adapting to a large, sparse adapter and testing with a stimulus with a double the density but half number of dots.


Asunto(s)
Adaptación Fisiológica , Estimulación Luminosa , Percepción del Tamaño , Humanos , Percepción del Tamaño/fisiología , Femenino , Adulto , Masculino , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Adaptación Fisiológica/fisiología , Adulto Joven , Efecto Tardío Figurativo/fisiología , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología
3.
Perception ; 52(5): 297-311, 2023 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37128745

RESUMEN

Visual adaptation occurs after a prolonged exposure to a stimulus. The duration of aftereffects differs across stimuli type, and face aftereffects may be especially long lasting. The current study investigates adaptation decay of category contingent opposing aftereffects. Specifically, we tested whether naïve undergraduate participants' adaptation to photos of faces with explicit religious labels, differed from that of participants who had adapted to the same faces 7 days previously. We also tested whether 7-day old category-contingent opposing aftereffects interfere with the ability to re-adapt to a new condition. In Session 1, undergraduates made attractiveness preference selections before and after adapting to two groups of distorted faces. Participants then returned 7 days later to re-assess the attractiveness of the same faces. Participants were then adapted to the two groups of faces distorted in the opposite direction. Adaptation strength was stronger in Session 1 than in Session 2, although adaptation strength was not related to pre-adaptation selections. Week-old aftereffects interfered with the creation of aftereffects in the opposite direction 7 days later.


Asunto(s)
Efecto Tardío Figurativo , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos , Humanos , Cara , Adaptación Fisiológica , Estudiantes
4.
Perception ; 52(1): 5-20, 2023 Jan.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36384361

RESUMEN

Opposing aftereffects have been observed for faces categorized by gender, race, and age. In order to form opposing aftereffects, it appears that the two face sets must be both physically distinct and differ in terms of social meaning. The current study tests whether (1) a face set that is diverse with respect to sex and race can produce a coherent aftereffect and (2) whether this diversity itself is socially meaningful enough to support opposing aftereffects. Participants adapted to a homogenous face set consisting of only White male Republican congressmen and a diverse face set consisting of White, Asian, Black, and Latino male and female Democratic congress members. Opposing aftereffects were observed: participants adapted simultaneously and in opposite directions to the face sets. These results are the first evidence of adaptation to a face set that varies based on race and sex, and the first evidence of diversity being perceived as a socially meaningful category marker.


Asunto(s)
Efecto Tardío Figurativo , Humanos , Femenino , Blanco , Cara , Adaptación Fisiológica , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos
5.
Psychol Res ; 86(4): 1184-1202, 2022 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34387745

RESUMEN

Protracted exposure to specific stimuli causes biased visual aftereffects at both low- and high-level dimensions of a stimulus. Recently, it has been proposed that alterations of these aftereffects could play a role in body misperceptions. However, since previous studies have mainly addressed manipulations of body size, the relative contribution of low-level retinotopic and/or high-level object-based mechanisms is yet to be understood. In three experiments, we investigated visual aftereffects for body-gender perception, testing for the tuning of visual aftereffects across different characters and orientation. We found that exposure to a distinctively female (or male) body makes androgynous bodies appear as more masculine (or feminine) and that these aftereffects were not specific for the individual characteristics of the adapting body (Exp.1). Furthermore, exposure to only upright bodies (Exp.2) biased the perception of upright, but not of inverted bodies, while exposure to both upright and inverted bodies (Exp.3) biased perception for both. Finally, participants' sensitivity to body aftereffects was lower in individuals with greater communication deficits and deeper internalization of a male gender role. Overall, our data reveals the orientation-, but not identity-tuning of body-gender aftereffects and points to the association between alterations of the malleability of body gender perception and social deficits.


