RESUMO
OBJECTIVE: Primary age-related tauopathy (PART) refers to tau neurofibrillary tangles restricted largely to the medial temporal lobe in the absence of significant beta-amyloid plaques. PART has been associated with cognitive impairment, but contributions from concomitant limbic age-related TDP-43 encephalopathy neuropathologic change (LATE-NC) are underappreciated. METHODS: We compare prevalence of LATE-NC and vascular copathologies in age- and Braak-matched patients with PART (n = 45, Braak stage I-IV, Thal phase 0-2) or early stage Alzheimer disease neuropathologic change (ADNC; n = 51, Braak I-IV, Thal 3-5), and examine their influence on clinical and cognitive decline. RESULTS: Concomitant LATE-NC and vascular pathology were equally common, and cognition was equally impaired, in PART (Mini-Mental State Examination [MMSE] = 24.8 ± 6.9) and ADNC (MMSE = 24.2 ± 6.0). Patients with LATE-NC were more impaired than those without LATE-NC on the MMSE (by 5.8 points, 95% confidence interval [CI] = 3.0-8.6), Mattis Dementia Rating Scale (DRS; 17.5 points, 95% CI = 7.1-27.9), Clinical Dementia Rating, sum of boxes scale (CDR-sob; 5.2 points, 95% CI = 2.1-8.2), memory composite (0.8 standard deviations [SD], 95% CI = 0.1-1.6), and language composite (1.1 SD, 95% CI = 0.2-2.0), and more likely to receive a dementia diagnosis (odds ratio = 4.8, 95% CI = 1.5-18.0). Those with vascular pathology performed worse than those without on the DRS (by 10.2 points, 95% CI = 0.1-20.3) and executive composite (1.3 SD, 95% CI = 0.3-2.3). Cognition declined similarly in PART and ADNC over the 5 years preceding death; however, LATE-NC was associated with more rapid decline on the MMSE (ß = 1.9, 95% CI = 0.9-3.0), DRS (ß = 7.8, 95% CI = 3.4-12.7), CDR-sob (ß = 1.9, 95% CI = 0.4-3.7), language composite (ß = 0.5 SD, 95% CI = 0.1-0.8), and vascular pathology with more rapid decline on the DRS (ß = 5.2, 95% CI = 0.6-10.2). INTERPRETATION: LATE-NC, and to a lesser extent vascular copathology, exacerbate cognitive impairment and decline in PART and early stage ADNC. ANN NEUROL 2022;92:425-438.
Assuntos
Doença de Alzheimer , Disfunção Cognitiva , Tauopatias , Doença de Alzheimer/patologia , Disfunção Cognitiva/patologia , Proteínas de Ligação a DNA , Humanos , Emaranhados Neurofibrilares/patologia , Placa Amiloide/patologia , Tauopatias/patologiaRESUMO
OBJECTIVE: The purpose of this study was to determine the sensitivity and specificity of α-synuclein seed amplification assay (αSyn-SAA) in antemortem and postmortem cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) of autopsy-confirmed patients with different distributions of pathological αSyn, co-pathologies, and clinical diagnoses. METHODS: The αSyn-SAA was used to test antemortem CSF samples from 119 subjects with a variety of clinical syndromes and standardized neuropathological examinations from Oregon Health and Science University (OHSU) and University of California San Diego (UCSD; 56 additional postmortem CSF samples available). The αSyn-SAA was also applied to frontal cortex and amygdala homogenates. Sensitivity and specificity were compared across distributions of αSyn pathology. Clinical data and co-pathologies were compared across αSyn-SAA positive and negative groups. RESULTS: Fifty-three individuals without and 66 with αSyn-pathology (neocortical [n = 38], limbic [n = 7], and amygdala-predominant [n = 21]) were included. There was a sensitivity of 97.8% and specificity of 98.1% of the αSyn-SAA to identify patients with limbic/neocortical pathology from antemortem CSF. Sensitivity to detect amygdala-predominant pathology was only 14.3%. Postmortem CSF and brain tissue αSyn-SAA analyses also showed higher assay positivity in samples from limbic/neocortical cases. INTERPRETATION: CSF αSyn-SAA reliably identifies αSyn seeds in patients with diffuse αSyn pathology in the context of co-pathology and non-Lewy body disease (LBD) diagnoses. The analysis of brain homogenates suggests that pathological αSyn in the amygdala might differ from pathological αSyn in the frontal cortex. The αSyn-SAA might facilitate the differential diagnosis of dementias with mixed pathologies. ANN NEUROL 2022;92:650-662.
