Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 72
Filtrar
1.
Int J Lang Commun Disord ; 57(1): 42-62, 2022 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34613648

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: A growing body of work shows that children with developmental language disorder (DLD) perform poorly on statistical word learning (SWL) tasks, consistent with the predictions of the Procedural Deficit Hypothesis that predicts that procedural memory is impaired in DLD. To date, however, SWL performance has not been compared across linguistically heterogeneous populations of children with DLD. AIMS: To compare SWL performance in a group of age, sex and non-verbal IQ-matched Catalan-Spanish and English-speaking children with and without DLD. METHODS & PROCEDURES: Two cohorts of children: (1) 35 Catalan-Spanish-speaking children with DLD (Mage = 8;7 years) and 35 age/sex-matched typical developing (TD) children (Mage = 8;9 years), and (2) 24 English-speaking children with DLD (Mage = 9;1 years) and 19 age/sex matched TD controls (Mage = 8;9 years) completed the tone version of a SWL task from Evans et al. (2009). Children listened to a tone language in which transitional probabilities within tone words were higher than those between words. OUTCOMES & RESULTS: For both Catalan-Spanish and English cohorts, overall performance for the children with DLD was poorer than that of the TD controls regardless of the child's native language. Item analysis revealed that children with DLD had difficulty tracking statistical information and using transitional probability to discover tone word boundaries within the input. For both the Catalan-Spanish and English-speaking children, SWL accounted for a significant amount of unique variance in Receptive and Expressive vocabulary. Likelihood ratio analysis revealed that for both Catalan-Spanish and English cohorts, children having performance ≤ 45% on the SWL task had an extremely high degree of likelihood of having DLD. The analysis also revealed that for the Catalan-Spanish and English-speaking children, scores of ≥ 75% and ≥ 70%, respectively, were highly likelihood to be children with normal language abilities. CONCLUSIONS & IMPLICATIONS: The findings add to a pattern suggesting that SWL is a mechanism that children rely on to acquire vocabulary. The results also suggest that SWL deficits, in particular when combined with other measures, may be a reliable diagnostic indicator for children with DLD regardless of the child's native language, and whether or not the child is bilingual or monolingual. WHAT THIS PAPER ADDS: What is already known on the subject Although there is some disagreement, a small but growing body of work suggests that deficits in procedural memory, as measured either by motor sequencing (Serial Reaction Time-SRT) or SWL tasks, may be part of the deficit profile of children with DLD. To date, studies have not examined SWL across linguistically heterogeneous populations of children with DLD to determine if it is a unique clinical marker of the disorder. What this paper adds to existing knowledge The results show that children with DLD, regardless of their native language, or whether the child is bi- or monolingual, have difficulties on SWL tasks, and that these deficits are linked to severity of the language disorder. Taken together, these results indicate that procedural memory deficits may be a core feature of DLD. This suggests that statistical-learning tasks using tone stimuli can also advance our understanding of statistical-learning abilities in children with DLD more globally. What are the potential or actual clinical implications of this work? The current study shows that statistical-learning tasks using tone stimuli can be used in conjunction with standardized assessment measures to differentiate children with DLD from children with typical language ability.


Assuntos
Transtornos do Desenvolvimento da Linguagem , Multilinguismo , Criança , Linguagem Infantil , Humanos , Idioma , Transtornos do Desenvolvimento da Linguagem/diagnóstico , Testes de Linguagem , Vocabulário
2.
Laterality ; 24(4): 450-481, 2019 Jul.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30417734

RESUMO

Structural lateralization of cortical language areas has been extensively studied in the past, with the conclusion that there is a predominant left lateralization in the majority of typically developing adolescents. However, lateralization in this age group has often been examined using cortical volumetric measures, without taking into account the independence of surface area and thickness. Utilizing structural MRI data in a relatively large sample size, the lateralization of cortical volume, surface area, and thickness was analysed across regions of interest (ROIs) known to support language processing in 118 typically developing adolescents, ages 13;9 to 18;9 using a laterality index. Results showed that the laterality index scores for volume and surface area were more strongly correlated than volume and thickness. Results also showed that not all language regions were left lateralized, with some ROIs being significantly right lateralized. Results also showed that surface area and thickness did not always share direction of lateralization. Taken together these results indicate that cortical ROIs supporting language are not all strongly left lateralized in adolescents. These data also show that cortical surface area and cortical thickness need to be treated independently in future studies characterizing language and lateralization in the adolescent brain.


Assuntos
Mapeamento Encefálico , Lateralidade Funcional/fisiologia , Idioma , Adolescente , Cérebro/fisiologia , Cognição/fisiologia , Feminino , Humanos , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética , Masculino , Lobo Temporal/fisiologia
3.
Cogn Dev ; 44: 1-11, 2017 Oct.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36777545

RESUMO

Patterns are often considered central to early mathematics learning; yet, the empirical evidence linking early pattern knowledge to mathematics performance is sparse. In the current study, 36 children ranging in age from 5 to 13 years old (M = 9.1 years) completed a pattern extension task with three pattern types that varied in difficulty. They also completed three math tasks that tapped calculation skill and knowledge of concepts. Children were successful on the pattern extension task, though older children fared better than younger children, potentially due in part to their explanations that considered both dimensions of the pattern (shape and size). Importantly, success on the pattern extension task was related to mathematics performance. After controlling for age and verbal working memory, patterning skill predicted calculation skill; however, patterning skill was not associated with knowledge of concepts. Results suggest that patterning may play a key role in the development of some aspects of early mathematics knowledge.

4.
Dev Sci ; 19(6): 918-932, 2016 11.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26452444

RESUMO

Recent research suggests that infants tend to add words to their vocabulary that are semantically related to other known words, though it is not clear why this pattern emerges. In this paper, we explore whether infants leverage their existing vocabulary and semantic knowledge when interpreting novel label-object mappings in real time. We initially identified categorical domains for which individual 24-month-old infants have relatively higher and lower levels of knowledge, irrespective of overall vocabulary size. Next, we taught infants novel words in these higher and lower knowledge domains and then asked if their subsequent real-time recognition of these items varied as a function of their category knowledge. While our participants successfully acquired the novel label-object mappings in our task, there were important differences in the way infants recognized these words in real time. Namely, infants showed more robust recognition of high (vs. low) domain knowledge words. These findings suggest that dense semantic structure facilitates early word learning and real-time novel word recognition.


Assuntos
Conhecimento , Vocabulário , Pré-Escolar , Humanos , Desenvolvimento da Linguagem , Reconhecimento Psicológico/fisiologia , Semântica , Percepção da Fala/fisiologia
5.
Child Dev ; 87(6): 1893-1908, 2016 11.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27302575

RESUMO

Although the size of a child's vocabulary associates with language-processing skills, little is understood regarding how this relation emerges. This investigation asks whether and how the structure of vocabulary knowledge affects language processing in English-learning 24-month-old children (N = 32; 18 F, 14 M). Parental vocabulary report was used to calculate semantic density in several early-acquired semantic categories. Performance on two language-processing tasks (lexical recognition and sentence processing) was compared as a function of semantic density. In both tasks, real-time comprehension was facilitated for higher density items, whereas lower density items experienced more interference. The findings indicate that language-processing skills develop heterogeneously and are influenced by the semantic network surrounding a known word.


Assuntos
Compreensão/fisiologia , Desenvolvimento da Linguagem , Semântica , Percepção da Fala/fisiologia , Vocabulário , Pré-Escolar , Feminino , Humanos , Lactente , Conhecimento , Masculino
6.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 110(25): 10165-70, 2013 Jun 18.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23723349

RESUMO

Precise nucleosome-positioning patterns at promoters are thought to be crucial for faithful transcriptional regulation. However, the mechanisms by which these patterns are established, are dynamically maintained, and subsequently contribute to transcriptional control are poorly understood. The switch/sucrose non-fermentable chromatin remodeling complex, also known as the Brg1 associated factors complex, is a master developmental regulator and tumor suppressor capable of mobilizing nucleosomes in biochemical assays. However, its role in establishing the nucleosome landscape in vivo is unclear. Here we have inactivated Snf5 and Brg1, core subunits of the mammalian Swi/Snf complex, to evaluate their effects on chromatin structure and transcription levels genomewide. We find that inactivation of either subunit leads to disruptions of specific nucleosome patterning combined with a loss of overall nucleosome occupancy at a large number of promoters, regardless of their association with CpG islands. These rearrangements are accompanied by gene expression changes that promote cell proliferation. Collectively, these findings define a direct relationship between chromatin-remodeling complexes, chromatin structure, and transcriptional regulation.