Asunto(s)
Efecto Tardío Figurativo , Adaptación Fisiológica , Tamaño Corporal , Cara , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Percepción Visual
6.
J Vis ; 22(11): 14, 2022 10 04.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36301525

RESUMEN

The appearance of a face can be strongly affected by adaptation to faces seen previously. A number of studies have examined the time course of these aftereffects, but the integration time over which adaptation pools signals to control the adaptation state remains uncertain. Here we examined the effects of temporal frequency on face gender aftereffects induced by a pair of faces alternating between the two genders to assess when the aftereffects were pooled over successive faces versus driven by the last face seen. In the first experiment, we found that temporal frequencies between 0.25 and 2.00 Hz all failed to produce an aftereffect, suggesting a fairly long integration time. In the second experiment, we therefore probed slower alternation rates of 0.03 to 0.25 Hz. A rate of 0.0625 Hz (i.e., 8 seconds per face) was required to generate significant aftereffects from the last presented face and was consistent with an average time constant of 15 to 20 seconds for an exponentially decaying integration window. This integration time is substantially longer than found previously for analogous effects for alternating colors, and thus points to a potentially slower mechanism of adaptation for faces compared with chromatic adaptation.


Asunto(s)
Efecto Tardío Figurativo , Humanos , Femenino , Masculino , Cara , Adaptación Fisiológica , Estimulación Luminosa , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos
7.
Cogn Emot ; 36(2): 240-253, 2022 03.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34775905

RESUMEN

The adaptation aftereffect plays a critical role in human development and survival. Existing studies have found that, compared with general individuals, individuals with learning disability, autism and dyslexia show a smaller amount of non-affective-based cognitive adaptation aftereffect. Nevertheless, it is unclear whether individuals with depression or depression tendency show similar phenomenon in the adaptation aftereffect, and whether such depression tendency occurs in the non-affective-based cognitive or emotional adaptation aftereffect. To address this question, the present study conducted two experiments. Experiments 1A and 1B used the emotional facial expression adaptation paradigm to examine whether Chinese participants showed the emotional adaptation aftereffect and whether the emotional adaptation aftereffect was influenced by physical features of faces, respectively. Experiment 2 recruited two groups of participants, with high and low depression, respectively, to examine whether they showed differences in the emotional or cognitive adaptation aftereffect. Results showed that Chinese participants showed the typical emotional adaptation aftereffect, which was not influenced by physical features of faces. More importantly, compared to the low-depression group, the high-depression group showed a smaller emotional adaptation aftereffect, but the two groups showed a similar cognitive adaptation aftereffect. These results suggest that level of depressive symptoms is associated with the emotional adaptation aftereffect.


Asunto(s)
Efecto Tardío Figurativo , Depresión , Ajuste Emocional , Emociones , Expresión Facial , Humanos
8.
J Vis ; 21(11): 11, 2021 10 05.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34668931

RESUMEN

The ability to estimate spatial extent is an important feature of the visual system. A previous study showed that perceived sizes of stimuli shrank after adaptation to a dense texture and that this density-size aftereffect was modulated by the degree of density. In this study, we found that the aftereffect was also modulated by the temporal density of the adapting texture. The test stimuli were two circles, and the adapting stimulus had a dotted texture. The adapting texture refreshed every 67 to 500 ms, or not at all (static), during the adaptation. The results showed that the aftereffects from a refreshing stimulus were larger than those under the static condition. On the other hand, density adaptation lacked such enhancement. This result indicates that repetitive presentation of an adapting texture enhanced the density-size cross-aftereffect. The fact that density modulation occurs in both the spatial and temporal domains is consistent with the theory of magnitude, which assumes that the processing of the magnitude estimation of space, time, and numbers share a common cortical basis.


Asunto(s)
Efecto Tardío Figurativo , Percepción de Movimiento , Adaptación Fisiológica , Humanos , Percepción del Tamaño
9.
J Vis ; 21(5): 17, 2021 05 03.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34007990

