Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 100
Filtrar
1.
J Pers ; 89(5): 867-882, 2021 10.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33523483

RESUMO

OBJECTIVE: Religious identification is associated with specific values, particularly conservation values that focus on social rather than personal interest. Recent research, however, suggests that the psychological repercussions of religious commitment can persist after people cease identifying as religious. We examine if this religion residue effect leads to differences in values between those who were once religious but no longer identify as religious and those who never identified as religious. METHODS: We use longitudinal survey data to examine how changes in identification with religion are associated with Schwartz's circle of values. RESULTS: Results show that religious affiliates were more likely than both those who disaffiliated across waves and those who consistently had no affiliation to endorse each of the social focus values except universalism. As hypothesized, when it came to conservation values, those who disaffiliated from religion were more similar to affiliates than were those who were consistently unaffiliated. Additional analyses showed that (a) associations between religious identification trajectories and values were largely consistent across genders, and (b) those who disaffiliated from evangelical Protestant denominations stood out from other disaffiliates. CONCLUSIONS: We conclude with a discussion of how these findings further understanding of the association between religion and personal values.


Assuntos
Religião , Feminino , Humanos , Estudos Longitudinais , Masculino , Estados Unidos
2.
Aggress Behav ; 45(5): 507-516, 2019 09.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30989667

RESUMO

People often have to make decisions between immediate rewards and more long-term goals. Such intertemporal judgments are often investigated in the context of monetary choice or drug use, yet not in regard to aggressive behavior. We combined a novel intertemporal aggression paradigm with functional neuroimaging to examine the role of temporal delay in aggressive behavior and the neural correlates thereof. Sixty-one participants (aged 18-22 years; 37 females) exhibited substantial variability in the extent to which they selected immediate acts of lesser aggression versus delayed acts of greater aggression against a same-sex opponent. Choosing delayed-yet-more-severe aggression was increased by provocation and associated with greater self-control. Preferences for delayed aggression were associated with greater activity in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (VMPFC) during such choices, and reduced functional connectivity between the VMPFC and brain regions implicated in motor impulsivity. Preferences for immediate aggression were associated with reduced functional connectivity between the VMPFC and the frontoparietal control network. Dispositionally aggressive participants exhibited reduced VMPFC activity, which partially explained and suppressed their preferences for delayed aggression. Blunted VMPFC activity may thus be a neural mechanism that promotes reactive aggression towards provocateurs among dispositionally aggressive individuals. These findings demonstrate the utility of an intertemporal framework for investigating aggression and provide further evidence for the similar underlying neurobiology between aggression and other rewarding behaviors.


Assuntos
Agressão/fisiologia , Desvalorização pelo Atraso/fisiologia , Córtex Pré-Frontal/fisiologia , Adolescente , Nível de Alerta/fisiologia , Mapeamento Encefálico , Comportamento de Escolha/fisiologia , Feminino , Humanos , Comportamento Impulsivo/fisiologia , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética , Masculino , Rede Nervosa/fisiologia , Autocontrole/psicologia , Adulto Jovem
3.
Aggress Behav ; 44(3): 235-245, 2018 05.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29265383

RESUMO

People differ in how much they seek retribution for interpersonal insults, slights, rejections, and other antagonistic actions. Identifying individuals who are most prone towards such revenge-seeking is a theoretically-informative and potentially violence-reducing endeavor. However, we have yet to understand the extent to which revenge-seeking individuals exhibit specific features of aggressiveness, impulsivity, and what motivates their hunt for retribution. Toward this end, we conducted three studies (total N = 673), in which revenge-seeking was measured alongside these other constructs. Analyses repeatedly demonstrated that revenge-seeking was associated with greater physical (but not verbal) aggressiveness, anger, and hostility. Revenge-seeking's link to physical aggression was partially accounted for by impulses toward enjoying aggression and the tendency to use aggression to improve mood. Dominance analyses revealed that sadism explained the most variance in revenge-seeking. Revenge-seeking was associated with greater impulsive responses to negative and positive affect, as well as greater premeditation of behavior. These findings paint a picture of revenge-seekers as physically aggressive curators of anger, whose retributive acts are performed with planned malice and motivated by the act's entertaining and therapeutic qualities.


