Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 8 de 8
Filtrar
Mais filtros

Base de dados
País/Região como assunto
Tipo de documento
Intervalo de ano de publicação
1.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 117(18): 10089-10096, 2020 05 05.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32321833

RESUMO

Synesthesia is a neurologic trait in which specific inducers, such as sounds, automatically elicit additional idiosyncratic percepts, such as color (thus "colored hearing"). One explanation for this trait-and the one tested here-is that synesthesia results from unusually weak pruning of cortical synaptic hyperconnectivity during early perceptual development. We tested the prediction from this hypothesis that synesthetes would be superior at making discriminations from nonnative categories that are normally weakened by experience-dependent pruning during a critical period early in development-namely, discrimination among nonnative phonemes (Hindi retroflex /d̪a/ and dental /ɖa/), among chimpanzee faces, and among inverted human faces. Like the superiority of 6-mo-old infants over older infants, the synesthetic groups were significantly better than control groups at making all the nonnative discriminations across five samples and three testing sites. The consistent superiority of the synesthetic groups in making discriminations that are normally eliminated during infancy suggests that residual cortical connectivity in synesthesia supports changes in perception that extend beyond the specific synesthetic percepts, consistent with the incomplete pruning hypothesis.


Assuntos
Cognição/fisiologia , Neuroimagem , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos/fisiologia , Sinestesia/diagnóstico por imagem , Adulto , Face/diagnóstico por imagem , Face/fisiologia , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Estimulação Luminosa , Sinestesia/fisiopatologia
2.
Conscious Cogn ; 48: 212-231, 2017 02.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28013176

RESUMO

According to one theory, synaesthesia develops, or is preserved, because it helps children learn. If so, it should be more common among adults who faced greater childhood learning challenges. In the largest survey of synaesthesia to date, the incidence of synaesthesia was compared among native speakers of languages with transparent (easier) and opaque (more difficult) orthographies. Contrary to our prediction, native speakers of Czech (transparent) were more likely to be synaesthetes than native speakers of English (opaque). However, exploratory analyses suggested that this was because more Czechs learned non-native second languages, which was strongly associated with synaesthesia, consistent with the learning hypothesis. Furthermore, the incidence of synaesthesia among speakers of opaque languages was double that among speakers of transparent languages other than Czech, also consistent with the learning hypothesis. These findings contribute to an emerging understanding of synaesthetic development as a complex and lengthy process with multiple causal influences.


Assuntos
Aprendizagem/fisiologia , Multilinguismo , Transtornos da Percepção/epidemiologia , Adulto , Canadá/epidemiologia , República Tcheca/epidemiologia , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Prevalência , Sinestesia , Adulto Jovem
3.
Behav Brain Sci ; 40: e403, 2017 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29342817

RESUMO

In our target article, we argued that the positive results of neonatal imitation are likely to be by-products of normal aerodigestive development. Our hypothesis elicited various responses on the role of social interaction in infancy, the methodological issues about imitation experiments, and the relation between the aerodigestive theory and the development of speech. Here we respond to the commentaries.


Assuntos
Comportamento Imitativo , Fala , Feminino , Humanos , Relações Interpessoais , Gravidez
4.
Behav Brain Sci ; 40: e381, 2017 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27412068

RESUMO

More than 35 years ago, Meltzoff and Moore (1977) published their famous article, "Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates." Their central conclusion, that neonates can imitate, was and continues to be controversial. Here, we focus on an often-neglected aspect of this debate, namely, neonatal spontaneous behaviors themselves. We present a case study of a paradigmatic orofacial "gesture," namely tongue protrusion and retraction (TP/R). Against the background of new research on mammalian aerodigestive development, we ask: How does the human aerodigestive system develop, and what role does TP/R play in the neonate's emerging system of aerodigestion? We show that mammalian aerodigestion develops in two phases: (1) from the onset of isolated orofacial movements in utero to the postnatal mastery of suckling at 4 months after birth; and (2) thereafter, from preparation to the mastery of mastication and deglutition of solid foods. Like other orofacial stereotypies, TP/R emerges in the first phase and vanishes prior to the second. Based upon recent advances in activity-driven early neural development, we suggest a sequence of three developmental events in which TP/R might participate: the acquisition of tongue control, the integration of the central pattern generator (CPG) for TP/R with other aerodigestive CPGs, and the formation of connections within the cortical maps of S1 and M1. If correct, orofacial stereotypies are crucial to the maturation of aerodigestion in the neonatal period but also unlikely to co-occur with imitative behavior.