Assuntos
Proteínas Cromossômicas não Histona/metabolismo , DNA Helicases/metabolismo , Proteínas Nucleares/metabolismo , Nucleossomos/metabolismo , Regiões Promotoras Genéticas/fisiologia , Fatores de Transcrição/metabolismo , Animais , Proliferação de Células , Cromatina/fisiologia , Proteínas Cromossômicas não Histona/genética , Ilhas de CpG/fisiologia , DNA Helicases/genética , Fibroblastos/citologia , Fibroblastos/fisiologia , Regulação Neoplásica da Expressão Gênica/fisiologia , Técnicas de Silenciamento de Genes , Camundongos , Neoplasias/genética , Neoplasias/metabolismo , Neoplasias/patologia , Proteínas Nucleares/genética , Nucleossomos/genética , Cultura Primária de Células , Ligação Proteica/fisiologia , Proteína SMARCB1 , Fatores de Transcrição/genética , Ativação Transcricional/fisiologia
7.
Int J Lang Commun Disord ; 49(6): 761-70, 2014 Nov.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25039671

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: Previous research suggests that speakers are especially likely to produce manual communicative gestures when they have relative ease in thinking about the spatial elements of what they are describing, paired with relative difficulty organizing those elements into appropriate spoken language. Children with specific language impairment (SLI) exhibit poor expressive language abilities together with within-normal-range nonverbal IQs. AIMS: This study investigated whether weak spoken language abilities in children with SLI influence their reliance on gestures to express information. We hypothesized that these children would rely on communicative gestures to express information more often than their age-matched typically developing (TD) peers, and that they would sometimes express information in gestures that they do not express in the accompanying speech. METHODS & PROCEDURES: Participants were 15 children with SLI (aged 5;6-10;0) and 18 age-matched TD controls. Children viewed a wordless cartoon and retold the story to a listener unfamiliar with the story. Children's gestures were identified and coded for meaning using a previously established system. Speech-gesture combinations were coded as redundant if the information conveyed in speech and gesture was the same, and non-redundant if the information conveyed in speech was different from the information conveyed in gesture. OUTCOMES & RESULTS: Children with SLI produced more gestures than children in the TD group; however, the likelihood that speech-gesture combinations were non-redundant did not differ significantly across the SLI and TD groups. In both groups, younger children were significantly more likely to produce non-redundant speech-gesture combinations than older children. CONCLUSIONS & IMPLICATIONS: The gesture-speech integration system functions similarly in children with SLI and TD, but children with SLI rely more on gesture to help formulate, conceptualize or express the messages they want to convey. This provides motivation for future research examining whether interventions focusing on increasing manual gesture use facilitate language and communication in children with SLI.


Assuntos
Gestos , Transtornos do Desenvolvimento da Linguagem/diagnóstico , Transtornos do Desenvolvimento da Linguagem/terapia , Terapia da Linguagem , Comunicação não Verbal , Comportamento Verbal , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Feminino , Humanos , Transtornos do Desenvolvimento da Linguagem/psicologia , Testes de Linguagem , Masculino , Semântica , Medida da Produção da Fala , Vocabulário
8.
J Child Lang ; 41(2): 327-51, 2014 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23425593

RESUMO

This study tested the predictions of the procedural deficit hypothesis by investigating the relationship between sequential statistical learning and two aspects of lexical ability, lexical-phonological and lexical-semantic, in children with and without specific language impairment (SLI). Participants included forty children (ages 8;5-12;3), twenty children with SLI and twenty with typical development. Children completed Saffran's statistical word segmentation task, a lexical-phonological access task (gating task), and a word definition task. Poor statistical learners were also poor at managing lexical-phonological competition during the gating task. However, statistical learning was not a significant predictor of semantic richness in word definitions. The ability to track statistical sequential regularities may be important for learning the inherently sequential structure of lexical-phonological, but not as important for learning lexical-semantic knowledge. Consistent with the procedural/declarative memory distinction, the brain networks associated with the two types of lexical learning are likely to have different learning properties.