Assuntos
Encéfalo , alfa-Sinucleína , Encéfalo/patologia , Humanos , Sensibilidade e Especificidade , alfa-Sinucleína/metabolismoRESUMO
Plasma biomarkers related to amyloid, tau, and neurodegeneration (ATN) show great promise for identifying these pathological features of Alzheimer's Disease (AD) as shown by recent clinical studies and selected autopsy studies. We have evaluated ATN plasma biomarkers in a series of 312 well-characterized longitudinally followed research subjects with plasma available within 5 years or less before autopsy and examined these biomarkers in relation to a spectrum of AD and related pathologies. Plasma Aß42, Aß40, total Tau, P-tau181, P-tau231 and neurofilament light (NfL) were measured using Single molecule array (Simoa) assays. Neuropathological findings were assessed using standard research protocols. Comparing plasma biomarkers with pathology diagnoses and ratings, we found that P-tau181 (AUC = 0.856) and P-tau231 (AUC = 0.773) showed the strongest overall sensitivity and specificity for AD neuropathological change (ADNC). Plasma P-tau231 showed increases at earlier ADNC stages than other biomarkers. Plasma Aß42/40 was decreased in relation to amyloid and AD pathology, with modest diagnostic accuracy (AUC = 0.601). NfL was increased in non-AD cases and in a subset of those with ADNC. Plasma biomarkers did not show changes in Lewy body disease (LBD), hippocampal sclerosis of aging (HS) or limbic-predominant age-related TDP-43 encephalopathy (LATE) unless ADNC was present. Higher levels of P-tau181, 231 and NfL predicted faster cognitive decline, as early as 10 years prior to autopsy, even among people with normal cognition or mild cognitive impairment. These results support plasma P-tau181 and 231 as diagnostic biomarkers related to ADNC that also can help to predict future cognitive decline, even in predementia stages. Although NfL was not consistently increased in plasma in AD and shows increases in several neurological disorders, it had utility to predict cognitive decline. Plasma Aß42/40 as measured in this study was a relatively weak predictor of amyloid pathology, and different assay methods may be needed to improve on this. Additional plasma biomarkers are needed to detect the presence and impact of LBD and LATE pathology.
Assuntos
Doença de Alzheimer , Disfunção Cognitiva , Doença por Corpos de Lewy , Doença de Alzheimer/patologia , Peptídeos beta-Amiloides , Biomarcadores , Cognição , Disfunção Cognitiva/diagnóstico , Humanos , Doença por Corpos de Lewy/patologia , Proteínas tauRESUMO
A major problem in the treatment of cocaine addiction is high rates of relapse. Relapse is often provoked by acute reexposure to cocaine-associated cues or to cocaine itself. The lateral habenula (LHb), an epithalamic nucleus, regulates midbrain dopaminergic systems that are known to be involved in cocaine taking and seeking behaviors. However, the role of this nucleus in cocaine self-administration and reinstatement of cocaine seeking has not been entirely parsed out. We used an operant self-administration and reinstatement procedure to explore the effect of Designer Receptors Exclusively Activated by Designer Drug (DREADD)-induced transient inhibition of LHb neurons on cocaine taking and seeking. Firstly, rats were injected with adeno-associated viral vectors expressing hM4 Di (a Gi/o -coupled DREADD) into the LHb, trained to self-administer cocaine (0.75 mg/kg/infusion), and the effect of clozapine-N-oxide (an inert ligand that activates DREADDs) was assessed on cocaine self-administration. Secondly, rats were injected with hM4 Di into the LHb, trained to self-administer cocaine; the operant response was extinguished, and cue- and cocaine priming-induced reinstatement was assessed. Thirdly, we tested the generality of the effect of inhibiting LHb neurons by assessing the effect of this manipulation on food-taking and seeking. hM4 Di -induced inhibition of LHb neurons increased cocaine but not food self-administration. In contrast, this manipulation decreased reinstatement of cocaine, but not food-seeking. Taken together, our data suggest that hM4 Di - induced LHb inhibition specifically mediates taking and seeking behaviors reinforced by cocaine but not by natural reinforcers. Further, our data indicate a dissociation in the role of LHb neurons on cocaine self-administration versus reinstatement of cocaine seeking.
Assuntos
Cocaína/farmacologia , Comportamento de Procura de Droga/efeitos dos fármacos , Comportamento Alimentar/efeitos dos fármacos , Habenula/efeitos dos fármacos , Animais , Transtornos Relacionados ao Uso de Cocaína/fisiopatologia , Condicionamento Operante/efeitos dos fármacos , Sinais (Psicologia) , Extinção Psicológica/efeitos dos fármacos , Inibição Psicológica , Masculino , Neurônios/efeitos dos fármacos , Ratos , Reforço Psicológico , AutoadministraçãoRESUMO
OBJECTIVE: The present study investigated cognitive mechanisms underlying the ability to stop "autocorrect" errors elicited by unexpected words in a read-aloud task, and the utility of autocorrection for predicting Alzheimer's disease (AD) biomarkers. METHOD: Cognitively normal participants (total n = 85; n = 64 with cerebrospinal fluid [CSF] biomarkers) read aloud six short paragraphs in which 10 critical target words were replaced with autocorrect targets, for example, The player who scored that final [paint] for the local team reported [him] experience. Autocorrect targets either replaced the most expected/dominant completion (i.e., point) or a less expected/nondominant completion (i.e., basket), and within each paragraph half of the autocorrect targets were content words (e.g., point/paint) and half were function words (e.g., his/him). Participants were instructed to avoid autocorrecting. RESULTS: Participants produced more autocorrect errors in paragraphs with dominant than with nondominant targets, and with function than with content targets. Cognitively normal participants with high CSF Tau/Aß42 (i.e., an AD-like biomarker profile) produced more autocorrect total errors than those below the Tau/Aß42 threshold, an effect also significant with dominant-function targets alone (e.g., saying his instead of him). A logistic regression model with dominant-function errors and age showed errors as the stronger predictor of biomarker status (sensitivity 83%; specificity 85%). CONCLUSIONS: Difficulty stopping autocorrect errors is associated with biomarkers indicating preclinical AD, and reveals promise as a diagnostic tool. Greater vulnerability of function over content words to autocorrection in individuals with AD-like biomarkers implicates monitoring and attention (rather than semantic processing) in the earliest of cognitive changes associated with AD risk. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).