Assuntos
Agressão/fisiologia , Ira/fisiologia , Hostilidade , Comportamento Impulsivo/fisiologia , Personalidade/fisiologia , Prazer/fisiologia , Sadismo/fisiopatologia , Adolescente , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Adulto Jovem
4.
Aggress Behav ; 44(3): 285-293, 2018 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29417595

RESUMO

Alcohol use and abuse (e.g., binge drinking) are among the most reliable causes of aggressive behavior. Conversely, people with aggressive dispositions (e.g., intermittent explosive disorder) are at greater risk for subsequent substance abuse. Yet it remains unknown why aggression might promote subsequent alcohol use. Both aggressive acts and alcohol use are rewarding and linked to greater activity in neural reward circuitry. Through this shared instantiation of reward, aggression may then increase subsequent alcohol consumption. Supporting this mechanistic hypothesis, participants' aggressive behavior directed at someone who had recently rejected them, was associated with more subsequent beer consumption on an ad-lib drinking task. Using functional MRI, both aggressive behavior and beer consumption were associated with greater activity in the bilateral ventral striatum during acts of retaliatory aggression. These results imply that aggression is linked to subsequent alcohol abuse, and that a mechanism underlying this effect is likely to be the activation of the brain's reward circuitry during aggressive acts.


Assuntos
Agressão/fisiologia , Consumo de Bebidas Alcoólicas/fisiopatologia , Alcoolismo/fisiopatologia , Recompensa , Estriado Ventral/fisiologia , Adulto , Alcoolismo/diagnóstico por imagem , Feminino , Humanos , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética , Masculino , Estriado Ventral/diagnóstico por imagem , Adulto Jovem
5.
Behav Sleep Med ; : 1-8, 2017 Apr 18.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28418727

RESUMO

BACKGROUND/OBJECTIVE: Although sleep problems are linked to relationship difficulties, the mechanisms involved have not been empirically demonstrated. The present study considers self-control as such a mechanism. PARTICIPANTS: Data were collected from 342 predominantly white, middle-class, married adults. METHOD: Participants completed online questionnaires about sleep, marital aggression, and self-control, and a virtual voodoo doll task. RESULTS AND CONCLUSIONS: Sleep problems were associated with higher levels of aggression on all measures, and lower self-control mediated these associations. Associations did not depend on participant gender, presence of children in the home, income, or length of marriage.

6.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 111(17): 6254-7, 2014 Apr 29.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24733932

RESUMO

Intimate partner violence affects millions of people globally. One possible contributing factor is poor self-control. Self-control requires energy, part of which is provided by glucose. For 21 days, glucose levels were measured in 107 married couples. To measure aggressive impulses, each evening participants stuck between 0 and 51 pins into a voodoo doll that represented their spouse, depending how angry they were with their spouse. To measure aggression, participants competed against their spouse on a 25-trial task in which the winner blasted the loser with loud noise through headphones. As expected, the lower the level of glucose in the blood, the greater number of pins participants stuck into the voodoo doll, and the higher intensity and longer duration of noise participants set for their spouse.


Assuntos
Agressão , Glicemia/metabolismo , Casamento , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Negociação
7.
Pers Individ Dif ; 111: 193-198, 2017 Jun 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28970645

RESUMO

Theorists argue that self-control failure is the underlying cause of criminal behavior, with previous research linking poor self-control to delinquency and drug use. The path from self-control to crime is well-established, but less is known about whether criminal behavior contributes to self-control deficits over time. We investigated bi-directional relations between self-control assessed via a delay discounting task and self-reported crime over a three-year period. During their first, second (73.38% retention rate), and third (63.12% retention rate) years of college, 526 undergraduates completed a delay discounting task and reported on their criminal behavior. In order to maximize variability, participants with conduct problems were overrecruited, comprising 23.1% of the final sample. As expected, more discounting of hypothetical monetary rewards significantly predicted future property crime across a one and two-year period, even when controlling for initial levels of both. This study also demonstrated evidence of a bi-directional relationship; violent crime predicted higher rates of delay discounting one year later. These results suggest that bi-directional relations exist between self-control and types of crime.

8.
Neuroimage ; 132: 43-50, 2016 05 15.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26892861

RESUMO

Self-control often fails when people experience negative emotions. Negative urgency represents the dispositional tendency to experience such self-control failure in response to negative affect. Neither the neural underpinnings of negative urgency nor the more general phenomenon of self-control failure in response to negative emotions are fully understood. Previous theorizing suggests that an insufficient, inhibitory response from the prefrontal cortex may be the culprit behind such self-control failure. However, we entertained an alternative hypothesis: negative emotions lead to self-control failure because they excessively tax inhibitory regions of the prefrontal cortex. Using fMRI, we compared the neural activity of people high in negative urgency with controls on an emotional, inhibitory Go/No-Go task. While experiencing negative (but not positive or neutral) emotions, participants high in negative urgency showed greater recruitment of inhibitory brain regions than controls. Suggesting a compensatory function, inhibitory accuracy among participants high in negative urgency was associated with greater prefrontal recruitment. Greater activity in the anterior insula on negatively-valenced, inhibitory trials predicted greater substance abuse one month and one year after the MRI scan among individuals high in negative urgency. These results suggest that, among people whose negative emotions often lead to self-control failure, excessive reactivity of the brain's regulatory resources may be the culprit.


Assuntos
Afeto/fisiologia , Córtex Pré-Frontal/fisiologia , Autocontrole , Adolescente , Adulto , Consumo de Bebidas Alcoólicas , Encéfalo/fisiologia , Mapeamento Encefálico , Feminino , Humanos , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética , Masculino , Adulto Jovem
9.
Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci ; 16(3): 541-50, 2016 06.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26912270

RESUMO

Physical pain motivates the healing of somatic injuries, yet it remains unknown whether social pain serves a similarly reparative function toward social injuries. Given the substantial overlap between physical and social pain, we predicted that social pain would mediate the effect of rejection on greater motivation for social reconnection and affiliative behavior toward rejecters. In Study 1, the effect of rejection on an increased need to belong was mediated by reports of more intense social pain. In Study 2, three neural signatures of social pain (i.e., activity in the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex, left and right anterior insula during social rejection), each predicted greater behavioral proximity to rejecters. Our findings reify the overlap between social and physical pain. Furthermore, these results are some of the first to demonstrate the reparative nature of social pain and lend insight into how this process may be harnessed to promote postrejection reconnection.


Assuntos
Córtex Cerebral/fisiopatologia , Giro do Cíngulo/fisiopatologia , Motivação/fisiologia , Dor/fisiopatologia , Comportamento Social , Adolescente , Adulto , Mapeamento Encefálico , Feminino , Humanos , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética/métodos , Masculino , Rejeição em Psicologia , Adulto Jovem
10.
J Pers ; 84(3): 361-8, 2016 06.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25564936

RESUMO

Narcissists behave aggressively when their egos are threatened by interpersonal insults. This effect has been explained in terms of narcissists' motivation to reduce the discrepancy between their grandiose self and its threatened version, though no research has directly tested this hypothesis. If this notion is true, the link between narcissism and retaliatory aggression should be moderated by neural structures that subserve discrepancy detection, such as the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (dACC). This study tested the hypothesis that narcissism would only predict greater retaliatory aggression in response to social rejection when the dACC was recruited by the threat. Thirty participants (15 females; Mage = 18.86, SD = 1.25; 77% White) completed a trait narcissism inventory, were socially accepted and then rejected while undergoing fMRI, and then could behave aggressively toward one of the rejecters by blasting him or her with unpleasant noise. When narcissists displayed greater dACC activation during rejection, they behaved aggressively. But there was only a weak or nonsignificant relation between narcissism and aggression among participants with a blunted dACC response. Narcissism's role in aggressive retaliation to interpersonal threats is likely determined by the extent to which the brain's discrepancy detector registers the newly created gap between the grandiose and threatened selves.