Assuntos
Transtornos do Desenvolvimento da Linguagem/fisiopatologia , Aprendizagem por Probabilidade , Estudos de Casos e Controles , Criança , Humanos , Fonética , Semântica , Vocabulário
9.
Community Pract ; 87(8): 21-5, 2014 Aug.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25226703

RESUMO

This paper outlines a review of the processes involved in setting up a pilot community group for mothers and babies around the theme of postnatal wellbeing. The group was designed and run by the Caerphilly Family Intervention Team (an Action For Children Project in South Wales) to meet what was considered to be an unmet need in provision in this area. The design and facilitation of the programme was framed relationally, with a focus on providing a context in which women could share their experiences and allow them to think about the transition to motherhood and their relationship with their baby. The report describes the background, context and intended purpose of the pilot, and then continues to give detail of the content, evaluation process and results. This article also offers a review of what works in this specific area of practice and suggests that community groups with a focus on transitions to motherhood, rather than mental ill health, can have a significant impact on wellbeing.


Assuntos
Serviços de Saúde Comunitária/organização & administração , Depressão/prevenção & controle , Serviços de Saúde Materna/organização & administração , Transtornos Mentais/prevenção & controle , Relações Mãe-Filho/psicologia , Parto/psicologia , Cuidado Pós-Natal/organização & administração , Adolescente , Feminino , Grupos Focais , Necessidades e Demandas de Serviços de Saúde , Humanos , Lactente , Recém-Nascido , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Cuidado Pós-Natal/psicologia , Gravidez , Avaliação de Programas e Projetos de Saúde , País de Gales , Saúde da Mulher
10.
Int J Lang Commun Disord ; 48(2): 144-59, 2013.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23472955

RESUMO

BACKGROUND & AIMS: The present study examined how phonological and lexical knowledge influences memory in children with specific language impairments (SLI). Previous work showed recall advantages for typical adults and children due to word frequency and phonotactic pattern frequency and a recall disadvantage due to phonological similarity among words. While children with SLI have well-documented memory difficulties, it is not clear whether these language knowledge factors also influence recall in this population. METHODS & PROCEDURES: Sixteen children with SLI (mean age = 10;2) and chronological age-matched typically developing children (CAM) controls recalled lists of words differing in phonological similarity, word frequency and phonotactic pattern frequency. While previous studies used a small set of words appearing in multiple word lists, the current study used a larger set of words, without replacement, so that children could not gain practice with individual test items. OUTCOMES & RESULTS: All main effects were significant. Interactions revealed that children with SLI were affected by similarity, but less so than their peers, comparably affected by word frequency and unaffected by phonotactic pattern frequency. CONCLUSIONS & IMPLICATIONS: Results due to phonological similarity suggest that children with SLI use less efficient encoding, while results due to word frequency and phonotactic pattern frequency were mixed. Children with SLI used coarse-grained language knowledge (word frequency) comparably with peers, but were less able to use fine-grained knowledge (phonotactic pattern frequency). Paired with phonological similarity results, this suggests that children with SLI have difficulty establishing robust phonological knowledge for use in language tasks.


Assuntos
Transtornos da Articulação/fisiopatologia , Transtornos do Desenvolvimento da Linguagem/fisiopatologia , Memória/fisiologia , Aprendizagem Verbal/fisiologia , Criança , Linguagem Infantil , Feminino , Humanos , Desenvolvimento da Linguagem , Masculino , Rememoração Mental/fisiologia , Fonética , Vocabulário
11.
J Speech Lang Hear Res ; 66(6): 2079-2094, 2023 06 20.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37227790

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: Phonological working memory is key to vocabulary acquisition, spoken word recognition, real-time language processing, and reading. Transcranial direct current stimulation, when coupled with behavioral training, has been shown to facilitate speech motor output processes, a key component of nonword repetition, the primary task used to assess phonological working memory. In this study, we examined the efficacy of combining overt nonword repetition training with anodal high-definition transcranial direct current stimulation (HD tDCS) to the presupplementary motor area (preSMA) to enhance nonword repetition. OBJECTIVE: This study investigated whether 20 min of active or sham anodal HD tDCS targeting preSMA concurrently with a nonword repetition task differentially impacted nonword repetition ability. METHOD: Twenty-eight neurotypical college-age adults (18-25 years; 19 females, eight males, one nonbinary) completed a 20-min nonword repetition training task where they received either active or sham 1-mA anodal HD tDCS to the preSMA while overtly repeating a list of four-, five-, six-, and seven-syllable English-like nonwords presented in a random order. Whole nonword accuracy and error patterns (phoneme and syllable) were measured prior to and following training. RESULTS: Following training, both groups showed a decrease in nonword repetition accuracy. The drop in performance was significantly greater for the active stimulation group compared to the sham stimulation group at the four-syllable nonword length. DISCUSSION: The findings suggest that targeting the speech motor component of nonword repetition through overt training and HD tDCS to the preSMA does not enhance phonological working memory ability.


Assuntos
Córtex Motor , Estimulação Transcraniana por Corrente Contínua , Masculino , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Adolescente , Adulto Jovem , Memória de Curto Prazo/fisiologia , Fonética , Fala
12.
Can Commun Dis Rep ; 49(7-8): 342-350, 2023 Aug 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38455877

RESUMO

Background: Invasive group A streptococcal disease (iGAS) is caused by Streptococcus pyogenes group A bacteria. In 2022, multiple disease alerts for iGAS in the Island Health region, in the context of increased infections in the paediatric population in Europe and the United States, prompted further investigation into local trends. This surveillance study summarizes epidemiological trends of iGAS in the region covered by Island Health, a regional health authority in British Columbia, in 2022. Methods: In British Columbia, iGAS is a reportable disease; all confirmed cases are reported to the regional authority and the provincial health authority (BC Centre for Disease Control). Island Health's iGAS surveillance system is passive and collects information on cases that are identified through laboratory testing. Surveillance data were summarized for 2022 and compared with historical data from 2017-2021. Results: In 2022, the incidence rate was 11.4 cases per 100,000 population (n=101), the highest observed rate in the last six years. The median age of cases was 53 years, with a range of 0-96 years, and 64% of cases were male. The highest risk of infection was reported in men 40-59 years of age, with an incidence rate of 21.3 cases per 100,000 population. The most common emm types were emm92 (n=14), emm49 (n=13), and emm83 (n=12). Overall, 85% (n=86) of cases were hospitalized, 21% (n=21) were admitted to the intensive care unit, and 6% (n=6) died. Conclusion: This study highlights that the incidence of iGAS in the Island Health region continued to increase throughout the coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19) pandemic, reaching its highest annual rate in 2022. In contrast to reports from Europe and the United States, there was no notable increase in infections in the paediatric population. Given the sustained increase in iGAS activity, continued monitoring and description of the epidemiology of these cases on a regular basis is imperative.

13.
J Neurodev Disord ; 14(1): 20, 2022 03 19.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35305572

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: Deficits in procedural memory have been proposed to account for the language deficits in specific language impairment (SLI). A key aspect of the procedural deficit hypothesis (PDH) account of SLI is that declarative memory is intact and functions as a compensatory mechanism in the acquisition of language in individuals with SLI. The current study examined the neural correlates of lexical-phonological and lexical-semantic processing with respect to these predictions in a group of adolescents with SLI with procedural memory impairment and a group of chronologically age-matched (CA) normal controls. METHODS: Participants completed tasks designed to measure procedural and declarative memory and two ERP tasks designed to assess lexical-semantic and lexical-phonological processing in the auditory modality. Procedural memory was assessed using a statistical learning task. Lexical-semantic processing was assessed using a sentence judgment task modulating semantic congruency and lexical-phonological processing was assessed using a word/nonword decision task modulating word frequency. Behavioral performance on the tasks, mean amplitude of the cortical response, and animated topographs were examined. RESULTS: Performance on the statistical word-learning task was at chance for the adolescents with SLI, whereas declarative memory was no different from the CA controls. Behavioral accuracy on the lexical-semantic task was the same for the adolescents with SLI and CA controls but accuracy on the lexical-phonological task was significantly poorer for the adolescents with SLI as compared to the CA controls. An N400 component was elicited in response to semantic congruency on the lexical-semantic task for both groups but differences were noted in both the location and time course of the cortical response for the SLI and CA groups. An N400 component was elicited by word frequency on the lexical-phonological task for the CA controls not for the adolescents with SLI. In contrast, post hoc analysis revealed a cortical response based on imageability for the adolescents with SLI, but not CA controls. Statistical word learning was significantly correlated with speed of processing on the lexical decision task for the CA controls but not for the adolescents with SLI. In contrast, statistical word learning ability was not correlated with the modulation of the N400 on either task for either group. CONCLUSION: The behavioral data suggests intact semantic conceptual knowledge, but impaired lexical phonological processing for the adolescents with SLI, consistent with the PDH. The pattern of cortical activation in response to semantic congruency and word frequency suggests, however, that the processing of lexical-semantic and lexical-phonological information by adolescents with a history of SLI may be supported by both overlapping and nonoverlapping neural generators to those of CA controls, and a greater reliance on declarative memory strategies. Taken together, the findings from this study suggest that the underlying representations of words in the lexicons of adolescents with a history of SLI may differ qualitatively from those of their typical peers, but these differences may only be evident when behavioral data and neural cortical patterns of activation are examined together.


Assuntos
Transtornos do Desenvolvimento da Linguagem , Transtorno Específico de Linguagem , Adolescente , Eletroencefalografia , Potenciais Evocados , Feminino , Humanos , Testes de Linguagem , Masculino
14.
Lang Speech Hear Serv Sch ; 52(2): 449-466, 2021 04 20.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33826402

RESUMO

Purpose The nature of the relationship between memory and sentence comprehension in school-age children with developmental language disorder (DLD) has been unclear. We present a novel perspective that highlights the relational influences of fluid intelligence, controlled attention, working memory (WM), and long-term memory (LTM) on sentence comprehension in children with and without DLD. This perspective has new and important implications for theory, assessment, and intervention. Method We review a large-scale study of children with and without DLD that focused on the connections between cognition, memory, and sentence comprehension. We also summarize a new model of these relationships. Results Our new model suggests that WM serves as a conduit through which syntactic knowledge in LTM, controlled attention, and general pattern recognition indirectly influence sentence comprehension in both children with DLD and typically developing children. For typically developing children, language-based LTM and fluid intelligence indirectly influence sentence comprehension. However, for children with DLD, controlled attention plays a larger indirect role. Conclusions WM plays a key role in children's ability to apply their syntactic knowledge when comprehending canonical and noncanonical sentences. Our new model has important implications for the assessment of sentence comprehension and for the treatment of larger sentence comprehension deficits.


Assuntos
Cognição , Compreensão , Inteligência , Transtornos do Desenvolvimento da Linguagem/fisiopatologia , Memória de Curto Prazo/fisiologia , Adolescente , Atenção , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Humanos , Lactente , Conhecimento , Idioma , Masculino , Projetos de Pesquisa , Instituições Acadêmicas
15.
Front Psychol ; 12: 600694, 2021.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33897523

RESUMO

PURPOSE: This study investigated whether the ability to utilize statistical regularities from fluent speech and map potential words to meaning at 18-months predicts vocabulary at 18- and again at 24-months. METHOD: Eighteen-month-olds (N = 47) were exposed to an artificial language with statistical regularities within the speech stream, then participated in an object-label learning task. Learning was measured using a modified looking-while-listening eye-tracking design. Parents completed vocabulary questionnaires when their child was 18-and 24-months old. RESULTS: Ability to learn the object-label pairing for words after exposure to the artificial language predicted productive vocabulary at 24-months and amount of vocabulary change from 18- to 24 months, independent of non-verbal cognitive ability, socio-economic status (SES) and/or object-label association performance. CONCLUSION: Eighteen-month-olds' ability to use statistical information derived from fluent speech to identify words within the stream of speech and then to map the "words" to meaning directly predicts vocabulary size at 24-months and vocabulary change from 18 to 24 months. The findings support the hypothesis that statistical word segmentation is one of the important aspects of word learning and vocabulary acquisition in toddlers.

16.
Front Psychol ; 12: 724356, 2021.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34621221

RESUMO

The purpose of this study was to investigate the dimensionality of the cognitive processes related to memory capacity and language ability and to assess the magnitude of the relationships among these processes in children developing typically (TD) and children with developmental language disorder (DLD). Participants were 234 children between the ages of 7;0 and 11;11 (117 TD and 117 DLD) who were propensity matched on age, sex, mother education and family income. Latent variables created from cognitive processing tasks and standardized measures of comprehension and production of lexical and sentential aspects of language were tested with confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) and structural regression. A five-factor CFA model that included the constructs of Fluid Intelligence, Controlled Attention, Working Memory, Long-Term Memory for Language Knowledge and Language Ability yielded better fit statistics than two four-factor nested models. The four cognitive abilities accounted for more than 92% of the variance in the language measures. A structural regression model indicated that the relationship between working memory and language ability was significantly greater for the TD group than the DLD group. These results are consistent with a broad conceptualization of the nature of language impairment in older, school-age children as encompassing a dynamic system in which cognitive abilities account for nearly all of the variance in linguistic abilities.

17.
Children (Basel) ; 8(2)2021 Jan 26.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33530420

RESUMO

Nonword repetition has been proposed as a diagnostic marker of developmental language disorder (DLD); however, the inconsistency in the ability of nonword repetition tasks (NRT) to identify children with DLD raises significant questions regarding its feasibility as a clinical tool. Research suggests that some of the inconsistency across NRT may be due to differences in the nature of the nonword stimuli. In this study, we compared children's performance on NRT between two cohorts: the children in the Catalan-Spanish cohort (CS) were bilingual, and the children in the European Portuguese cohort (EP) were monolingual. NRT performance was assessed in both Spanish and Catalan for the bilingual children from Catalonia-Spain and in Portuguese for the monolingual children from Portugal. Results show that although the absolute performance differed across the two cohorts, with NRT performance being lower for the CS, in both Catalan and Spanish, as compared to the EP cohort in both, the cut-points for the likelihood ratios (LH) were similar across the three languages and mirror those previously reported in previous studies. However, the absolute LH ratio values for this study were higher than those reported in prior research due in part to differences in wordlikeness and frequency of the stimuli in the current study. Taken together, the findings from this study show that an NRT consisting of 3-, 4-, and 5-syllable nonwords, which varies in wordlikeness ratings, when presented in a random order accurately identifies and correctly differentiates children with DLD from TD controls the child is bilingual or monolingual.

18.
Int J Lang Commun Disord ; 45(4): 494-509, 2010.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19821795

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: Children with specific language impairments (SLI) repeat nonwords less accurately than typically developing children, suggesting a phonological deficit. Much work has attempted to explain these results in terms of a phonological memory deficit. However, subsequent work revealed that these results might be explained better as a deficit in phonological sensitivity. AIMS: This study used a nonword repetition task to examine how children with SLI extract phonological regularities from their language input. METHODS & PROCEDURES: Eighteen English-speaking children with SLI (7;3-10;6) and 18 age-matched controls participated in two English nonword repetition tasks. Three- and four-syllable nonwords varied in a single phonotactic frequency manipulation, either consonant frequency or phoneme co-occurrence frequency, while all other factors were held constant. Repetitions were scored in terms of accuracy as either the percentage of phonemes correctly produced or phoneme co-occurrences (diphones) correctly produced. In addition, onset-to-onset reaction times and repetition durations were measured. OUTCOMES & RESULTS: Accuracy results revealed significant group, length, and phonotactic frequency effects. Children with SLI repeated nonwords less accurately than age-matched peers, and all children repeated three-syllable nonwords and those with higher frequency phonotactic patterns more accurately. However, phonotactic frequency by group interactions were not significant. Timing results were mixed, with group reaction time differences for co-occurrence frequency, but not consonant frequency, and no group repetition duration differences. CONCLUSIONS & IMPLICATIONS: While children with SLI were less accurate overall, non-significant interactions indicate that both groups of children were comparably affected by differences in consonant and diphone frequency.


Assuntos
Linguagem Infantil , Comportamento Imitativo , Transtornos do Desenvolvimento da Linguagem/psicologia , Fonética , Fala , Estudos de Casos e Controles , Criança , Feminino , Humanos , Idioma , Testes de Linguagem , Masculino , Psicolinguística , Tempo de Reação , Fatores de Tempo
19.
Front Hum Neurosci ; 14: 362, 2020.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33132869

RESUMO

Background: Current research suggests a neurobiological marker of developmental language disorder (DLD) in adolescents and young adults may be an atypical neural profile coupled with behavioral performance that overlaps with that of normal controls. Although many imaging techniques are not suitable for the study of speech and language processing in DLD populations, fNIRS may be a viable option. In this study we asked if fNIRS can be used to identify atypical cortical activation patterns in individual adults with DLD and track potential changes in cortical activation patterns following a phonological working memory training protocol enhanced with anodal HD tDCS stimulation to the presupplementary motor area (preSMA). Objective/Hypothesis: The purpose of this study was two-fold: (1) to determine if fNIRS can be used to identify atypical hemodynamic responses in individual young adults with DLD during active spoken word processing and, (2) to determine if fNIRS can detect changes in hemodynamic response in these same adults with DLD following anodal HD tDCS enhanced phonological working memory training. Methods: Two adult subjects with DLD (female, age 25) completed a total of two sessions of fNIRs working memory task prior to and following one session of a non-word repetition task paired with anodal HD tDCS (1.0 mA tDCS; 20 min) to the preSMA. Standardized z-scores of behavioral measures (accuracy and reaction time) and changes in hemodynamic response during an n-back working memory task for the two participants with DLD was compared to that of a normative sample of 21 age- and gender- matched normal controls (ages 18 to 25) prior to and following phonological working memory training. Results: Individual standardized z-scores for each participant with DLD indicated that prior to training, hemoglobin response in the prefrontal lobe for both participants was markedly different from each other and normal controls. Following training, standard scores showed that the hemodynamic response for both participants moved within normal limits for ROIs. Conclusion: These findings highlight the feasibility of fNIRS to establish individual differences in the link between behavior and neural patterns in single subjects with DLD, as well as track individual differences in changes in brain activity following working memory training.

20.
Opt Express ; 17(14): 11652-64, 2009 Jul 06.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19582080

RESUMO

High-contrast adaptive optics systems, such as those needed to image extrasolar planets, are known to require excellent wavefront control and diffraction suppression. The Laboratory for Adaptive Optics at UC Santa Cruz is investigating limits to high-contrast imaging in support of the Gemini Planet Imager (GPI). In this paper we examine the effect of heat sources in the testbed on point-spread-function (PSF) stability. Introducing a heat source primarily introduces image motion. The GPI error budget requires image motion to be less than 0.1 lambda /D. Systematic motion of the PSF core is typically 0.01 lambda /D rms and with a 20 watt heat source introduced near the pupil plane image motion is increased to 0.02 lambda /D rms. Therefore, even a heat source as large as 20 watts near the pupil plane causes errors below the GPI requirement, but the combination of the heat source and additional air turbulence on the system introduced by changes to the enclosure or the fan of other components can produce significantly more motion. Heat also can affect the speckle pattern in the high-contrast region, but in the final instrument other sources of error should be more significant.


Assuntos
Óptica e Fotônica , Ar , Algoritmos , Desenho de Equipamento , Temperatura Alta , Processamento de Imagem Assistida por Computador/métodos , Dispositivos Ópticos
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
Detalhe da pesquisa