RESUMEN

Sensory adaptation is a useful tool to identify the links between perceptual effects and neural mechanisms. Even though motion adaptation is one of the earliest and most documented aftereffects, few studies have investigated the perception of direction and speed of the aftereffect at the same time, that is the perceived velocity. Using a novel experimental paradigm, we simultaneously recorded the perceived direction and speed of leftward or rightward moving random dots before and after adaptation. For the adapting stimulus, we chose a horizontally-oriented broadband grating moving upward behind a circular aperture. Because of the aperture problem, the interpretation of this stimulus is ambiguous, being consistent with multiple velocities, and yet it is systematically perceived as moving at a single direction and speed. Here we ask whether the visual system adapts to the multiple velocities of the adaptor or to just the single perceived velocity. Our results show a strong repulsion aftereffect, away from the adapting velocity (downward and slower), that increases gradually for faster test stimuli as long as these stimuli include some velocities that match some of the ambiguous ones of the adaptor. In summary, the visual system seems to adapt to the multiple velocities of an ambiguous stimulus even though a single velocity is perceived. Our findings can be well described by a computational model that assumes a joint encoding of direction and speed and that includes an extended adaptation component that can represent all the possible velocities of the ambiguous stimulus.


Asunto(s)
Efecto Tardío Figurativo , Percepción de Movimiento , Adaptación Ocular , Adaptación Fisiológica , Humanos , Movimiento (Física) , Programas Informáticos
10.
Psychol Sci ; 31(11): 1470-1474, 2020 11.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33079641

RESUMEN

Rapidly judging the number of objects in a scene is an important perceptual ability. Recent debates have centered on whether number perception is accomplished by dedicated mechanisms and, in particular, on whether number-adaptation aftereffects reflect adaptation of number per se or adaptation of related stimulus properties, such as density. Here, we report an adaptation experiment (N = 8) for which the predictions of number and density theories are diametrically opposed. We found that when a reference stimulus has higher density than an adaptation stimulus but contains fewer elements, adaptation reduces the perceived number of elements in the reference stimulus. This is consistent with number adaptation and inconsistent with density adaptation. Thus, number-adaptation aftereffects are more than a by-product of density adaptation: When density and number are dissociated, adaptation effects are in the direction predicted by adaptation to number, not density.


Asunto(s)
Efecto Tardío Figurativo , Adaptación Fisiológica , Humanos , Estimulación Luminosa , Psicofísica
11.
Psychol Sci ; 31(6): 654-662, 2020 06.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32348188

RESUMEN

The visual system adapts to the environment, changing neural responses to aid efficiency and improve perception. However, these changes sometimes lead to negative consequences: If neurons at later processing stages fail to account for adaptation at earlier stages, perceptual errors result, including common visual illusions. These negative effects of adaptation have been termed the coding catastrophe. How does the visual system resolve them? We hypothesized that higher-level adaptation can correct errors arising from the coding catastrophe by changing what appears normal, a common form of adaptation across domains. Observers (N = 15) viewed flickering checkerboards that caused a normal face to appear distorted. We tested whether the visual system can adapt to this adaptation-distorted face through repeated viewing. Results from two experiments show that such meta-adaptation does occur and that it makes the distorted face gradually appear more normal. Meta-adaptation may be a general strategy to correct negative consequences of low-level adaptation.


Asunto(s)
Percepción Visual , Adaptación Ocular , Adaptación Psicológica , Cara , Efecto Tardío Figurativo , Humanos , Aprendizaje , Variaciones Dependientes del Observador , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos , Estimulación Luminosa , Vías Visuales/fisiología
12.
Perception ; 49(8): 835-857, 2020 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32605425

RESUMEN

The ability to process information despite the lack of perceptual awareness is one of the most fascinating aspects of the visual system. Such unconscious processing is often investigated using adaptation, where any presence of the former can be traced by its footprint on aftereffects following the latter. We have investigated the mechanisms of the motion aftereffect (MAE) using random dot displays of varying motion coherence as well as crowding to modulate both the physical as well as the perceptual strength of the adaptation stimulus. Perceptual strength was quantitatively measured as the performance in a forced-choice direction-discrimination task. A motion-nulling technique was used to quantitatively measure the strength of the MAE. We show that the strength of the dynamic MAE is independently influenced by both the physical stimulus strength as well as the subjective perceptual strength, with the effect of the former being more prominent than that of the latter. We further show that the MAE still persists under conditions of subthreshold perception. Our results suggest that perceptual awareness can influence the strength of visual processing, but the latter is not fully dependent on the former and can still take place at its partial or even total absence.