Assuntos
Doença de Alzheimer , Masculino , Humanos , Doença de Alzheimer/psicologia , Leitura , Semântica , Atenção , Biomarcadores/líquido cefalorraquidiano , Peptídeos beta-Amiloides , Proteínas tauRESUMO
BACKGROUND AND OBJECTIVE: Patients with earlier age at onset of sporadic Alzheimer disease (AD) are more likely than those with later onset to present with atypical clinical and cognitive features. We sought to determine whether this age-related clinical and cognitive heterogeneity is mediated by different topographic distributions of tau-aggregate neurofibrillary tangles (NFTs) or by variable amounts of concomitant non-AD neuropathology. METHODS: The relative distribution of NFT density in hippocampus and midfrontal neocortex was calculated, and α-synuclein, TAR DNA binding protein 43 (TDP-43), and microvascular copathologies were staged, in patients with severe AD and age at onset of 51-60 (n = 40), 61-70 (n = 41), and >70 (n = 40) years. Regression, mediation, and mixed effects models examined relationships of pathologic findings with clinical features and longitudinal cognitive decline. RESULTS: Patients with later age at onset of AD were less likely to present with nonmemory complaints (odds ratio [OR] 0.46 per decade, 95% confidence interval [CI] 0.22-0.88), psychiatric symptoms (ß = -0.66, 95% CI -1.15 to -0.17), and functional impairment (ß = -1.25, 95% CI -2.34 to -0.16). TDP-43 (OR 2.00, 95% CI 1.23-3.35) and microvascular copathology (OR 2.02, 95% CI 1.24-3.40) were more common in later onset AD, and α-synuclein copathology was not related to age at onset. NFT density in midfrontal cortex (ß = -0.51, 95% CI -0.72 to -0.31) and midfrontal/hippocampal NFT ratio (ß = -0.18, 95% CI -0.26 to -0.10) were lower in those with later age at onset. Executive function (ß = 0.48, 95% CI 0.09-0.90) and visuospatial cognitive deficits (ß = 0.97, 95% CI 0.46-1.46) were less impaired in patients with later age at onset. Mediation analyses showed that the effect of age at onset on severity of executive function deficits was mediated by midfrontal/hippocampal NFT ratio (ß = 0.21, 95% CI 0.08-0.38) and not by concomitant non-AD pathologies. Midfrontal/hippocampal NFT ratio also mediated an association between earlier age at onset and faster decline on tests of global cognition, executive function, and visuospatial abilities. DISCUSSION: Worse executive dysfunction and faster cognitive decline in people with sporadic AD with earlier rather than later age at onset is mediated by greater relative midfrontal neocortical to hippocampal NFT burden and not by concomitant non-AD neuropathology.
Assuntos
Doença de Alzheimer , Neocórtex , Idade de Início , Doença de Alzheimer/patologia , Autopsia , Humanos , Neocórtex/patologia , Emaranhados Neurofibrilares/patologia , Proteínas tau/metabolismoRESUMO
BACKGROUND AND OBJECTIVE: Patients with dementia with Lewy bodies perform worse than those with Alzheimer disease (AD) on tests of visual perception, but the clinical utility of these tests remains unknown because studies often had clinically diagnosed groups that may inadvertently cross-contaminate Lewy body disease (LBD) with pure AD pathology, used experimental tests not easily adaptable for clinical use, and had no way to examine relationships between the severity of LBD pathology and degree of cognitive impairment. Therefore, we sought to determine whether performance on a widely used clinical test of visuoperceptual ability effectively differentiates between patients with autopsy-confirmed LBD or AD and correlates with the severity of LBD pathology. METHODS: Patients with mild to moderate dementia (n = 42) and cognitively healthy controls (n = 22) performed a Fragmented Letters Test in which they identified letters of the alphabet that were randomly visually degraded by 70% and additional visuospatial and episodic memory tests. At autopsy, dementia cases were confirmed to have LBD (n = 19), all with concomitant AD, or only AD (n = 23). Severity of α-synuclein pathology in the hippocampus and neocortex was rated on an ordinal scale. RESULTS: Patients with LBD performed worse than those with AD (B = -2.80 ± 0.91, p = 0.009) and healthy controls (B = -3.34 ± 1.09, p = 0.01) on the Fragmented Letters Test after adjustment for age, sex, education, Mini-Mental State Examination score, and ability to name intact letters. Patients with AD did not differ from controls (B = -0.55 ± 1.08, p = 0.87). The test effectively distinguished between patients with LBD or AD with 73% sensitivity and 87% specificity, and the area under the curve in receiver operating characteristic analyses was 0.85 (95% CI 0.72-0.95), higher than for standard tests of visuospatial ability (Block Design; 0.72; CI 0.35-0.75) or memory (California Verbal Learning Test, trials 1-5; 0.55; CI 0.57-0.88). Fragmented Letters Test scores were negatively correlated with LBD pathology density ratings in hippocampus and neocortical regions (Spearman rs = -0.53 to -0.69). DISCUSSION: Fragmented Letters Test performance can effectively differentiate patients with LBD pathology from those with only AD pathology at a mild to moderate stage of dementia, even when LBD occurs with significant concomitant AD pathology, and may also be useful for gauging the severity of cortical α-synuclein pathology in those with LBD.
