Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 10 de 10
Filtrar
1.
J Youth Adolesc ; 44(5): 1078-91, 2015 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25287000

RESUMO

Previous research suggests that adolescents' social network site use is related to their sexual development. However, the associations between adolescents' exposure to sexy self-presentations of others on social network sites and their sexual attitudes and experience have not yet been empirically supported. This study investigated reciprocal longitudinal relationships between adolescents' exposure to others' sexy self-presentations on social network sites and their sexual attitudes (i.e., sexual objectification of girls and instrumental attitudes towards sex) and sexual experience. We further tested whether these associations depended on adolescents' age and gender. Results from a representative two-wave panel study among 1,636 Dutch adolescents (aged 13-17, 51.5 % female) showed that exposure to sexy online self-presentations of others predicted changes in adolescents' experience with oral sex and intercourse 6 months later, but did not influence their sexual attitudes. Adolescents' instrumental attitudes towards sex, in turn, did predict their exposure to others' sexy online self-presentations. Sexual objectification increased such exposure for younger adolescents, but decreased exposure for older adolescents. In addition, adolescents' experience with genital touching as well as oral sex (only for adolescents aged 13-15) predicted their exposure to sexy self-presentations of others. These findings tentatively suggest that the influence on adolescents' sexual attitudes previously found for sexual media content may not hold for sexy self-presentations on social network sites. However, exposure to sexy self-presentations on social network sites is motivated by adolescents' sexual attitudes and behavior, especially among young adolescents.


Assuntos
Comportamento do Adolescente/psicologia , Imagem Corporal/psicologia , Internet , Comportamento Sexual/psicologia , Sexualidade/psicologia , Adolescente , Feminino , Humanos , Estudos Longitudinais , Masculino , Mídias Sociais
2.
Brain Cogn ; 78(1): 50-8, 2012 Feb.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22088775

RESUMO

Thinking about the abstract concept power may automatically activate the spatial up-down image schema (powerful up; powerless down) and consequently direct spatial attention to the image schema-congruent location. Participants indicated whether a word represented a powerful or powerless person (e.g. 'king' or 'servant'). Following each decision, they identified a target at the top or bottom of the visual field. In Experiment 1 participants identified the target faster when their spatial position was congruent with the perceived power of the preceding word than when it was incongruent. In Experiment 2 ERPs showed a higher N1 amplitude for congruent spatial positions. These results support the view that attention is driven to the image schema congruent location of a power word. Thus, power is partially understood in terms of vertical space, which demonstrates that abstract concepts are grounded in sensory-motor processing.


Assuntos
Atenção/fisiologia , Córtex Cerebral/fisiologia , Cognição/fisiologia , Formação de Conceito/fisiologia , Potenciais Evocados/fisiologia , Adolescente , Adulto , Eletroencefalografia , Feminino , Humanos , Imaginação/fisiologia , Masculino , Metáfora , Estimulação Luminosa , Tempo de Reação/fisiologia , Percepção Espacial/fisiologia , Vocabulário
3.
Clin Cancer Res ; 11(10): 3806-13, 2005 May 15.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15897580