Asunto(s)
Concienciación/fisiología , Efecto Tardío Figurativo/fisiología , Percepción de Movimiento/fisiología , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología , Percepción Espacial/fisiología , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Campos Visuales/fisiología , Adulto Joven
13.
Psychol Res ; 84(4): 866-880, 2020 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30406829

RESUMEN

The human nervous system displays such plasticity that we can adapt our motor behavior to various changes in environmental or body properties. However, how sensorimotor adaptation generalizes to new situations and new effectors, and which factors influence the underlying mechanisms, remains unclear. Here we tested the general hypothesis that differences across participants can be exploited to uncover what drives interlimb transfer. Twenty healthy adults adapted to prismatic glasses while reaching to visual targets with their dominant arm. Classic adaptation and generalization across movement directions were observed but transfer to the non-dominant arm was not significant and inter-individual differences were substantial. Interlimb transfer resulted for some participants in a directional shift of non-dominant arm movements that was consistent with an encoding of visuomotor adaptation in extrinsic coordinates. For some other participants, transfer was consistent with an intrinsic coordinate system. Simple and multiple regression analyses showed that a few kinematic parameters such as peak acceleration (or peak velocity) and variability of movement direction were correlated with interlimb transfer. Low peak acceleration and low variability were related to extrinsic transfer, while high peak acceleration and high variability were related to intrinsic transfer. Motor variability was also positively correlated with the magnitude of the after-effect systematically observed on the dominant arm. Overall, these findings on unconstrained movements support the idea that individual movement features could be linked to the sensorimotor adaptation and its generalization. The study also suggests that distinct movement characteristics may be related to different coordinate frames of action representations in the nervous system.


Asunto(s)
Adaptación Fisiológica/fisiología , Efecto Tardío Figurativo/fisiología , Movimiento/fisiología , Transferencia de Experiencia en Psicología/fisiología , Fenómenos Biomecánicos/fisiología , Femenino , Humanos , Individualidad , Masculino , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Adulto Joven
14.
J Vis ; 20(13): 10, 2020 12 02.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33325995

RESUMEN

Spatially varying distortions (SVDs) are common artifacts of spectacles like progressive additional lenses (PALs). To habituate to distortions of PALs, the visual system has to adapt to distortion-induced image alterations, termed skew adaptation. But how this visual adjustment is achieved is largely unknown. This study examines the properties of visual adaptation to distortions of PALs in natural scenes. The visual adaptation in response to altered form and motion features of the natural stimuli were probed in two different psychophysical experiments. Observers were exposed to distortions in natural images, and form and motion aftereffects were tested subsequently in a constant stimuli procedure where subjects were asked to judge the skew, or the motion direction of an according test stimulus. Exposure to skewed natural stimuli induced a shift in perceived undistorted form as well as motion direction, when viewing distorted dynamic natural scenes, and also after exposure to static distorted natural images. Therefore, skew adaptation occurred in form and motion for dynamic visual scenes as well as static images. Thus, specifically in the condition of static skewed images and the test feature of motion direction, cortical interactions between motion-form processing presumably contributed to the adaptation process. In a nutshell, interfeature cortical interactions constituted the adaptation process to distortion of PALs. Thus, comprehensive investigation of adaptation to distortions of PALs would benefit from taking into account content richness of the stimuli to be used, like natural images.


Asunto(s)
Adaptación Fisiológica/fisiología , Efecto Tardío Figurativo/fisiología , Percepción de Forma/fisiología , Percepción de Movimiento/fisiología , Adulto , Humanos , Masculino , Psicofísica , Adulto Joven
15.
J Neurosci ; 38(5): 1189-1201, 2018 01 31.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29255004

