Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 53
Filtrar
1.
Trends Neurosci ; 20(3): 106-11, 1997 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9061863

RESUMO

How the brain generates visual percepts is a central problem in neuroscience. We propose a detailed neural model of how lateral geniculate nuclei and the interblob cortical stream through V1 and V2 generate context-sensitive perceptual groupings from visual inputs. The model suggests a functional role for cortical layers, columns, maps and networks, and proposes homologous circuits for V1 and V2 with larger-scale processing in V2. An integrated treatment of interlaminar, horizontal, orientational and endstopping cortical interactions and a role for corticogeniculate feedback in grouping are proposed. Modeled circuits simulate parametric psychophysical data about boundary grouping and illusory contour formation.


Assuntos
Retroalimentação/fisiologia , Córtex Visual/fisiologia , Percepção Visual/fisiologia , Animais , Modelos Neurológicos
2.
Am J Psychiatry ; 138(8): 1092-5, 1981 Aug.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-7258389

RESUMO

The authors gave 22 chemical workers neurological, psychiatric, and neuropsychological examinations and placed them in one of two groups according to their degree of exposure to trimethyltin chloride spillage during January 1978. Other chemicals to which they had been exposed were dimethyltin dichloride and methyl chloride. Specific and nonspecific symptoms of intoxication of the CNS showed a significantly greater frequency in the highly exposed group, including cycles of depression and destructive rage, each lasting a few hours. These observations should alert diagnosticians to this type of occupational exposure.


Assuntos
Doenças Profissionais/induzido quimicamente , Compostos Orgânicos de Estanho/efeitos adversos , Transtornos Relacionados ao Uso de Substâncias/etiologia , Adulto , Atenção/efeitos dos fármacos , Indústria Química , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Doenças do Sistema Nervoso/induzido quimicamente , Fúria/efeitos dos fármacos
3.
Psychol Rev ; 101(3): 470-89, 1994 Jul.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-7938340

RESUMO

Visual search data are given a unified quantitative explanation by a model of how spatial maps in the parietal cortex and object recognition categories in the inferotemporal cortex deploy attentional resources as they reciprocally interact with visual representations in the prestriate cortex. The model visual representations are organized into multiple boundary and surface representations. Visual search in the model is initiated by organizing multiple items that lie within a given boundary or surface representation into a candidate search grouping. These items are compared with object recognition categories to test for matches or mismatches. Mismatches can trigger deeper searches and recursive selection of new groupings until a target object is identified. The model provides an alternative to Feature Integration and Guided Search models.


Assuntos
Percepção Espacial , Percepção Visual , Algoritmos , Percepção de Cores , Percepção de Forma , Humanos , Tempo de Reação
4.
Am J Med Genet ; 5(2): 125-35, 1980.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-7395907

RESUMO

Reported anthropometric data on 121 subjects with 47,XYY,47XXY,47,XXX, and 45,X aneuploidies were compared to those from 578 male and female control subjects by use of a single, unisex reference person ("phantom"). Subjects and controls were geometrically scaled to a standard stature of 170.18 cm, thus eliminating variance due to height. Deviations of anthropometric variables from specified phantom values were expressed as standard z-scores. By comparing z-scores of individual aneuploidy classes with those of their controls, further differences in proportionality came to light. The stratagem disclosed a systematic proportionality pattern between subjects and controls which appeared to be related to each specific sex chromosome aneuploidy. The phantom stratagem for proportional growth assessment appears to merit further use in genetic investigations where individual differences in size and shape confound the analysis of anthropometric data.


Assuntos
Antropometria , Síndrome de Klinefelter/fisiopatologia , Aberrações dos Cromossomos Sexuais/fisiopatologia , Síndrome de Turner/fisiopatologia , Cariótipo XYY/fisiopatologia , Feminino , Transtornos do Crescimento/genética , Transtornos do Crescimento/fisiopatologia , Humanos , Masculino , Matemática , Fenótipo , Valores de Referência
5.
Am J Hum Biol ; 4(4): 453-460, 1992.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28524392

RESUMO

Variability in both skin thickness and skinfold compressibility affects the relationship between the skinfold caliper reading at a particular site on the body and the actual adipose thickness at that site, thus inducing error in the estimation of body fatness. To investigate this variability, skinfold thickness by caliper and incised depth of subcutaneous adipose tissue were measured at 13 skinfold sites in 6 male and 7 female unembalmed cadavers aged 55 to 94 years. All skin was then removed and its thickness measured at the exact sites of skinfold measurement. The regional patterns for skin thickness were similar in men and women, though women had significantly (P < .05) thinner skin than men at the biceps, chest, supraspinale, and abdominal sites. Mean (SD) skin thickness for each cadaver over all sites ranged from 0.76 mm (0.28 mm) to 1.47 mm (0.43 mm), with an overall mean for men of 1.22 mm (0.36 mm) and for women of 0.98 mm (0.36 mm). The thickness of a double layer of skin expressed as a percentage of skinfold thickness for all cadavers over all 13 sites ranged from 7.1% to 33.4%. Because of their leanness and thicker skin, the mean for men, 22.7% (10.1%), was significantly greater than that for women, 10.8% (6.2%) (P < .0001). Mean skinfold compressibility over all sites was 53.5% (16.4%) in men adn 51.9% (16.5%) in women (not significant). Such marked variability in skinfold compressibility and in the relative contribution of skin thickness to skinfold thickness suggests the need for caution in comparing estimates of fatness by skinfold caliper between different subjects. © 1992 Wiley-Liss, Inc.