Assuntos
Doença de Alzheimer , Doença por Corpos de Lewy , Humanos , Doença de Alzheimer/patologia , Doença por Corpos de Lewy/complicações , alfa-Sinucleína/metabolismo , Corpos de Lewy/patologia , Percepção VisualRESUMO
OBJECTIVE: To characterize age-related clinical heterogeneity in Alzheimer disease (AD) and determine whether it is modified by APOE genotype or concomitant non-AD pathology, we analyzed data from 1,750 patients with sporadic, pathologically confirmed severe AD. METHODS: In this retrospective cohort study, regression and mixed effects models assessed effects of estimated age at onset, APOE genotype, and their interaction on standardized clinical, cognitive, and pathologic outcome measures from the National Alzheimer's Coordinating Center database. RESULTS: A bimodal distribution of age at onset frequency in APOE ε4- cases showed best separation at age 63. Using this age cutoff, cases were grouped as ε4- early-onset AD (EOAD) (n = 169), ε4+ EOAD (n = 273), ε4- late-onset AD (LOAD) (n = 511), and ε4+ LOAD (n = 797). Patients with EOAD were more likely than patients with LOAD to present with noncognitive behavioral or motor symptoms or nonmemory cognitive complaints, and had more executive dysfunction, but less language impairment on objective cognitive testing. Age at onset and ε4- genotype were independently associated with lower baseline Mini-Mental State Examination scores and greater functional impairment and patients with EOAD had faster cognitive and functional decline than patients with LOAD regardless of APOE genotype. Patients with EOAD were more likely than patients with LOAD to receive a non-AD clinical diagnosis even though they were more likely to have pure AD without concomitant vascular or other non-AD neurodegenerative pathology. CONCLUSIONS: Early-onset sporadic AD is associated with a greater likelihood of an atypical, non-memory-dominant clinical presentation, especially in the absence of the APOE ε4 allele, which may lead to misattribution to non-AD underlying pathology.
Assuntos
Doença de Alzheimer/genética , Doença de Alzheimer/patologia , Apolipoproteínas E/genética , Heterogeneidade Genética , Idade de Início , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Doença de Alzheimer/epidemiologia , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Estudos RetrospectivosRESUMO
BACKGROUND: Arterial stiffening has emerged as an important risk factor for Alzheimer's disease (AD) and related dementias. Carotid-femoral pulse wave velocity has been proposed as a non-invasive and reproducible method to assess arterial stiffness. However, the association of pulse wave velocity with performance across multiple cognitive domains as well as interactions with in vivo AD biomarkers and apolipoprotein E (APOE) genotype has received limited study. METHOD: We studied 193 older adult volunteers (167 with normal cognition and 26 with mild cognitive impairment) who underwent comprehensive medical and neuropsychological evaluation at the University of California, San Diego Alzheimer's Disease Research Center. Cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) biomarkers were available on 123 participants (63%). Linear models examined whether pulse wave velocity significantly interacted with APOE ε4 status and CSF AD biomarker positivity (based on the ratio of total tau over beta-amyloid [tau/Aß42]) on memory, language, executive functioning, attention, and visuospatial abilities. RESULTS: After adjusting for demographic characteristics and vascular risk burden, across the entire sample, pulse wave velocity was associated with poorer executive functioning but not the performance in the other cognitive domains. When the modifying effects of AD genetic risk and CSF AD biomarkers were considered, pulse wave velocity interacted with APOE genotype and CSF tau/Aß ratio such that a stronger association between elevated pulse wave velocity and poorer memory performance was found among those positive for CSF and genetic AD markers. There were no significant interaction effects for non-memory cognitive domains. CONCLUSION: The findings suggest that pulse wave velocity, a non-invasive method to assess arterial wall properties, may be a useful marker of risk for cognitive decline, particularly among individuals who are APOE ε4 carriers or CSF AD biomarke0r-positive.
Assuntos
Doença de Alzheimer , Disfunção Cognitiva , Idoso , Doença de Alzheimer/genética , Peptídeos beta-Amiloides , Apolipoproteína E4/genética , Apolipoproteínas E/genética , Biomarcadores , Disfunção Cognitiva/genética , Genótipo , Humanos , Fragmentos de Peptídeos , Análise de Onda de Pulso , Proteínas tauRESUMO
BACKGROUND AND OBJECTIVES: Given prior work demonstrating that mild cognitive impairment (MCI) can be empirically differentiated into meaningful cognitive subtypes, we applied actuarial methods to comprehensive neuropsychological data from the University of California San Diego Alzheimer's Disease Research Center (ADRC) in order to identify cognitive subgroups within ADRC participants without dementia and to examine cognitive, biomarker, and neuropathologic trajectories. METHODS: Cluster analysis was performed on baseline neuropsychological data (n = 738; mean age 71.8). Survival analysis examined progression to dementia (mean follow-up 5.9 years). CSF Alzheimer disease (AD) biomarker status and neuropathologic findings at follow-up were examined in a subset with available data. RESULTS: Five clusters were identified: optimal cognitively normal (CN; n = 130) with above-average cognition, typical CN (n = 204) with average cognition, nonamnestic MCI (naMCI; n = 104), amnestic MCI (aMCI; n = 216), and mixed MCI (mMCI; n = 84). Progression to dementia differed across MCI subtypes (mMCI > aMCI > naMCI), with the mMCI group demonstrating the highest rate of CSF biomarker positivity and AD pathology at autopsy. Actuarial methods classified 29.5% more of the sample with MCI and outperformed consensus diagnoses in capturing those who had abnormal biomarkers, progressed to dementia, or had AD pathology at autopsy. DISCUSSION: We identified subtypes of MCI and CN with differing cognitive profiles, clinical outcomes, CSF AD biomarkers, and neuropathologic findings over more than 10 years of follow-up. Results demonstrate that actuarial methods produce reliable cognitive phenotypes, with data from a subset suggesting unique biological and neuropathologic signatures. Findings indicate that data-driven algorithms enhance diagnostic sensitivity relative to consensus diagnosis for identifying older adults at risk for cognitive decline.
Assuntos
Doença de Alzheimer/diagnóstico , Biomarcadores/líquido cefalorraquidiano , Disfunção Cognitiva/diagnóstico , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Doença de Alzheimer/líquido cefalorraquidiano , Doença de Alzheimer/patologia , Peptídeos beta-Amiloides/líquido cefalorraquidiano , Encéfalo/patologia , Disfunção Cognitiva/líquido cefalorraquidiano , Consenso , Progressão da Doença , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Proteínas tau/líquido cefalorraquidianoRESUMO
INTRODUCTION: Although subjective cognitive decline (SCD) may be an early risk marker of Alzheimer's Disease (AD), research on SCD among Hispanics/Latinos/as/x (henceforth Latinos/as) living in the U.S. is lacking. We investigated if the cross-sectional relationship of self-reported SCD with objective cognition varies as a function of ethnic background (Latinos/as versus Non-Hispanic Whites [NHWs]). Secondary analyses conducted solely within the Latino/a group investigated if informant reported SCD is associated with objective cognition and whether self-reported SCD is related to markers of brain health in a sub-sample of Latinos/as with available MRI data. METHODS: Eighty-three participants (≥60 years of age) without dementia (35 Latinos/as; 48 NHWs) completed the Mattis Dementia Rating Scale (MDRS) and the Subjective Cognitive Decline-Questionnaire (SCD-Q). Additionally, 22 Latino/a informants completed the informant-version of the SCD-Q. Hierarchical regression models investigated if ethnicity moderates the association of MDRS and SCD-Q scores after adjusting for demographics and depressive symptoms. Correlational analyses within the Latino/a group investigated self- and informant-reported associations of SCD-Q scores with objective cognition, and associations of self-reported SCD-Q scores with medial temporal lobe volume and thickness. RESULTS: Latinos/as had lower education and MDRS scores than NHWs. Higher SCD-Q scores were associated with lower MDRS scores only in Latinos/as. Within the Latino/a group, self, but not informant reported SCD was related to objective cognition. Medium to large effect sizes were found whereby higher self-reported SCD was associated with lower entorhinal cortex thickness and left hippocampal volume in Latinos/as. CONCLUSIONS: The association of SCD and concurrent objectively measured global cognition varied by ethnic background and was only significant in Latinos/as. Self-reported SCD may be an indicator of cognitive and brain health in Latinos/as without dementia, prompting clinicians to monitor cognition. Future studies should explore if SCD predicts objective cognitive decline in diverse groups of Latinos/as living in the U.S.
Assuntos
Disfunção Cognitiva , Cognição , Hispânico ou Latino , Humanos , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Autorrelato , Estados UnidosRESUMO
The present study examined the effects of aging and CSF biomarkers of Alzheimer's disease (AD) on the ability to control production of unexpected words in connected speech elicited by reading aloud. Fifty-two cognitively healthy participants aged 66-86 read aloud 6 paragraphs with 10 malapropisms including 5 on content words (e.g., "window cartons" that elicited autocorrect errors to "window curtains") and 5 on function words (e.g., "thus concept" that elicited autocorrections to "this concept") and completed a battery of neuropsychological tests including a standardized Stroop task. Reading aloud elicited more autocorrect errors on function than content words, but these were equally correlated with age and Aß1-42 levels. The ability to stop autocorrect errors declined in aging and with lower (more AD-like) levels of Aß1-42, and multiplicatively so, such that autocorrect errors were highest in the oldest-old with the lowest Aß1-42 levels. Critically, aging effects were significant even when controlling statistically for Aß1-42. Finally, both autocorrect and Stroop errors were correlated with Aß1-42, but only autocorrect errors captured unique variance in predicting Aß1-42 levels. Reading aloud requires simultaneous planning and monitoring of upcoming speech. These results suggest that healthy aging leads to decline in the ability to intermittently monitor for and detect conflict during speech planning and that subtle cognitive changes in preclinical AD magnify this aging deficit. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2020 APA, all rights reserved).
Assuntos
Doença de Alzheimer/psicologia , Biomarcadores/líquido cefalorraquidiano , Testes Neuropsicológicos/normas , Leitura , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Envelhecimento , Feminino , Humanos , MasculinoRESUMO
OBJECTIVE: To examine whether domain-specific patterns of cognitive impairment and trajectories of decline differed in patients with clinically diagnosed Parkinson disease dementia (PDD) (N = 29) and autopsy-confirmed dementia with Lewy bodies (DLB) (N = 58) or Alzheimer disease (AD) (N = 174) and to determine the impact of pooling patients with PDD and DLB in clinical trials targeting cognition. METHODS: Patients were matched on demographics and level of global cognitive impairment. Patterns of cross-sectional performance and longitudinal decline were examined in 4 cognitive domains: Visuospatial, Memory, Executive, and Language. Power analyses were performed to determine the numbers of participants needed to adequately power a hypothetical clinical trial to slow cognitive decline in pure PDD, pure DLB, or a mixed PDD/DLB group. RESULTS: Both DLB and PDD were more impaired and declined more rapidly than AD in the Visuospatial domain. Patients with PDD exhibited the most impairment and fastest decline in Executive, although patients with DLB also declined faster than AD. Memory was more impaired in AD than DLB and in both compared with PDD; however, all 3 groups declined at comparable rates. In contrast, PDD declined at a slower rate on Language measures than DLB or AD. Power analyses suggest that Visuospatial and Executive outcome measures would be most sensitive in PDD, but Memory and Language in DLB. CONCLUSION: DLB and PDD differ from each other, and from AD, in a cognitive domain-specific manner. As such, different outcome measures may be most sensitive to detecting changes in DLB vs PDD, suggesting that the 2 should be analyzed separately in clinical trials.
Assuntos
Cognição/fisiologia , Demência/fisiopatologia , Doença por Corpos de Lewy/fisiopatologia , Doença de Parkinson/fisiopatologia , Idoso , Doença de Alzheimer/fisiopatologia , Estudos Transversais , Demência/diagnóstico , Progressão da Doença , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Memória/fisiologia , Testes NeuropsicológicosRESUMO
Structural adaptations in brain regions involved in domain-general cognitive control are associated with life-long bilingualism and may contribute to the executive function advantage of bilinguals over monolinguals. To the degree that these adaptations support bilingualism, their disruption by Alzheimer's disease (AD) may compromise the ability to maintain proficiency in two languages, particularly in the less proficient, or nondominant, language that has greater control demands. The present study assessed this possibility in Spanish-English bilinguals with AD (nâ¯=â¯21) and cognitively normal controls (nâ¯=â¯30) by examining the brain correlates of dominant versus nondominant language performance on the Multilingual Naming Test (MINT), adjusting for age and education. There were no significant structural correlates of naming performance for either language in controls. In patients with AD, dominant language MINT performance was associated with cortical thickness of the entorhinal cortex and middle temporal gyrus, consistent with previous findings of temporal atrophy and related decline of naming abilities in AD. Nondominant language MINT performance, in contrast, was correlated with thickness of the left caudal anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), a central cognitive control region involved in error monitoring and task switching. The relationship between naming in the nondominant language and ACC in patients with AD but not in controls may reflect increased reliance on the ACC for nondominant language use in the face of atrophy of other control network components. The results are consistent with the possibility that the increased burden nondominant language use places on cognitive control systems compromised in AD may account for faster nondominant than dominant language decline in AD.
Assuntos
Doença de Alzheimer/patologia , Doença de Alzheimer/fisiopatologia , Função Executiva/fisiologia , Giro do Cíngulo/patologia , Multilinguismo , Lobo Temporal/patologia , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Doença de Alzheimer/diagnóstico por imagem , Feminino , Giro do Cíngulo/diagnóstico por imagem , Humanos , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Lobo Temporal/diagnóstico por imagemRESUMO
Hippocampal sclerosis (HS) is a prevalent cause of dementia in the oldest old but is generally misdiagnosed as Alzheimer's disease (AD) due to similarities in clinical presentation. To determine if clinical and cognitive features diverge over time, we compared results from longitudinal evaluations of participants in the University of California, San Diego, Alzheimer's Disease Research Center with autopsy-confirmed AD (n = 195), HS (n = 21), or both HS + AD (n = 18). Each group exhibited decline in all cognitive measures, with HS declining at a slower rate than AD on the Mini-Mental State Examination, immediate recall condition of a word-list learning test, and Dementia Rating Scale total and subtest scores (except memory). Five years before the final evaluation, more prominent semantic and visuospatial deficits were apparent in AD participants than in HS participants despite comparable global cognitive impairment. Groups did not differ on any measure of executive function. HS + AD differed from AD only on the Boston Naming Test. Overall, results suggest that HS dementia is associated with cognitive deficits that progress more slowly than, but generally mimic, those observed in AD.