RESUMEN

Prismatic adaption (PA) has been proposed as a tool to induce neural plasticity and is used to help neglect rehabilitation. It leads to a recalibration of visuomotor coordination during pointing as well as to aftereffects on a number of sensorimotor and attention tasks, but whether these effects originate at a motor or attentional level remains a matter of debate. Our aim was to further characterize PA aftereffects by using an approach that allows distinguishing between effects on attentional and motor processes. We recorded EEG in healthy human participants (9 females and 7 males) while performing a new double step, anticipatory attention/motor preparation paradigm before and after adaptation to rightward-shifting prisms, with neutral lenses as a control. We then examined PA aftereffects through changes in known oscillatory EEG signatures of spatial attention orienting and motor preparation in the alpha and beta frequency bands. Our results were twofold. First, we found PA to rightward-shifting prisms to selectively affect EEG signatures of motor but not attentional processes. More specifically, PA modulated preparatory motor EEG activity over central electrodes in the right hemisphere, contralateral to the PA-induced, compensatory leftward shift in pointing movements. No effects were found on EEG signatures of spatial attention orienting over occipitoparietal sites. Second, we found the PA effect on preparatory motor EEG activity to dominate in the beta frequency band. We conclude that changes to intentional visuomotor, rather than attentional visuospatial, processes underlie the PA aftereffect of rightward-deviating prisms in healthy participants.SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Prismatic adaptation (PA) has been proposed as a tool to induce neural plasticity in both healthy participants and patients, due to its aftereffect impacting on a number of visuospatial and visuomotor functions. However, the neural mechanisms underlying PA aftereffects are poorly understood as only little neuroimaging evidence is available. Here, we examined, for the first time, the origin of PA aftereffects studying oscillatory brain activity. Our results show a selective modulation of preparatory motor activity following PA in healthy participants but no effect on attention-related activity. This provides novel insight into the PA aftereffect in the healthy brain and may help to inform interventions in neglect patients.


Asunto(s)
Adaptación Psicológica/fisiología , Anticipación Psicológica/fisiología , Atención/fisiología , Electroencefalografía , Adulto , Ritmo alfa/fisiología , Ritmo beta/fisiología , Potenciales Evocados Visuales/fisiología , Femenino , Efecto Tardío Figurativo , Lateralidad Funcional/fisiología , Voluntarios Sanos , Humanos , Masculino , Orientación/fisiología , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Percepción Espacial/fisiología , Adulto Joven
16.
Perception ; 48(4): 286-315, 2019 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30885042

RESUMEN

Glass patterns (GPs) consist of randomly distributed dot pairs (dipoles) whose orientations are determined by specific geometric transforms. We investigated the role of visuospatial attention in the processing of global form from GPs by measuring the effect of distraction on adaptation to GPs. In the nondistracted condition, observers were adapted to coherent GPs. After the adaptation period, they were presented with a test GP divided in two halves along the vertical and were required to judge which side of the test GP was more coherent. In the attention-distracted condition, a high-load rapid serial visual presentation task was performed during the adapting period. The magnitude of the form after-effect was measured using a technique that measures the coherence level at which the test GP appears random. The rationale was that if attention has a modulatory effect on the spatial summation of dipoles, in the attention-distracted condition, we should expect a weaker form after-effect. However, the results showed stronger form after-effect in the attention-distracted condition than in the nondistracted condition, suggesting that distraction during adaptation increases the strength of form adaptation. Additional experiments suggested that distraction may reduce the spatial suppression from large-scale textures, strengthening the spatial summation of local-oriented signals.


Asunto(s)
Atención/fisiología , Efecto Tardío Figurativo/fisiología , Percepción de Forma/fisiología , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología , Adulto , Humanos , Adulto Joven
17.
J Vis ; 19(12): 4, 2019 10 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31613953

RESUMEN

Studies have revealed that textures suppress the processing of the shapes of contours they surround. One manifestation of texture-surround suppression is the reduction in the magnitude of adaptation-induced contour-shape aftereffects when the adaptor contour is surrounded by a texture. Here we utilize this phenomenon to investigate the nature of the first-order inputs to texture-surround suppression of contour shape by examining its selectivity to luminance polarity and the magnitude of luminance contrast. Stimuli were constructed from sinusoidal-shaped strings of either "bright" or "dark" elongated Gaussians. Observers adapted to pairs of contours, and the aftereffect was measured as the shift in the apparent shape frequency of subsequently presented test contours. We found that the suppression of the contour-shape aftereffect by a surround texture made of similar contours was maximal when the adaptor's center and surround contours were of the same polarity, revealing polarity specificity of the surround-suppression effect. We also measured the effect of varying the relative contrasts of the adaptor's center and surround and found that the reduction in the contour-shape aftereffect was determined by the surround-to-center contrast ratio. Finally, we measured the selectivity to luminance polarity of the texture-shape aftereffect itself and found that it was reduced when the adaptors and tests were of opposite luminance polarity. We conclude that texture-surround suppression of contour-shape as well as texture-shape processing itself depend on "on-off" luminance-polarity channel interactions. These selectivities may constitute an important neural substrate underlying efficient figure-ground segregation and image segmentation.


Asunto(s)
Sensibilidad de Contraste/fisiología , Percepción de Forma/fisiología , Luz , Adaptación Fisiológica , Efecto Tardío Figurativo , Humanos , Distribución Normal
18.
J Vis ; 19(14): 21, 2019 12 02.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31868893

RESUMEN

Facial expressions are used as critical social cues in everyday life. Adaptation to expressions causes expression aftereffects. These aftereffects are thought to reflect the operation of face-selective neural mechanisms, and are used by researchers to investigate the nature of those mechanisms. However, recent evidence suggests that expression aftereffects could be at least partially explained by the inheritance of lower-level tilt adaptation through the visual hierarchy. We investigated whether expression aftereffects could be entirely explained by tilt adaptation. Participants completed an expression adaptation task in which we controlled for the influence of tilt by changing the orientation of the adaptor relative to the test stimuli. Although tilt adaptation appeared to make some contribution to the expression aftereffect, robust expression aftereffects still remained after minimizing tilt inheritance, indicating that expression aftereffects cannot be fully explained by tilt adaptation. There was also significant reduction in the expression aftereffects after inverting the adapting face, providing evidence that face-selective processing is involved in these aftereffects.


Asunto(s)
Expresión Facial , Efecto Tardío Figurativo , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos , Adaptación Fisiológica , Adolescente , Adulto , Señales (Psicología) , Movimientos Oculares , Cara/fisiología , Femenino , Movimientos de la Cabeza , Humanos , Masculino , Orientación Espacial , Adulto Joven
19.
J Acoust Soc Am ; 146(2): EL177, 2019 08.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31472570

RESUMEN

Visual calibration of auditory space requires re-alignment of representations differing in (1) format (auditory hemispheric channels vs visual maps) and (2) reference frames (head-centered vs eye-centered). Here, a ventriloquism paradigm from Kopco, Lin, Shinn-Cunningham, and Groh [J. Neurosci. 29, 13809-13814 (2009)] was used to examine these processes in humans for ventriloquism induced within one spatial hemifield. Results show that (1) the auditory representation can be adapted even by aligned audio-visual stimuli, and (2) the spatial reference frame is primarily head-centered, with a weak eye-centered modulation. These results support the view that the ventriloquism aftereffect is driven by multiple spatially non-uniform, hemisphere-specific processes.


Asunto(s)
Efecto Tardío Figurativo , Lateralidad Funcional , Localización de Sonidos , Encéfalo/fisiología , Señales (Psicología) , Movimientos Oculares , Humanos , Ilusiones/fisiología , Percepción del Habla , Adulto Joven
20.
Sheng Li Xue Bao ; 71(1): 95-104, 2019 Feb 25.
Artículo en Zh | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30778508

RESUMEN

Recent sensory history plays a critical role in the perception of event duration. For example, repetitive exposure to a particular duration leads to the distortion of subsequent duration perception. This phenomenon, termed duration adaptation, induces a robust repulsive duration aftereffect. In particular, adaptation to relatively long sensory events shortens the perceived duration of a subsequent event, while adaptation to relatively short sensory events lengthens the perception of subsequent event durations. This phenomenon implies the plasticity of duration perception and offers important clues for revealing the cognitive neural mechanism of duration perception. Duration aftereffect has received more and more attention in recent years. In this review, we introduce recent research advances in our understanding of duration aftereffect, especially with regards to its manifestations, origin, and cognitive neural mechanisms. We also propose possible directions for future research. In sum, we posit that studies on the duration aftereffect phenomenon are helpful in understanding general duration perception, and as such, should receive more attention in future.


Asunto(s)
Efecto Tardío Figurativo , Percepción de Movimiento , Percepción Visual , Humanos
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
Detalles de la búsqueda