Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 14 de 14
Filtrar
1.
Twin Res Hum Genet ; 15(5): 668-71, 2012 Oct.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22877833

RESUMO

The mothers of 603 pairs of 3- to 13-year-old twins in Korea completed the Emotionality, Activity, Sociability (EAS) Temperament Survey and the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire in reference to their twins. Principal factor analysis of the seven scales comprising these measures yielded a general factor on which all the scales had moderate to large loadings. Univariate behavioral genetic analyses showed that individual differences on this general factor could best be accounted for by additive genetic and non-shared environmental effects, with a heritability of 53%. The results strengthen the construct validity of the general factor of personality (GFP) by extracting this higher-order dimension from disparate measures, and have implications regarding social desirability criticisms applied to the GFP theory.


Assuntos
Emoções , Personalidade , Temperamento , Gêmeos Dizigóticos/psicologia , Gêmeos Monozigóticos/psicologia , Adolescente , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Coleta de Dados , Análise Fatorial , Feminino , Humanos , Individualidade , Masculino , Personalidade/genética , República da Coreia , Comportamento Social , Desejabilidade Social , Inquéritos e Questionários
2.
Twin Res Hum Genet ; 13(2): 131-4, 2010 Apr.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20397743

RESUMO

In two studies, the General Factor of Personality (GFP) remained intact after controlling for the Lie scale from the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire, despite the Lie Scale showing significant correlations with the GFP defining traits. In Study 1, a re-analysis of 29 self-ratings from 322 pairs of twins (644 individuals) yielded a GFP both before and after controlling for social desirability. In Study 2, four measures of affect in 133 university students loaded on a GFP both before and after controlling for social desirability such that those high on the GFP were high in self-esteem and positive affect and low in depression and negative affect. These results join those from other studies failing to find evidence that the GFP is merely an artifact of evaluative bias.


Assuntos
Personalidade , Desejabilidade Social , Adulto , Depressão/patologia , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Determinação da Personalidade , Psicometria , Autoimagem
3.
Twin Res Hum Genet ; 13(4): 301-11, 2010 Aug.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20707700

RESUMO

We used structural equation modeling to test the hypothesis that a General Factor of Personality (GFP) occupies the apex of the hierarchy of personality disorders in three validation samples of the Dimensional Assessment of Personality Pathology - Basic Questionnaire (DAPP-BQ). In a general population sample (N = 942), we found a GFP explained 34% of the variance in four first-order factors and 33% of the variance in all 18 scales. In a twin sample (N = 1,346), a GFP explained 35% of the variance in four first-order factors and 34% of the variance in all 18 scales. In a clinical sample (N = 656), a GFP explained 34% of the variance in four first-order factors and 30% of the variance in all 18 scales.


Assuntos
Determinação da Personalidade , Transtornos da Personalidade/diagnóstico , Inquéritos e Questionários , Análise Fatorial , Humanos , Transtornos da Personalidade/psicologia , Psicometria
4.
Twin Res Hum Genet ; 12(6): 555-63, 2009 Dec.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19943718

RESUMO

We reanalyze previously published data on 309 MZ and 333 DZ twin pairs aged 25 to 74 years from the MIDUS survey, a nationally representative archived sample, to examine how much of the genetic covariance between a general factor of personality (GFP), a lower-order life history factor, and a general physical and mental health factor, is of the nonadditive variety. We found nonadditive genetic effects (D) could not be ruled out as a contributor to the shared variance of these three latent factors to a Super-K Life History factor. We suggest these genetic correlations support the view that a slow (K-selected) life history strategy, good health, and the GFP coevolved and are mutually coadapted through directional selection.