Assuntos
Agressão/fisiologia , Giro do Cíngulo/fisiologia , Narcisismo , Personalidade/fisiologia , Distância Psicológica , Adolescente , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética , Masculino , Adulto Jovem
11.
J Pers ; 84(6): 737-749, 2016 12.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26248974

RESUMO

Why do people experience anger? Most of our knowledge on anger-triggering events is based on the study of reactions at a single time point in a person's life. Little research has examined how people experience anger in their daily life over time. In this study, we conducted a comprehensive examination of the situational determinants of anger over the course of 3 weeks. Using daily diary methodology, people (N = 173; 2,342 anger episodes) reported their most intense daily anger and, with an open-ended format, described the trigger. Participants also answered questions on anger intensity, control, and regulatory strategies, along with baseline personality trait measures. Using an iterative coding system, five anger trigger categories emerged: other people, psychological and physical distress, intrapersonal demands, environment, and diffuse/undifferentiated/unknown. Compared with other triggers, when anger was provoked by other people or when the source was unknown, there was a stronger positive association with anger intensity and lack of control. Personality traits (i.e., anger, mindfulness, psychological need satisfaction, the Big Five) showed few links to the experience and regulation of daily anger. Although aversive events often spur anger, the correlates and consequences of anger differ depending on the source of aversion; personality traits offer minimal value in predicting anger in daily life.


Assuntos
Ira/fisiologia , Relações Interpessoais , Personalidade/fisiologia , Autocontrole , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Adulto Jovem
12.
Aggress Behav ; 41(5): 443-54, 2015.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26918433

RESUMO

The negative affect that results from negative feedback is a substantial, proximal cause of aggression. People high in maladaptive perfectionism, the tendency to focus on the discrepancy between one's standards and performance, are characterized by an exaggerated negative affective response to negative feedback. This exacerbated affective response to failure may then dispose them to hurt others and themselves as aggression and self-harm are often perceived as a means to regulate negative affect. In Study 1, we demonstrated that maladaptive perfectionism was linked to greater aggressive behavior towards others after receiving negative feedback. Suggesting the presence of an emotion regulation strategy, this effect was mediated by the motivation to use aggression to improve mood. In Study 2, maladaptive perfectionism was linked to self-harm, an effect exacerbated by negative feedback and mediated by negative affect. These findings suggest that maladaptive perfectionists are at risk for greater harm towards others and the self because negative feedback has a stronger affective impact and harming others and the self is perceived a means to alleviate this aversive state.


Assuntos
Agressão/psicologia , Inteligência Emocional , Personalidade , Comportamento Autodestrutivo/psicologia , Adolescente , Afeto , Ira , Ajustamento Emocional , Retroalimentação Psicológica , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Inquéritos e Questionários , Adulto Jovem
13.
Aggress Behav ; 41(6): 537-43, 2015.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26198908

RESUMO

Social rejection can increase aggression, especially among people high in rejection sensitivity. Rejection impairs self-control, and deficits in self-control often result in aggression. A dose of glucose can counteract the effect of situational factors that undermine self-control. But no research has integrated these literatures to understand why rejection increases aggression, and how to reduce it. Using the I(3) model of aggression, we proposed that aggression would be highest under conditions of high instigation (rejection), high impellance (high rejection sensitivity), and low inhibition (drinking a beverage sweetened with a sugar substitute instead of glucose). As predicted, aggression was highest among participants who experienced social rejection, were high in rejection sensitivity, and drank a placebo beverage. A dose of glucose reduced aggression, especially among rejected people high in rejection sensitivity. These findings point to the importance of self-control in understanding why social rejection increases aggression, and how to prevent it.


Assuntos
Agressão/fisiologia , Glucose/farmacologia , Distância Psicológica , Autocontrole , Adulto , Agressão/efeitos dos fármacos , Feminino , Glucose/administração & dosagem , Humanos , Masculino , Placebos , Teoria Psicológica , Adulto Jovem
14.
J Pers Assess ; 97(6): 638-49, 2015.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26055531

RESUMO

In contexts that increasingly demand brief self-report measures (e.g., experience sampling, longitudinal and field studies), researchers seek succinct surveys that maintain reliability and validity. One such measure is the 12-item Brief Aggression Questionnaire (BAQ; Webster et al., 2014), which uses 4 3-item subscales: Physical Aggression, Verbal Aggression, Anger, and Hostility. Although prior work suggests the BAQ's scores are reliable and valid, we addressed some lingering concerns. Across 3 studies (N = 1,279), we found that the BAQ had a 4-factor structure, possessed long-term test-retest reliability across 12 weeks, predicted differences in behavioral aggression over time in a laboratory experiment, generalized to a diverse nonstudent sample, and showed convergent validity with a displaced aggression measure. In addition, the BAQ's 3-item Anger subscale showed convergent validity with a trait anger measure. We discuss the BAQ's potential reliability, validity, limitations, and uses as an efficient measure of aggressive traits.