6.
Talanta ; 15(1): 87-94, 1968 Jan.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18960261

RESUMO

The electron capture detector has been used to measure ultra-trace quantities of beryllium separated as beryllium(II) trifluoroacetylacetonate by gas Chromatographic techniques. The lower limit of detectability is ca. 4 x 10(-13) g of beryllium. Calibration plots extend from 8 x 10(-13) to 4 x 10(-11) g of beryllium. Samples of beryllium in aqueous solution at four concentrations (1.18 x 10(-7), 1.18 x 10(-8), 2.95 x 10(-9), and 8.84 x 10(-10)g ml ) were analysed quantitatively by combining solvent extraction and gas chromatography. The distribution of beryllium during the extraction procedures was determined independently by use of radioactive beryllium-7, but the use of tracers is not required in the recommended procedure. Interference studies were made on cations and anions found in biological samples. At the concentrations used in the extraction procedure and the gas Chromatographic process, none of the fifteen ions studied interferes appreciably.

7.
Neural Netw ; 13(6): 571-88, 2000 Jul.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10987511

RESUMO

The visual cortex has a laminar organization whose circuits form functional columns in cortical maps. How this laminar architecture supports visual percepts is not well understood. A neural model proposes how the laminar circuits of V1 and V2 generate perceptual groupings that maintain sensitivity to the contrasts and spatial organization of scenic cues. The model can decisively choose which groupings cohere and survive, even while balanced excitatory and inhibitory interactions preserve contrast-sensitive measures of local boundary likelihood or strength. In the model, excitatory inputs from lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN) activate layers 4 and 6 of V1. Layer 6 activates an on-center off-surround network of inputs to layer 4. Together these layer 4 inputs preserve analog sensitivity to LGN input contrasts. Layer 4 cells excite pyramidal cells in layer 2/3, which activate monosynaptic long-range horizontal excitatory connections between layer 2/3 pyramidal cells, and short-range disynaptic inhibitory connections mediated by smooth stellate cells. These interactions support inward perceptual grouping between two or more boundary inducers, but not outward grouping from a single inducer. These boundary signals feed back to layer 4 via the layer 6-to-4 on-center off-surround network. This folded feedback joins cells in different layers into functional columns while selecting winning groupings. Layer 6 in V1 also sends top-down signals to LGN using an on-center off-surround network, which suppresses LGN cells that do not receive feedback, while selecting, enhancing, and synchronizing activity of those that do. The model is used to simulate psychophysical and neurophysiological data about perceptual grouping, including various Gestalt grouping laws.


Assuntos
Modelos Neurológicos , Rede Nervosa/fisiologia , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos/fisiologia , Córtex Visual/fisiologia , Animais , Biorretroalimentação Psicológica/fisiologia , Corpos Geniculados/citologia , Corpos Geniculados/fisiologia , Humanos , Rede Nervosa/citologia , Fechamento Perceptivo/fisiologia , Células Piramidais/citologia , Células Piramidais/fisiologia , Retina/citologia , Retina/fisiologia , Córtex Visual/citologia , Vias Visuais/citologia , Vias Visuais/fisiologia
8.
IEEE Trans Neural Netw ; 6(4): 805-18, 1995.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18263371

RESUMO

A new neural network architecture is introduced for the recognition of pattern classes after supervised and unsupervised learning. Applications include spatio-temporal image understanding and prediction and 3D object recognition from a series of ambiguous 2D views. The architecture, called ART-EMAP, achieves a synthesis of adaptive resonance theory (ART) and spatial and temporal evidence integration for dynamic predictive mapping (EMAP). ART-EMAP extends the capabilities of fuzzy ARTMAP in four incremental stages. Stage 1 introduces distributed pattern representation at a view category field. Stage 2 adds a decision criterion to the mapping between view and object categories, delaying identification of ambiguous objects when faced with a low confidence prediction. Stage 3 augments the system with a field where evidence accumulates in medium-term memory. Stage 4 adds an unsupervised learning process to fine-tune performance after the limited initial period of supervised network training. Each ART-EMAP stage is illustrated with a benchmark simulation example, using both noisy and noise-free data.

14.
Phys Sportsmed ; 9(2): 13, 1981 Feb.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27452839
16.
J Sports Sci ; 25(1): 43-53, 2007 Jan 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17127580

RESUMO

Rowers competing at the 2000 Olympic Games were measured for 38 anthropometric dimensions. The aim was to identify common physical characteristics that could provide a competitive advantage. The participants included 140 male open-class rowers, 69 female open-class rowers, 50 male lightweight rowers, and 14 female lightweight rowers. Body mass, stature, and sitting height were different (P < 0.01) between the open-class and lightweight rowers, as well as a comparison group of healthy young adults ("non-rowers", 42 males, 71 females), for both sexes. After scaling for stature, the open-class rowers remained proportionally heavier than the non-rowers, with greater proportional chest, waist, and thigh dimensions (P < 0.01). Rowers across all categories possessed a proportionally smaller hip girth than the non-rowers (P < 0.01), which suggested the equipment places some constraints on this dimension. Top-ranked male open-class rowers were significantly taller and heavier and had a greater sitting height (P < 0.01) than their lower-ranked counterparts. They were also more muscular in the upper body, as indicated by a larger relaxed arm girth and forearm girth (P < 0.01). For the male lightweight rowers, only proportional thigh length was greater in the best competitors (P < 0.01). In the female open-class rowers, skinfold thicknesses were lower in the more highly placed competitors (P < 0.01). In conclusion, the rowers in this sample demonstrated distinctive physical characteristics that distinguish them from non-rowers and other sports performers.


Assuntos
Antropometria , Tamanho Corporal/fisiologia , Esportes/fisiologia , Adulto , Composição Corporal/fisiologia , Estudos de Casos e Controles , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Dobras Cutâneas , Somatotipos/fisiologia
17.
Am J Phys Anthropol ; 66(1): 93-6, 1985 Jan.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-3976873

RESUMO

A fixed-rod, slider with a window, a double-scribed line for viewing a measuring tape affixed to the rod, and a sturdy footpiece are design features of an anthropometer for measuring projected lengths. The new instrument is designed to replace contemporary models which may be inaccessible to investigators with limited budgets or currency restrictions. None of the design features are beyond the ingenuity of local machinists to modify, find alternate materials, and use different machine procedures. The same principles of construction can be applied in making sliding calipers with straight and recurved branches. While there is some loss in portability of the fixed-length anthropometer, the improved stability and superior visual scale more than compensate for the disadvantages and make the new anthropometer a candidate for the instrument of choice in obtaining projected length measurements.


Assuntos
Antropometria/instrumentação , Antropologia Física , Desenho de Equipamento , Humanos
18.
Appl Opt ; 11(9): 1916-8, 1972 Sep 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20119257

RESUMO

The refractive index of a particle can be determined with microscope equipment for measuring interference in transmitted light. If the path difference is several wavelengths, white illumination will used. If the particle has strong dispersion, measurements made with white light give an apparent refractive index much higher than the true one. The apparent index is related to the group velocity of light waves in the substance in the same way that the true index is related to phase velocity. Flaky crystals of hematite showed apparent index 4.52, in agreement with theory, compared with the true ordinary index around 3.09.

19.
Appl Opt ; 11(9): 1919-23, 1972 Sep 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20119258

RESUMO

Computations of light scattering require finding Bessel functions of a series of orders. These are found most easily by recurrence, but excessive rounding errors may accumulate. Satisfactory procedures for cylinder and sphere functions are described. If argument z is real, find Y(n)(z) by recurrence to high orders. From two high orders of Y(n)(z) estimate J(n)(z). Use backward recurrence to maximum J(n)(z). Correct by forward recurrence to maximum. If z is complex, estimate high orders of J(n)(z) without Y(n)(z) and use backward recurrence.

20.
Percept Psychophys ; 62(6): 1160-81, 2000 Aug.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11019614

RESUMO

As has been observed by Wallach (1948), perceived lightness is proportional to the ratio between the luminances of adjacent regions in simple disk-annulus or bipartite scenes. This psychophysical finding resonates with neurophysiological evidence that retinal mechanisms of receptor adaptation and lateral inhibition transform the incoming illuminance array into local measures of luminance contrast. In many scenic configurations, however, the perceived lightness of a region is not proportional to its ratio with immediately adjacent regions. In a particularly striking example of this phenomenon, called White's illusion, the relationship between the perceived lightnesses of two gray regions is the opposite of what is predicted by local edge ratios or contrasts. This paper offers a new treatment of how local measures of luminance contrast can be selectively integrated to simulate lightness percepts in a wide range of image configurations. Our approach builds on a tradition of edge integration models (Horn, 1974; Land & McCann, 1971) and contrast/filling-in models (Cohen & Grossberg, 1984; Gerrits & Vendrik 1970; Grossberg & Mingolla, 1985a, 1985b). Our selective integration model (SIM) extends the explanatory power of previous models, allowing simulation of a number of phenomena, including White's effect, the Benary Cross, and shading and transparency effects reported by Adelson (1993), as well as aspects of motion, depth, haploscopic, and Gelb induced contrast effects. We also include an independently derived variant of a recent depthful version of White's illusion, showing that our model can inspire new stimuli.


Assuntos
Sensibilidades de Contraste/fisiologia , Luz , Modelos Teóricos , Percepção de Profundidade/fisiologia , Humanos
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
Detalhe da pesquisa