Assuntos
Evolução Biológica , Saúde Mental , Personalidade/fisiologia , Aptidão Física/fisiologia , Gêmeos Dizigóticos/genética , Gêmeos Monozigóticos/genética , Adulto , Idoso , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade
5.
Twin Res Hum Genet ; 12(4): 356-65, 2009 Aug.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19653836

RESUMO

In three studies, a General Factor of Personality (GFP) was found to occupy the apex of the hierarchical structure. In Study 1, a GFP emerged independent of method variance and accounted for 54% of the reliable variance in a multitrait-multimethod assessment of 391 Italian high school students that used self-, teacher-, and parent-ratings on the Big Five Questionnaire - Children. In Study 2, a GFP was found in the seven dimensions of Cloninger's Temperament and Character Inventory as well as the Big Five of the NEO PI-R, with the GFPtci correlating r = .72 with the GFPneo. These results indicate that the GFP is practically the same in both test batteries, and its existence does not depend on being extracted using the Big Five model. The GFP accounted for 22% of the total variance in these trait measures, which were assessed in 651 pairs of 14- to 30-year-old Japanese twins. In Study 3, a GFP accounted for 32% of the total variance in nine scales derived from the NEO PI-R, the Humor Styles Questionnaire, and the Trait Emotional Intelligence Questionnaire assessed in 386 pairs of 18- to 74-year-old Canadian and U.S. twins. The GFP was found to be 50% heritable with high scores indicating openness, conscientiousness, sociability, agreeableness, emotional stability, good humor and emotional intelligence. The possible evolutionary origins of the GFP are discussed.


Assuntos
Comparação Transcultural , Personalidade , Gêmeos/genética , Adolescente , Adulto , Idoso , Canadá , Feminino , Humanos , Inteligência , Relações Interpessoais , Japão , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Personalidade/genética , Determinação da Personalidade , Temperamento , Gêmeos/psicologia , Estados Unidos
6.
Int J Neurosci ; 119(5): 691-731, 2009.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19283594

RESUMO

We review the literature on the relation between whole brain size and general mental ability (GMA) both within and between species. Among humans, in 28 samples using brain imaging techniques, the mean brain size/GMA correlation is 0.40 (N = 1,389; p < 10(-10)); in 59 samples using external head size measures it is 0.20 (N = 63,405; p < 10(-10)). In 6 samples using the method of correlated vectors to distill g, the general factor of mental ability, the mean r is 0.63. We also describe the brain size/GMA correlations with age, socioeconomic position, sex, and ancestral population groups, which also provide information about brain-behavior relationships. Finally, we examine brain size and mental ability from an evolutionary and behavior genetic perspective.


Assuntos
Encéfalo/anatomia & histologia , Cognição , Inteligência , Envelhecimento , Animais , Evolução Biológica , Tamanho Corporal , Encéfalo/fisiologia , Cabeça/anatomia & histologia , Humanos , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética , Tamanho do Órgão , Grupos Raciais , Tempo de Reação/fisiologia , Caracteres Sexuais , Fatores Socioeconômicos
7.
Med Hypotheses ; 71(5): 629-40, 2008 Nov.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18656315

RESUMO

Recent editorials in this journal have defended the right of eminent biologist James Watson to raise the unpopular hypothesis that people of sub-Saharan African descent score lower, on average, than people of European or East Asian descent on tests of general intelligence. As those editorials imply, the scientific evidence is substantial in showing a genetic contribution to these differences. The unjustified ill treatment meted out to Watson therefore requires setting the record straight about the current state of the evidence on intelligence, race, and genetics. In this paper, we summarize our own previous reviews based on 10 categories of evidence: The worldwide distribution of test scores; the g factor of mental ability; heritability differences; brain size differences; trans-racial adoption studies; racial admixture studies; regression-to-the-mean effects; related life-history traits; human origins research; and the poverty of predictions from culture-only explanations. The preponderance of evidence demonstrates that in intelligence, brain size, and other life-history variables, East Asians average a higher IQ and larger brain than Europeans who average a higher IQ and larger brain than Africans. Further, these group differences are 50-80% heritable. These are facts, not opinions and science must be governed by data. There is no place for the "moralistic fallacy" that reality must conform to our social, political, or ethical desires.


Assuntos
Etnicidade/genética , Ciência/métodos , Adolescente , Povo Asiático/genética , População Negra/genética , Criança , Características Culturais , Humanos , Inteligência , Testes de Inteligência , Modelos Teóricos , Princípios Morais , Grupos Raciais , Ciência/normas , Confiança , População Branca/genética
8.
Proc Biol Sci ; 274(1619): 1773-7, 2007 Jul 22.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17504738

RESUMO

We carried out two studies to test the hypothesis that genetic and environmental influences explain population group differences in general mental ability just as they do individual differences within a group. We estimated the heritability and environmentality of scores on the diagrammatic puzzles of the Raven's Coloured and/or Standard Progressive Matrices (CPM/SPM) from two independent twin samples and correlated these estimates with group differences on the same items. In Study 1, 199 pairs of 5- to 7-year-old monozygotic (MZ) and dizygotic (DZ) twins reared together provided estimates of heritability and environmentality for 36 puzzles from the CPM. These estimates correlated with the differences between the twins and 94 Serbian Roma (both rs=0.32; Ns=36; ps<0.05). In Study 2, 152 pairs of adult MZ and DZ twins reared apart provided estimates of heritability and environmentality for 58 puzzles from the SPM. These estimates correlated with the differences among 11 diverse samples including (i) the reared-apart twins, (ii) another sample of Serbian Roma, and (iii) East Asian, White, South Asian, Coloured and Black high school and university students in South Africa. In 55 comparisons, group differences were more pronounced on the more heritable and on the more environmental items (mean rs=0.40 and 0.47, respectively; Ns=58; ps<0.05). After controlling for measurement reliability and variance in item pass rates, the heritabilities still correlated with the group differences, although the environmentalities did not. Puzzles found relatively difficult (or easy) by the twins were those found relatively difficult (or easy) by the others (mean r=0.87). These results suggest that population group differences are part of the normal variation expected within a universal human cognition.


Assuntos
Meio Ambiente , Inteligência/genética , Resolução de Problemas , Característica Quantitativa Herdável , Gêmeos , Adulto , Criança , Humanos , Inteligência/fisiologia , Testes de Inteligência , Minnesota , Ontário
9.
Proc Biol Sci ; 271(1557): 2583-5, 2004 Dec 22.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15615684

RESUMO

Although 51 twin and adoption studies have been performed on the genetic architecture of antisocial behaviour, only four previous studies have examined a genetic contribution to pro-social behaviour. Earlier work by the author with the University of London Institute of Psychiatry Adult Twin Register found that genes contributed approximately half of the variance to measures of self-report altruism, empathy, nurturance and aggression, including acts of violence. The present study extends those results by using a 22-item Social Responsibility Questionnaire with 174 pairs of monozygotic twins and 148 pairs of dizygotic twins. Forty-two per cent of the reliable variance was due to the twins' genes, 23% to the twins' common environment and the remainder to the twins' non-shared environment.


Assuntos
Atitude , Responsabilidade Social , Gêmeos/psicologia , Adulto , Cultura , Feminino , Genética Comportamental , Humanos , Masculino , Meio Social , Inquéritos e Questionários
10.
Am Psychol ; 67(6): 500-1, 2012 Sep.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22963424

RESUMO

Comments on the original article, "Intelligence: New findings and theoretical developments," by R. E. Nisbett, J. Aronson, C. Blair, W. Dickens, J. Flynn, D. F. Halpern, and E. Turkheimer (see record 2011-30298-001). The present authors assert Nisbett et al were incorrect when they claimed that between 1972 and 2002 there was a 5.5-point narrowing of the 15-point IQ gap between Blacks and Whites (p. 146). In doing so, they sidestepped Rushton and Jensen's (2006) objections to Dickens and Flynn's (2006) evidence and failed to include subsequent evidence. The present authors maintain that Nisbett et al failed to describe accurately how heritable g provides evidence of a significant genetic contribution to Black-White differences. The present authors claim Nisbett et al obscured the topic by invoking alleged age and social class interactions and adoption studies of very young children. Many twin and adoption studies have shown that by adolescence, there are equal heritabilities (about 50%) for Whites, Blacks, and East Asians (Hur, Shin, Jeong, & Han, 2006; Rushton & Jensen, 2010b). There is no evidence of any special cultural influence, such as extreme deprivation or being raised as a visible minority, that operates in one group and not in others.


Assuntos
Interação Gene-Ambiente , Testes de Inteligência , Inteligência , Humanos
11.
Biol Lett ; 3(6): 664-6, 2007 Dec 22.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17725970

RESUMO

Although over 50 twin and adoption studies have been performed on the genetic architecture of antisocial behaviour, far fewer studies have investigated prosocial behaviour, and none have done so on a non-western population. The present study examined mothers' ratings of prosocial behaviour in 514 pairs of 2- to 9-year-old South Korean monozygotic and dizygotic twins. Correlational analyses showed a tendency of increasing genetic effects and decreasing shared environmental effects with age although shared family environment effects and the moderating effects of age did not attain statistical significance in model-fitting analyses. The best-fitting model indicated that 55% (95% CI: 45-64%) of the variance in the 2- to 9-year-olds' prosocial behaviour was due to genetic factors and 45% (95% CI: 36-55%) was due to non-shared environmental factors. It is concluded that genetic and environmental influences on prosocial behaviour in young South Koreans are mostly similar to those in western samples.


Assuntos
Comportamento Social , Gêmeos Dizigóticos/psicologia , Gêmeos Monozigóticos/psicologia , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Feminino , Variação Genética , Humanos , Coreia (Geográfico) , Masculino , Fatores Sexuais , Gêmeos Dizigóticos/genética , Gêmeos Monozigóticos/genética
12.
Biol Lett ; 3(2): 157-60, 2007 Apr 22.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17251122

RESUMO

We test the hypothesis that polymorphisms of the brain regulator genes MCPH1 and ASPM contribute to variations in human brain size and its correlates. We measured general mental ability, head circumference and social intelligence in 644 Canadian adults (496 Caucasians, 36 Orientals, 84 Mixed Race/Other and 28 Blacks; 257 men and 387 women). The gene polymorphisms were assessed from buccal DNA; mental ability by Wonderlic Personnel Test and Multidimensional Aptitude Battery; head circumference by stretchless tape; and social intelligence by prosocial attitude questionnaires. Although all measures were construct valid and the allele frequencies showed expected population differences, no relationship was found between the genes and any of the criteria. Among Caucasian 18-25 year olds, for example, the two mental ability tests correlated with each other (r=0.78, N=476, p<0.001), with head circumference (r=0.17, N=182, p<0.05) and with prosocial attitudes (r=0.23, N=182, p<0.001).


Assuntos
Altruísmo , Inteligência/genética , Proteínas do Tecido Nervoso/genética , Polimorfismo Genético/fisiologia , Adolescente , Adulto , Proteínas de Ciclo Celular , Proteínas do Citoesqueleto , Feminino , Frequência do Gene , Cabeça/anatomia & histologia , Humanos , Masculino , Grupos Raciais/genética
13.
Psychol Sci ; 16(7): 555-9, 2005 Jul.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16008789

RESUMO

This study examined the genetic and environmental contribution to people's preference for spouses and friends to be similar to themselves. In their responses to 130 personality, attitude, and demographic questions, 174 pairs of monozygotic (MZ) twins resembled each other (r= .53) more than did 148 pairs of dizygotic (DZ) twins (r= .32), 322 pairs of spouses (r= .32), and 563 pairs of best friends (r= .20). It was not previously recognized that spouses and friends are as similar as DZ twins. MZ twins also chose spouses and best friends more similar to their co-twins' friends and spouses than did DZ twins (mean rs = .22 vs. .14). The twins' preference for spouses and friends similar to themselves was about 34% due to the twins' genes, 12% due to the twins' common environment, and 54% due to the twins' unique (nonshared) environment. Similarity to partners was more pronounced on the more heritable items than the less heritable items. It is concluded that people are genetically inclined to choose as social partners those who resemble themselves at a genetic level.


Assuntos
Comportamento de Escolha , Amigos , Casamento , Gêmeos/genética , Gêmeos/psicologia , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Meio Social , Inquéritos e Questionários
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
Detalhe da pesquisa