Assuntos
Agressão/psicologia , Ira , Hostilidade , Adolescente , Adulto , Análise Fatorial , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Psicometria , Reprodutibilidade dos Testes , Inquéritos e Questionários , Adulto Jovem
15.
Neuroimage ; 101: 485-93, 2014 Nov 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25094019

RESUMO

Social rejection impairs self-regulation, yet the neural mechanisms underlying this relationship remain unknown. The right ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (rVLPFC) facilitates self-regulation and plays a robust role in regulating the distress of social rejection. However, recruiting this region's inhibitory function during social rejection may come at a self-regulatory cost. As supported by prominent theories of self-regulation, we hypothesized that greater rVLPFC recruitment during rejection would predict a subsequent self-regulatory imbalance that favored reflexive impulses (i.e., cravings), which would then impair self-regulation. Supporting our hypotheses, rVLPFC activation during social rejection was associated with greater subsequent nucleus accumbens (NAcc) activation and lesser functional connectivity between the NAcc and rVLPFC to appetitive cues. Over seven days, the effect of daily felt rejection on daily self-regulatory impairment was exacerbated among participants who showed a stronger rVLPFC response to social rejection. This interactive effect was mirrored in the effect of daily felt rejection on heightened daily alcohol cravings. Our findings suggest that social rejection likely impairs self-regulation by recruiting the rVLPFC, which then tips the regulatory balance towards reward-based impulses.


Assuntos
Fissura/fisiologia , Ego , Núcleo Accumbens/fisiologia , Córtex Pré-Frontal/fisiologia , Rejeição em Psicologia , Adolescente , Adulto , Consumo de Bebidas Alcoólicas/psicologia , Mapeamento Encefálico , Feminino , Humanos , Estudos Longitudinais , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética , Masculino , Recompensa , Adulto Jovem
16.
Aggress Behav ; 40(2): 120-39, 2014.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24115185

RESUMO

A key problem facing aggression research is how to measure individual differences in aggression accurately and efficiently without sacrificing reliability or validity. Researchers are increasingly demanding brief measures of aggression for use in applied settings, field studies, pretest screening, longitudinal, and daily diary studies. The authors selected the three highest loading items from each of the Aggression Questionnaire's (Buss & Perry, 1992) four subscales--Physical Aggression, Verbal Aggression, anger, and hostility--and developed an efficient 12-item measure of aggression--the Brief Aggression Questionnaire (BAQ). Across five studies (N = 3,996), the BAQ showed theoretically consistent patterns of convergent and discriminant validity with other self-report measures, consistent four-factor structures using factor analyses, adequate recovery of information using item response theory methods, stable test-retest reliability, and convergent validity with behavioral measures of aggression. The authors discuss the reliability, validity, and efficiency of the BAQ, along with its many potential applications.


Assuntos
Agressão/psicologia , Inquéritos e Questionários/normas , Adolescente , Adulto , Ira/fisiologia , Medicina Baseada em Evidências/instrumentação , Medicina Baseada em Evidências/normas , Feminino , Hostilidade , Humanos , Masculino , Personalidade , Valor Preditivo dos Testes , Psicometria/instrumentação , Reprodutibilidade dos Testes , Adulto Jovem
17.
J Pers ; 81(1): 87-102, 2013 Feb.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22329537

RESUMO

OBJECTIVE: Curiosity is the propensity to recognize and seek out new information and experience, including an intrinsic interest in learning and developing one's knowledge. With few exceptions, researchers have often ignored the social consequences of being curious. METHOD: In four studies using cross-sectional (N = 64), daily diary (Ns = 150 and 110, respectively), and behavioral experimental (N= 132) designs, we tested the hypothesis that individual differences in curiosity are linked to less aggression, even when people are provoked. RESULTS: We showed that both trait and daily curiosity were linked to less aggressive responses toward romantic relationship partners and people who caused psychological hurt. In time-lagged analyses, daily curiosity predicted less aggression from one day to the next, with no evidence for the reverse direction. Studies 3 and 4 showed that the inverse association between curiosity and aggression was strongest in close relationships and in fledgling (as opposed to long-lasting) romantic relationships. That is, highly curious people showed evidence of greater context sensitivity. Intensity of hurt feelings and other personality and relationship variables failed to account for these effects. CONCLUSIONS: Curiosity is a neglected mechanism of resilience in understanding aggression.


Assuntos
Agressão , Comportamento Exploratório , Estudos Transversais , Feminino , Humanos , Relações Interpessoais , Amor , Masculino , Narcisismo , Inventário de Personalidade , Testes Psicológicos , Resiliência Psicológica , Adulto Jovem
18.
Aggress Behav ; 39(6): 419-39, 2013.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23878068

RESUMO

Aggression pervades modern life. To understand the root causes of aggression, researchers have developed several methods to assess aggressive inclinations. The current article introduces a new behavioral method-the voodoo doll task (VDT)-that offers a reliable and valid trait and state measure of aggressive inclinations across settings and relationship contexts. Drawing on theory and research on the law of similarity and magical beliefs (Rozin, Millman, & Nemeroff [1986], Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 703-712), we propose that people transfer characteristics of a person onto a voodoo doll representing that person. As a result, causing harm to a voodoo doll by stabbing it with pins may have important psychological similarities to causing actual harm to the person the voodoo doll represents. Nine methodologically diverse studies (total N = 1,376) showed that the VDT had strong reliability, construct validity, and convergent validity. Discussion centers on the importance of magical beliefs in understanding the causes of aggressive inclinations.


Assuntos
Agressão/psicologia , Violência/psicologia , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Reprodutibilidade dos Testes , Fatores Sexuais , Adulto Jovem
19.
J Soc Psychol ; 163(4): 554-565, 2023 Jul 04.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34749593

RESUMO

In the United States, prospective adoptive parents often express preferences related to race. In two studies, we examined whether implicit racial bias against Black people may contribute to disparities in much less willingness to adopt Black children. The first study (N = 510) assessed individuals' implicit racial bias and their willingness to adopt a Black child. The second study (N = 2,001,652) used U.S. state-level implicit racial bias to predict adoption rates of Black foster children in each U.S. state. Greater implicit racial bias predicted less willingness to adopt Black children and less frequent adoptions of Black foster children. Implicit bias contributed to these disparities above and beyond explicit bias, with implicit bias having a 43% larger effect size than explicit bias on willingness to adopt a Black child. These are the first findings to demonstrate the role implicit bias plays in explaining large disparities between Americans' willingness to adopt Black and White children.


Assuntos
Adoção , Viés Implícito , Negro ou Afro-Americano , Racismo , Criança , Humanos , Estados Unidos , Brancos
20.
J Exp Psychol Gen ; 152(1): 98-119, 2023 Jan.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35913873

RESUMO

People often favor their ingroup and derogate members of the outgroup. However, less is known about "religious dones," who used to identify as religious but no longer do and have more transitional identities. Across six studies (N = 5,001; four preregistered), we examined the affiliative tendencies of religious dones and how they are perceived by other religious groups. In Study 1, using a Cyberball paradigm, religious dones included atheist targets relative to Christian targets. In Studies 2 and 3, currently religious participants demonstrated an attenuated tendency to commit the conjunction fallacy (i.e., associating people with heinous acts of violence) for religious dones compared to never religious targets. In Study 4, using a behavioral sacrifice paradigm (e.g., reducing compensation to reduce an uncomfortable noise blast to a partner), religious dones favored never religious partners (who did not reciprocate) and did not sacrifice as much for currently religious partners (who sacrificed for them as a member of their ingroup). Studies 5 and 6, investigating belief and identity, revealed that religious dones hold favorable attitudes toward other dones (and former believers) and the never religious (and never believers), whereas other groups view dones "in the middle." We also identified mediating mechanisms of trust, ingroup identification, and belief superiority. Taken together, these six studies suggest that religious dones are viewed as "a sheep in wolf's clothing," in which they are treated favorably by currently religious individuals but often prefer never religious individuals, even though that warmth is not consistently reciprocated. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).


Assuntos
Atitude , Religião , Humanos , Confiança
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
Detalhe da pesquisa