RESUMO

PURPOSE: TZT-1027 [N(2)-(N,N-dimethyl-l-valyl)-N-[(1S,2R)-2-methoxy-4-[(2S)-2-[(1R,2R)-1-methoxy-2-methyl-3-oxo-3-[(2-phenylethyl)]amino]propyl]-1-pyrrolidinyl]-1-[(S)-1-methylpropyl]-4-oxobutyl]-N-methyl-l-valinamide] is a cytotoxic dolastatin 10 derivative inhibiting microtubule assembly through the binding to tubulins. The objectives of this phase I study was to assess the dose-limiting toxicities (DLT), to determine the maximum tolerated dose, and to study the pharmacokinetics of TZT-1027 when given i.v. over 60 minutes on days 1 and 8 every 3 weeks to patients with advanced solid tumors. EXPERIMENTAL DESIGN: Patients were treated with escalating doses of TZT-1027 at doses ranging from 1.35 to 2.7 mg/m(2). For pharmacokinetic analysis, plasma sampling was done during the first and second course and assayed using a validated high-performance liquid chromatographic assay with mass spectrometric detection. RESULTS: Seventeen patients received a total of >70 courses. The stopping dose was reached at 2.7 mg/m(2), with neutropenia and infusion arm pain as DLT. Neutropenia was not complicated by fever. Over all dose levels, eight patients experienced pain in the infusion arm 1 to 2 days after administration of the drug, which seemed ameliorated by adding additional flushing after drug administration. Other side effects included nausea, vomiting, diarrhea, and fatigue. One partial response lasting >54 weeks was observed in an extensively pretreated patient with metastatic liposarcoma. The pharmacokinetics of TZT-1027 suggested linearity over the dose ranges. No correlation between body surface area and absolute CL of TZT-1027 was established, vindicating that a flat dosing regimen might be used in the future. A correlation was observed between the percentage decrease in neutrophil count and the AUC of TZT-1027. CONCLUSIONS: In this study, the DLT of TZT-1027 was neutropenia and infusion arm pain. The recommended dose for phase II studies of TZT-1027 is 2.4 mg/m(2) given i.v. over 60 minutes, on days 1 and 8 every 21 days. Phase II studies have recently started.


Assuntos
Antineoplásicos/farmacocinética , Neoplasias/tratamento farmacológico , Oligopeptídeos/farmacocinética , Adulto , Idoso , Antineoplásicos/administração & dosagem , Antineoplásicos/efeitos adversos , Cromatografia Líquida de Alta Pressão , Esquema de Medicação , Feminino , Humanos , Infusões Intravenosas , Masculino , Dose Máxima Tolerável , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Neoplasias/metabolismo , Oligopeptídeos/administração & dosagem , Oligopeptídeos/efeitos adversos
4.
J Sex Res ; 52(3): 306-16, 2015.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24511896

RESUMO

Previous research on women's responses to male-targeted sexually explicit material (SEM) suggests that women may be critical toward such content. However, women's critical responses to SEM have not been explained empirically. The present study had two goals: (1) to investigate whether women's critical responses to male-targeted SEM depend on individual differences in gender role orientation (i.e., hyperfemininity) and (2) to explain the effect of hyperfemininity on critical responses to SEM by looking at the way sexual material is processed. In an online experiment among women aged 18 to 30 (N = 195), both the type of SEM (a male- versus female-targeted erotic story) and processing style (stimulus- versus response-focused) were manipulated. In addition, participants were divided into three groups based on low, moderate, or high hyperfemininity. When using stimulus-focused processing (i.e., attending to the characters and situational context of the story), women were more critical toward male-targeted SEM (relative to female-targeted material), but only when they had low and moderate degrees of hyperfemininity.


Assuntos
Literatura Erótica/psicologia , Feminilidade , Adolescente , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Adulto Jovem
5.
Exp Psychol ; 58(2): 162-70, 2011.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20705546

RESUMO

In the present study we investigated whether the mental representation of the concept categories is represented by the container image schema (Lakoff & Johnson, 1980). In two experiments participants decided whether two pictures were from the same category (animal or vehicle). Pictures were presented inside or outside a frame that should activate the container schema. We found that performance to pictures was influenced by the frame in congruence with the metaphorical mapping (same category­inside bounded region; different category­not in same bounded region). These results show that the concept categories is metaphorically represented by containers.


Assuntos
Cognição/fisiologia , Formação de Conceito/fisiologia , Imaginação/fisiologia , Adulto , Análise de Variância , Humanos , Estimulação Luminosa , Tempo de Reação/fisiologia
6.
Front Psychol ; 2: 121, 2011.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21713061

RESUMO

Numbers might be understood by grounding in spatial orientation, where small numbers are represented as low or to the left and large numbers are represented as high or to the right. We presented numbers in concrete (seven shoes in a shoe shop) or abstract (29 - 7) contexts and asked participants to make relative magnitude judgments. Following the judgment a target letter was presented at the top or bottom (Experiments 1-3) or left or right (Experiment 4) of the visual field. Participants were better at identifying letters at congruent than incongruent locations, but this effect was obtained only when numbers were presented in concrete contexts. We conclude that spatial grounding might have a smaller role for numbers in abstract than in concrete context.

7.
Exp Psychol ; 58(6): 454-63, 2011.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21592947

RESUMO

Previous studies (e.g., Pecher, Zeelenberg, & Wagenmakers, 2005) found that semantic classification performance is better for target words with orthographic neighbors that are mostly from the same semantic class (e.g., living) compared to target words with orthographic neighbors that are mostly from the opposite semantic class (e.g., nonliving). In the present study we investigated the contribution of phonology to orthographic neighborhood effects by comparing effects of phonologically congruent orthographic neighbors (book-hook) to phonologically incongruent orthographic neighbors (sand-wand). The prior presentation of a semantically congruent word produced larger effects on subsequent animacy decisions when the previously presented word was a phonologically congruent neighbor than when it was a phonologically incongruent neighbor. In a second experiment, performance differences between target words with versus without semantically congruent orthographic neighbors were larger if the orthographic neighbors were also phonologically congruent. These results support models of visual word recognition that assume an important role for phonology in cascaded access to meaning.


Assuntos
Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos , Reconhecimento Psicológico , Semântica , Vocabulário , Humanos , Desempenho Psicomotor , Tempo de Reação
8.
Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) ; 63(5): 942-54, 2010 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19731189

RESUMO

The conceptual metaphor theory states that abstract concepts are represented by image schemas from concrete domains. In the present study we investigated the mapping for SIMILARITY IS CLOSENESS using tasks with nonlinguistic materials. In Experiments 1 and 2 participants decided whether two squares were similar or dissimilar in colour. The spatial distance between the squares was varied. Performance to similar colours was better at shorter distances, whereas performance to dissimilar colours was better at longer distances. In Experiments 3 and 4 participants made distance decisions to similar and dissimilar colours squares. Performance was not affected by similarity. These results show that metaphorical mappings can be found even beyond the context of linguistic metaphors and that the mapping between SIMILARITY and CLOSENESS is asymmetrical.


Assuntos
Mapeamento Encefálico , Formação de Conceito/fisiologia , Discriminação Psicológica/fisiologia , Metáfora , Reconhecimento Psicológico/fisiologia , Tomada de Decisões/fisiologia , Lateralidade Funcional , Humanos , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos/fisiologia , Tempo de Reação/fisiologia , Estudantes , Universidades
9.
Front Psychol ; 1: 30, 2010.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21833200

RESUMO

We report an experiment that compared two explanations for the effect of congruency between a word's on screen spatial position and its meaning. On one account, congruency is explained by the match between position and a mental simulation of meaning. Alternatively, congruency is explained by the polarity alignment principle. To distinguish between these accounts we presented the same object names (e.g., shark, helicopter) in a sky decision task or an ocean decision task, such that response polarity and typical location were disentangled. Sky decision responses were faster to words at the top of the screen compared to words at the bottom of the screen, but the reverse was found for ocean decision responses. These results are problematic for the polarity principle, and support the claim that spatial attention is directed by mental simulation of the task-relevant conceptual dimension.

10.
Conscious Cogn ; 14(2): 316-26, 2005 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15950885

RESUMO

Participants studied lists of nonwords (e.g., froost, floost, stoost, etc.) that were orthographic-phonologically similar to a nonpresented critical lure, which was also a nonword (e.g., ploost). Experiment 1 showed a high level of false recognition for the critical lure. Experiment 2 showed that the false recognition effect was also present for forewarned participants who were informed about the nature of the false recognition effect and told to avoid making false recognition judgments. The present results show that false recognition effects can be obtained even when the critical lure itself is not stored during study. This finding is problematic for accounts that attribute false memories to implicit associative responses or spreading activation but is easily explained by global familiarity models of recognition memory.


Assuntos
Julgamento , Reconhecimento Psicológico , Humanos , Memória , Fonética , Percepção Visual
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
Detalhe da pesquisa