Assuntos
Comportamento Imitativo/fisiologia , Córtex Sensório-Motor/fisiologia , Língua/fisiologia , Animais , Animais Recém-Nascidos , Feminino , Humanos , Recém-Nascido , Masculino , Movimento , Sensação
5.
Conscious Cogn ; 21(3): 1533-40, 2012 Sep.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22763316

RESUMO

Researchers have long suspected that grapheme-color synaesthesia is useful, but research on its utility has so far focused primarily on episodic memory and perceptual discrimination. Here we ask whether it can be harnessed during rule-based Category learning. Participants learned through trial and error to classify grapheme pairs that were organized into categories on the basis of their associated synaesthetic colors. The performance of synaesthetes was similar to non-synaesthetes viewing graphemes that were physically colored in the same way. Specifically, synaesthetes learned to categorize stimuli effectively, they were able to transfer this learning to novel stimuli, and they falsely recognized grapheme-pair foils, all like non-synaesthetes viewing colored graphemes. These findings demonstrate that synaesthesia can be exploited when learning the kind of material taught in many classroom settings.


Assuntos
Percepção de Cores , Aprendizagem , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos , Transtornos da Percepção/psicologia , Formação de Conceito , Humanos , Estimulação Luminosa , Sinestesia , Transferência de Experiência
6.
Curr Biol ; 24(23): 2822-6, 2014 Dec 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25456450

RESUMO

The illumination of a scene strongly affects our perception of objects in that scene, e.g., the pages of a book illuminated by candlelight will appear quite yellow relative to other types of artificial illuminants. Yet at the same time, the reader still judges the pages as white, their surface color unaffected by the interplay of paper and illuminant. It has been shown empirically that we can indeed report two quite different interpretations of "color": one is dependent on the constant surface spectral reflectance of an object (surface color) and the other on the power of light of different wavelengths reflected from that object (reflected color). How then are these two representations related? The common view, dating from Aristotle, is that our experience of surface color is derived from reflected color or, in more familiar terms, that color perception follows from color sensation. By definition, color constancy requires that vision "discounts the illuminant"; thus, it seems reasonable that vision begins with the color of objects as they naively appear and that we infer from their appearances their surface color. Here, we question this classic view. We use metacontrast-masked priming and, by presenting the unseen prime and the visible mask under different illuminants, dissociate two ways in which the prime matched the mask: in surface color or in reflected color. We find that priming of the mask occurs when it matches the prime in surface color, not reflected color. It follows that color perception can arise without prior color sensation.


Assuntos
Percepção de Cores/fisiologia , Adaptação Fisiológica , Humanos , Iluminação , Experimentação Humana não Terapêutica
7.
Front Hum Neurosci ; 8: 98, 2014.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24592232

RESUMO

Learning and synesthesia are profoundly interconnected. On the one hand, the development of synesthesia is clearly influenced by learning. Synesthetic inducers - the stimuli that evoke these unusual experiences - often involve the perception of complex properties learned in early childhood, e.g., letters, musical notes, numbers, months of the year, and even swimming strokes. Further, recent research has shown that the associations individual synesthetes make with these learned inducers are not arbitrary, but are strongly influenced by the structure of the learned domain. For instance, the synesthetic colors of letters are partially determined by letter frequency and the relative positions of letters in the alphabet. On the other hand, there is also a small, but growing, body of literature which shows that synesthesia can influence or be helpful in learning. For instance, synesthetes appear to be able to use their unusual experiences as mnemonic devices and can even exploit them while learning novel abstract categories. Here we review these two directions of influence and argue that they are interconnected. We propose that synesthesia arises, at least in part, because of the cognitive demands of learning in childhood, and that it is used to aid perception and understanding of a variety of learned categories. Our thesis is that the structural similarities between synesthetic triggering stimuli and synesthetic experiences are the remnants, the fossilized traces, of past learning challenges for which synsethesia was helpful.

8.
Psychon Bull Rev ; 19(2): 211-7, 2012 Apr.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22231728

RESUMO

Typically, the search for order in grapheme-color synesthesia has been conducted by looking at the frequency of certain letter-color associations. Here, we report stronger associations when second-order similarity mappings are examined--specifically, mappings between the synesthetic colors of letters and letter shape, frequency, and position in the alphabet. The analyses demonstrate that these relations are independent of one other. More strikingly, our analyses show that each of the letter-color mappings is restricted to one dimension of color, with letter shape and ordinality linked to hue, and letter frequency linked to luminance. These results imply that synesthetic associations are acquired as the alphabet is learned, with associations involving letter shape, ordinality, and frequency being made independently and idiosyncratically. Because these mappings of similarity structure between domains (letters and colors) are similar to those found in numerous other cognitive and perceptual domains, they imply that synesthetic associations operate on principles common to many aspects of human cognition.


Assuntos
Percepção de Cores , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos , Cor , Humanos , Transtornos da Percepção/psicologia , Estimulação Luminosa , Tempo de Reação
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA