Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 27
Filtrar
1.
Brain ; 146(6): 2502-2511, 2023 06 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36395092

RESUMO

Idiopathic rapid eye movement sleep behaviour disorder (iRBD) has now been established as an important marker of the prodromal stage of Parkinson's disease and related synucleinopathies. However, although dopamine transporter single photon emission computed tomography (SPECT) has been used to demonstrate the presence of nigro-striatal deficit in iRBD, quantifiable correlates of this are currently lacking. Sensitivity to rewarding stimuli is reduced in some people with Parkinson's disease, potentially contributing to aspects of the neuropsychiatric phenotype in these individuals. Furthermore, a role for dopaminergic degeneration is suggested by the fact that reward insensitivity can be improved by dopaminergic medications. Patients with iRBD present a unique opportunity to study the relationship between reward sensitivity and early dopaminergic deficit in the unmedicated state. Here, we investigate whether a non-invasive, objective measure of reward sensitivity might be a marker of dopaminergic status in prodromal Parkinson's disease by comparing with SPECT/CT measurement of dopaminergic loss in the basal ganglia. Striatal dopaminergic deficits in iRBD are associated with progression to Parkinsonian disorders. Therefore, identification of a clinically measurable correlate of this degenerative process might provide a basis for the development of novel risk stratification tools. Using a recently developed incentivized eye-tracking task, we quantified reward sensitivity in a cohort of 41 patients with iRBD and compared this with data from 40 patients with Parkinson's disease and 41 healthy controls. Patients with iRBD also underwent neuroimaging with dopamine transporter SPECT/CT. Overall, reward sensitivity, indexed by pupillary response to monetary incentives, was reduced in iRBD cases compared with controls and was not significantly different to that in patients with Parkinson's disease. However, in iRBD patients with normal dopamine transporter SPECT/CT imaging, reward sensitivity was not significantly different from healthy controls. Across all iRBD cases, a positive association was observed between reward sensitivity and dopaminergic SPECT/CT signal in the putamen. These findings demonstrate a direct relationship between dopaminergic deficit and reward sensitivity in patients with iRBD and suggest that measurement of pupillary responses could be of value in models of risk stratification and disease progression in these individuals.


Assuntos
Doença de Parkinson , Transtorno do Comportamento do Sono REM , Humanos , Transtorno do Comportamento do Sono REM/diagnóstico por imagem , Doença de Parkinson/diagnóstico por imagem , Proteínas da Membrana Plasmática de Transporte de Dopamina , Dopamina , Recompensa
2.
Brain ; 144(4): 1247-1262, 2021 05 07.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33734344

RESUMO

Patients with small vessel cerebrovascular disease frequently suffer from apathy, a debilitating neuropsychiatric syndrome, the underlying mechanisms of which remain to be established. Here we investigated the hypothesis that apathy is associated with disrupted decision making in effort-based decision making, and that these alterations are associated with abnormalities in the white matter network connecting brain regions that underpin such decisions. Eighty-two patients with MRI evidence of small vessel disease were assessed using a behavioural paradigm as well as diffusion weighted MRI. The decision-making task involved accepting or rejecting monetary rewards in return for performing different levels of physical effort (hand grip force). Choice data and reaction times were integrated into a drift diffusion model that framed decisions to accept or reject offers as stochastic processes approaching a decision boundary with a particular drift rate. Tract-based spatial statistics were used to assess the relationship between white matter tract integrity and apathy, while accounting for depression. Overall, patients with apathy accepted significantly fewer offers on this decision-making task. Notably, while apathetic patients were less responsive to low rewards, they were also significantly averse to investing in high effort. Significant reductions in white matter integrity were observed to be specifically related to apathy, but not to depression. These included pathways connecting brain regions previously implicated in effort-based decision making in healthy people. The drift rate to decision parameter was significantly associated with both apathy and altered white matter tracts, suggesting that both brain and behavioural changes in apathy are associated with this single parameter. On the other hand, depression was associated with an increase in the decision boundary, consistent with an increase in the amount of evidence required prior to making a decision. These findings demonstrate altered effort-based decision making for reward in apathy, and also highlight dissociable mechanisms underlying apathy and depression in small vessel disease. They provide clear potential brain and behavioural targets for future therapeutic interventions, as well as modelling parameters that can be used to measure the effects of treatment at the behavioural level.


Assuntos
Apatia/fisiologia , Encéfalo/fisiopatologia , Doenças de Pequenos Vasos Cerebrais/complicações , Doenças de Pequenos Vasos Cerebrais/fisiopatologia , Tomada de Decisões/fisiologia , Idoso , Doenças de Pequenos Vasos Cerebrais/psicologia , Feminino , Humanos , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade
3.
Psychol Sci ; 32(5): 668-681, 2021 05.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33860711

RESUMO

Social cohesion relies on prosociality in increasingly aging populations. Helping other people requires effort, yet how willing people are to exert effort to benefit themselves and others, and whether such behaviors shift across the life span, is poorly understood. Using computational modeling, we tested the willingness of 95 younger adults (18-36 years old) and 92 older adults (55-84 years old) to put physical effort into self- and other-benefiting acts. Participants chose whether to work and exert force (30%-70% of maximum grip strength) for rewards (2-10 credits) accrued for themselves or, prosocially, for another. Younger adults were somewhat selfish, choosing to work more at higher effort levels for themselves, and exerted less force in prosocial work. Strikingly, compared with younger adults, older people were more willing to put in effort for others and exerted equal force for themselves and others. Increased prosociality in older people has important implications for human behavior and societal structure.


Assuntos
Motivação , Recompensa , Adolescente , Adulto , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Envelhecimento , Comportamento Cooperativo , Humanos , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Esforço Físico , Comportamento Social , Adulto Jovem
4.
Brain ; 143(8): 2502-2518, 2020 08 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32761061

RESUMO

Impulse control disorders in Parkinson's disease are common neuropsychiatric complications associated with dopamine replacement therapy. Some patients treated with dopamine agonists develop pathological behaviours, such as gambling, compulsive eating, shopping, or disinhibited sexual behaviours, which can have a severe impact on their lives and that of their families. In this study we investigated whether hypersensitivity to reward might contribute to these pathological behaviours and how this is influenced by dopaminergic medication. We asked participants to shift their gaze to a visual target as quickly as possible, in order to obtain reward. Critically, the reward incentive on offer varied over trials. Motivational effects were indexed by pupillometry and saccadic velocity, and patients were tested ON and OFF dopaminergic medication, allowing us to measure the effect of dopaminergic medication changes on reward sensitivity. Twenty-three Parkinson's disease patients with a history of impulse control disorders were compared to 26 patients without such behaviours, and 31 elderly healthy controls. Intriguingly, behavioural apathy was reported alongside impulsivity in the majority of patients with impulse control disorders. Individuals with impulse control disorders also exhibited heightened sensitivity to exogenous monetary rewards cues both ON and OFF (overnight withdrawal) dopamine medication, as indexed by pupillary dilation in anticipation of reward. Being OFF dopaminergic medication overnight did not modulate pupillary reward sensitivity in impulse control disorder patients, whereas in control patients reward sensitivity was significantly reduced when OFF dopamine. These effects were independent of cognitive impairment or total levodopa equivalent dose. Although dopamine agonist dose did modulate pupillary responses to reward, the pattern of results was replicated even when patients with impulse control disorders on dopamine agonists were excluded from the analysis. The findings suggest that hypersensitivity to rewards might be a contributing factor to the development of impulse control disorders in Parkinson's disease. However, there was no difference in reward sensitivity between patient groups when ON dopamine medication, suggesting that impulse control disorders may not emerge simply because of a direct effect of dopaminergic drug level on reward sensitivity. The pupillary reward sensitivity measure described here provides a means to differentiate, using a physiological measure, Parkinson's disease patients with impulse control disorder from those who do not experience such symptoms. Moreover, follow-up of control patients indicated that increased pupillary modulation by reward can be predictive of the risk of future emergence of impulse control disorders and may thereby provide the potential for early identification of patients who are more likely to develop these symptoms.


Assuntos
Antiparkinsonianos/efeitos adversos , Transtornos Disruptivos, de Controle do Impulso e da Conduta/induzido quimicamente , Levodopa/efeitos adversos , Doença de Parkinson/tratamento farmacológico , Recompensa , Idoso , Transtornos Disruptivos, de Controle do Impulso e da Conduta/psicologia , Dopamina/metabolismo , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Doença de Parkinson/psicologia
5.
Hippocampus ; 29(2): 63-67, 2019 02.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30069971

RESUMO

Classical views of the medial temporal lobe (MTL) have established that it plays a crucial role in long-term memory (LTM). Here we demonstrate, in a sample of patients who have undergone anterior temporal lobectomy for the treatment of pharmacoresistant epilepsy, that the MTL additionally plays a specific, causal role in short-term memory (STM). Patients (n=22) and age-matched healthy control participants (n=26) performed a STM task with a sensitive continuous report measure. This paradigm allowed us to examine recall memory for object identity, location and object-location binding, independently on a trial-by-trial basis. Our findings point to a specific involvement of MTL in object-location binding, but, crucially, not retention of either object identity or location. Therefore the MTL appears to perform a specific computation: binding disparate features that belong to a memory. These results echo findings from previous studies, which have identified a role for the MTL in relational binding for LTM, and support the proposal that MTL regions perform such a function for both STM and LTM, independent of the retention duration. Furthermore, these findings and the methodology employed here may provide a simple, sensitive and clinically valuable means to test memory dysfunuction in MTL disorders.


Assuntos
Lobectomia Temporal Anterior/tendências , Memória de Curto Prazo/fisiologia , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Lobo Temporal/diagnóstico por imagem , Lobo Temporal/fisiologia , Adulto , Lobectomia Temporal Anterior/efeitos adversos , Epilepsia Resistente a Medicamentos/diagnóstico por imagem , Epilepsia Resistente a Medicamentos/cirurgia , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Testes Neuropsicológicos
6.
Anal Biochem ; 567: 14-20, 2019 02 15.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30528914

RESUMO

The mechanism for the lysis pathway of double-stranded DNA bacteriophages involves a small hole-forming class of membrane proteins, the holins. This study focuses on a poorly characterized class of holins, the pinholin, of which the S21 protein of phage ϕ21 is the prototype. Here we report the first in vitro synthesis of the wildtype form of the S21 pinholin, S2168, and negative-dominant mutant form, S21IRS, both prepared using solid phase peptide synthesis and studied using biophysical techniques. Both forms of the pinholin were labeled with a nitroxide spin label and successfully incorporated into both bicelles and multilamellar vesicles which are membrane mimetic systems. Circular dichroism revealed the two forms were both >80% alpha helical, in agreement with the predictions based on the literature. The molar ellipticity ratio [θ]222/[θ]208 for both forms of the pinholin was 1.4, suggesting a coiled-coil tertiary structure in the bilayer consistent with the proposed oligomerization step in models for the mechanism of hole formation. 31P solid-state NMR spectroscopic data on pinholin indicate a strong interaction of both forms of the pinholin with the membrane headgroups. The 31P NMR data has an axially symmetric line shape which is consistent with lamellar phase proteoliposomes lipid mimetics.


Assuntos
Bacteriófagos/metabolismo , Proteínas Virais/síntese química , Sequência de Aminoácidos , Dicroísmo Circular , Bicamadas Lipídicas/química , Bicamadas Lipídicas/metabolismo , Ressonância Magnética Nuclear Biomolecular , Técnicas de Síntese em Fase Sólida , Marcadores de Spin , Proteínas Virais/química , Proteínas Virais/metabolismo
7.
Brain ; 141(10): 2848-2854, 2018 10 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30212839

RESUMO

Apathy is a common and under-recognized disorder that often emerges in the prodromal phase of Parkinsonian diseases. The mechanism by which this occurs is not known, but recent evidence from patients with established Parkinson's disease suggests that serotonergic dysfunction may play a role. The integrity of the raphe serotonergic system can be assessed alongside dopaminergic basal ganglia imaging using the radioligand 123I-ioflupane, which binds both serotonin and dopamine transporters. To investigate the relative roles of these neurotransmitters in prodromal parkinsonism, we imaged patients with idiopathic rapid eye movement sleep behaviour disorder, the majority of whom will develop a parkinsonian disorder in future. Forty-three patients underwent brain imaging with 123I-ioflupane single photon emission computed tomography and structural MRI. Apathy was quantified using the Lille Apathy Rating Scale. Other clinical parkinsonian features were assessed using standard measures. A negative correlation was observed between apathy severity and serotonergic 123I-ioflupane signal in the dorsal raphe nucleus (r = -0.55, P < 0.001). There was no significant correlation between apathy severity and basal ganglia dopaminergic signal, nor between dorsal raphe signal and other neuropsychiatric scores. This specific association between apathy and raphe 123I-ioflupane signal suggests that the serotonergic system might represent a target for the treatment of apathy.


Assuntos
Apatia/fisiologia , Núcleo Dorsal da Rafe/metabolismo , Transtorno do Comportamento do Sono REM/metabolismo , Transtorno do Comportamento do Sono REM/psicologia , Serotonina/metabolismo , Idoso , Feminino , Humanos , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Transtornos Parkinsonianos/psicologia , Sintomas Prodrômicos , Tomografia Computadorizada de Emissão de Fóton Único
8.
Psychol Sci ; 28(7): 1016-1026, 2017 Jul.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28488927

RESUMO

When rewards are available, people expend more energy, increasing their motivational vigor. In theory, incentives might drive behavior for two distinct reasons: First, they increase expected reward; second, they increase the difference in subjective value between successful and unsuccessful performance, which increases contingency-the degree to which action determines outcome. Previous studies of motivational vigor have never compared these directly. Here, we indexed motivational vigor by measuring the speed of eye movements toward a target after participants heard a cue indicating how outcomes would be determined. Eye movements were faster when the cue indicated that monetary rewards would be contingent on performance than when the cue indicated that rewards would be random. But even when the cue indicated that a reward was guaranteed regardless of speed, movement was still faster than when no reward was available. Motivation by contingent and certain rewards was uncorrelated across individuals, which suggests that there are two separable, independent components of motivation. Contingent motivation generated autonomic arousal, and unlike noncontingent motivation, was effective with penalties as well as rewards.


Assuntos
Movimentos Oculares/fisiologia , Motivação/fisiologia , Tempo de Reação/fisiologia , Recompensa , Adolescente , Adulto , Nível de Alerta/fisiologia , Comportamento/fisiologia , Comportamento de Escolha/fisiologia , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Estimulação Luminosa/métodos , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Adulto Jovem
9.
Nat Hum Behav ; 8(7): 1403-1416, 2024 Jul.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38802539

RESUMO

Ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC) is vital for decision-making. Functional neuroimaging links vmPFC to processing rewards and effort, while parallel work suggests vmPFC involvement in prosocial behaviour. However, the necessity of vmPFC for these functions is unknown. Patients with rare focal vmPFC lesions (n = 25), patients with lesions elsewhere (n = 15) and healthy controls (n = 40) chose between rest and exerting effort to earn rewards for themselves or another person. vmPFC damage decreased prosociality across behavioural and computational measures. vmPFC patients earned less, discounted rewards by effort more, and exerted less force when another person benefited, compared to both control groups. Voxel-based lesion mapping revealed dissociations between vmPFC subregions. While medial damage led to antisocial behaviour, lateral damage increased prosocial behaviour relative to patients with damage elsewhere. vmPFC patients also showed reduced effort sensitivity overall, but reward sensitivity was limited to specific subregions. These results reveal multiple causal contributions of vmPFC to prosocial behaviour, effort and reward.


Assuntos
Motivação , Córtex Pré-Frontal , Recompensa , Humanos , Córtex Pré-Frontal/diagnóstico por imagem , Córtex Pré-Frontal/fisiopatologia , Córtex Pré-Frontal/fisiologia , Masculino , Motivação/fisiologia , Feminino , Adulto , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Comportamento Social , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética , Tomada de Decisões/fisiologia , Idoso
10.
Sci Rep ; 12(1): 21476, 2022 12 12.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36509827

RESUMO

Apathy and impulsivity are expressed in a wide range of neuropsychiatric disorders, and, to a less severe extent, in healthy people too. Although traditionally considered to be opposite extremes of a single motivational spectrum, recent epidemiological questionnaire-based data suggest that both traits can in fact co-exist within the same individual. Here, we sought to investigate the relationship between these constructs in healthy people within a controlled task environment that examines the ability to make a decision under temporal uncertainty and measures the vigour of the response. Sixty participants performed a new version of the Traffic Light Task and completed self-report questionnaire measures of apathy and impulsivity. The task required individuals to make rapid decision-making for time-sensitive reward by squeezing a hand-held dynamometer as quickly as possible after a predictable event occurred (a traffic light turning green). Although apathy and impulsivity were positively correlated in questionnaire assessments, the two traits were associated with distinct behavioural signatures on the task. Impulsivity was expressed as an inflexible tendency to generate rapid anticipatory responses, regardless of cost-benefit information. Apathy, on the other hand, was associated with a blunted effect of reward on response vigour. These findings reveal how apathy and impulsivity are related to distinct dimensions of goal-directed behaviour, explaining how these traits might co-exist in the same individuals.


Assuntos
Apatia , Tomada de Decisões , Humanos , Tomada de Decisões/fisiologia , Apatia/fisiologia , Comportamento Impulsivo/fisiologia , Recompensa , Motivação
11.
Biochim Biophys Acta Biomembr ; 1864(3): 183836, 2022 03 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34906602

RESUMO

There have recently been advances in methods for detecting local secondary structures of membrane protein using electron paramagnetic resonance (EPR). A three pulsed electron spin echo envelope modulation (ESEEM) approach was used to determine the local helical secondary structure of the small hole forming membrane protein, S21 pinholin. This ESEEM approach uses a combination of site-directed spin labeling and 2H-labeled side chains. Pinholin S21 is responsible for the permeabilization of the inner cytosolic membrane of double stranded DNA bacteriophage host cells. In this study, we report on the overall global helical structure using circular dichroism (CD) spectroscopy for the active form and the negative-dominant inactive mutant form of S21 pinholin. The local helical secondary structure was confirmed for both transmembrane domains (TMDs) for the active and inactive S21 pinholin using the ESEEM spectroscopic technique. Comparison of the ESEEM normalized frequency domain intensity for each transmembrane domain gives an insight into the α-helical folding nature of these domains as opposed to a π or 310-helix which have been observed in other channel forming proteins.


Assuntos
Bacteriófagos/metabolismo , Espectroscopia de Ressonância de Spin Eletrônica/métodos , Bicamadas Lipídicas/química , Proteínas de Membrana/química , Proteínas Virais/química , Sequência de Aminoácidos , Estrutura Secundária de Proteína
12.
J Neuropsychol ; 16(1): 236-258, 2022 03.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34532963

RESUMO

Apathy is a common, disabling neuropsychiatric syndrome that occurs across many brain disorders and may be associated with diminished motivation in behavioural, cognitive, emotional and social domains. Assessment is complicated by the variability of symptoms across apathy domains and self-report from patients, which can be misleading due to their lack of insight. Independent evaluation by clinicians also has limitations though if it has to be performed with limited time. Caregiver reports are a viable alternative, but current assessments for them either do not distinguish between different apathy domains or are interview-based and take long to administer. In this study, we developed a brief caregiver questionnaire version of the recently developed Apathy Motivation Index (AMI), which is a self-report tool. We confirmed three apathy factors in this new caregiver measure (AMI-CG) that were also present in the AMI: Behavioural Activation, Emotional Sensitivity and Social Motivation. Furthermore, we validated the scores against more extensive caregiver interviews using the established Lillle apathy rating scale as well as patient self-reports of apathy, measures of depression, anhedonia, cognition, activities of daily living and caregiver burden across four different neurological conditions: Parkinson's disease, Alzheimer's disease, subjective cognitive impairment and limbic encephalitis. The AMI-CG showed good internal reliability, external validity and diagnostic accuracy. It also uncovered cases of social apathy overlooked by traditional instruments. Crucially, patients who under-rated their apathy compared to informants were more likely to have difficulties performing everyday activities and to be a greater burden to caregivers. The findings provide evidence for a multidimensional conceptualization of apathy and an instrument for efficient detection of apathy based on caregiver reports for use in clinical practice.


Assuntos
Apatia , Atividades Cotidianas , Apatia/fisiologia , Cuidadores/psicologia , Humanos , Motivação , Reprodutibilidade dos Testes
13.
Cortex ; 146: 186-199, 2022 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34894605

RESUMO

Mechanisms underlying visual imagery, the ability to create vivid mental representations of a scene in the absence of sensory input, remain to be fully understood. Some previous studies have proposed that visual imagery might be related to visual short-term memory (STM), with a common mechanism involving retention of visual information over short periods of time. Other observations have shown a strong relationship between visual imagery and functional activity in the hippocampus and primary visual cortex, both regions also associated with visual STM. Here we examined the relationship of visual imagery to STM and hippocampal and primary visual cortex volumes, first in a large sample of healthy people across a large age range (N = 229 behavioural data; N = 56 MRI data in older participants) and then in patients with Alzheimer's disease and Parkinson's disease (N = 19 in each group compared to 19 age-matched healthy controls). We used a variant of the "What was where?" visual object-location binding task to assess the quality of remembered information over short delays. In healthy people, no evidence of a relationship between the vividness of visual imagery and any visual STM performance parameter was found. However, there was a significant positive correlation between visual imagery and the volumes of the hippocampus and primary visual cortex. Although visual STM performance was significantly impaired in patients with Alzheimer's disease, their vividness of visual imagery scores were comparable to those of age-matched elderly controls and patients with Parkinson's disease. Despite hippocampal volumes also being reduced in Alzheimer's patients, there appeared to be no impact on their self-reported visual imagery. In conclusion, visual imagery was not significantly related to visual STM performance, either in healthy controls or Alzheimer's or Parkinson's disease but it was related to hippocampal and visual cortex volume in healthy people.


Assuntos
Memória de Curto Prazo , Córtex Visual Primário , Idoso , Humanos , Imagens, Psicoterapia , Imaginação , Inquéritos e Questionários
14.
Biochim Biophys Acta Biomembr ; 1864(11): 184010, 2022 11 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35870481

RESUMO

KCNQ1 (Kv7.1 or KvLQT1) is a voltage-gated potassium ion channel that is involved in the ventricular repolarization following an action potential in the heart. It forms a complex with KCNE1 in the heart and is the pore forming subunit of slow delayed rectifier potassium current (Iks). Mutations in KCNQ1, leading to a dysfunctional channel or loss of activity have been implicated in a cardiac disorder, long QT syndrome. In this study, we report the overexpression, purification, biochemical characterization of human KCNQ1100-370, and lipid bilayer dynamics upon interaction with KCNQ1100-370. The recombinant human KCNQ1 was expressed in Escherichia coli and purified into n-dodecylphosphocholine (DPC) micelles. The purified KCNQ1100-370 was biochemically characterized by SDS-PAGE electrophoresis, western blot and nano-LC-MS/MS to confirm the identity of the protein. Circular dichroism (CD) spectroscopy was utilized to confirm the secondary structure of purified protein in vesicles. Furthermore, 31P and 2H solid-state NMR spectroscopy in DPPC/POPC/POPG vesicles (MLVs) indicated a direct interaction between KCNQ100-370 and the phospholipid head groups. Finally, a visual inspection of KCNQ1100-370 incorporated into MLVs was confirmed by transmission electron microscopy (TEM). The findings of this study provide avenues for future structural studies of the human KCNQ1 ion channel to have an in depth understanding of its structure-function relationship.


Assuntos
Síndrome do QT Longo , Canais de Potássio de Abertura Dependente da Tensão da Membrana , Humanos , Canal de Potássio KCNQ1/metabolismo , Potássio/metabolismo , Canais de Potássio , Canais de Potássio de Abertura Dependente da Tensão da Membrana/metabolismo , Espectrometria de Massas em Tandem
15.
Brain Commun ; 3(2): fcab022, 2021.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33855297

RESUMO

Clinical apathy results in dysfunction of goal directed behaviour, a key component of which is the initiation of action. Previous work has suggested that blunting of reward sensitivity is an important mechanism underlying apathy. However, an additional component might be impoverished initiation of action itself. This study aims to investigate the link between motivation and motor output and its association with apathy and dopamine. An oculomotor task that measures pupillary and saccadic response to monetary incentives was used to assess reward sensitivity, first in 23 young and 18 elderly controls, and then in 22 patients with Parkinson's disease tested ON and OFF dopaminergic medication. To distinguish between pupillary responses to anticipated reward alone versus responses associated with motor preparation, a saccadic 'go/no-go' task was performed. Half of the trials required a saccade to be initiated to receive a reward and in the remaining trials no action was required but reward was still obtained. No significant difference in pupil response was demonstrated between the two conditions in all groups tested, suggesting pupillary responses to rewards are not contingent upon motor preparation in Parkinson's disease. Being ON or OFF dopamine did not influence this response either. Previous work demonstrated associations between apathy and pupillary reward insensitivity in Parkinson's disease. Here we observed this effect only when an action was required to receive a reward, and only in the ON state. These findings suggest that apathy in Parkinson's disease is linked to reduced reward sensitivity and that this is most prominently observed when actions have to be initiated to rewarding goals, with the effect modulated by being ON dopaminergic medication. OFF medication, there was no such strong relationship, and similarly in the 'no-go' conditions, either ON or OFF dopaminergic drugs. The results provide preliminary data which suggest that apathy in Parkinson's disease is associated with a reduction in reward sensitivity and this is most evident when associated with initiation of goal directed actions in the presence of adequate dopamine.

16.
Sci Rep ; 11(1): 4830, 2021 03 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33649399

RESUMO

Apathy and impulsivity are debilitating conditions associated with many neuropsychiatric conditions, and expressed to variable degrees in healthy people. While some theories suggest that they lie at different ends of a continuum, others suggest their possible co-existence. Surprisingly little is known, however, about their empirical association in the general population. Here, gathering data from six large studies ([Formula: see text]), we investigated the relationship between measures of apathy and impulsivity in young adults. The questionnaires included commonly used self-assessment tools-Apathy Evaluation Scale, Barratt Impulsiveness Scale (BIS-11) and UPPS-P Scale-as well as a more recent addition, the Apathy Motivation Index (AMI). Remarkably, across datasets and assessment tools, global measures of apathy and impulsivity correlated positively. However, analysis of sub-scale scores revealed a more complex relationship. Although most dimensions correlated positively with one another, there were two important exceptions revealed using the AMI scale. Social apathy was mostly negatively correlated with impulsive behaviour, and emotional apathy was orthogonal to all other sub-domains. These results suggest that at a global level, apathy and impulsivity do not exist at distinct ends of a continuum. Instead, paradoxically, they most often co-exist in young adults. Processes underlying social and emotional apathy, however, appear to be different and dissociable from behavioural apathy and impulsivity.


Assuntos
Apatia , Comportamento Impulsivo , Motivação , Comportamento Social , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade
17.
Nat Commun ; 12(1): 4440, 2021 07 21.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34290236

RESUMO

Reinforcement learning is a fundamental mechanism displayed by many species. However, adaptive behaviour depends not only on learning about actions and outcomes that affect ourselves, but also those that affect others. Using computational reinforcement learning models, we tested whether young (age 18-36) and older (age 60-80, total n = 152) adults learn to gain rewards for themselves, another person (prosocial), or neither individual (control). Detailed model comparison showed that a model with separate learning rates for each recipient best explained behaviour. Young adults learned faster when their actions benefitted themselves, compared to others. Compared to young adults, older adults showed reduced self-relevant learning rates but preserved prosocial learning. Moreover, levels of subclinical self-reported psychopathic traits (including lack of concern for others) were lower in older adults and the core affective-interpersonal component of this measure negatively correlated with prosocial learning. These findings suggest learning to benefit others is preserved across the lifespan with implications for reinforcement learning and theories of healthy ageing.


Assuntos
Envelhecimento/psicologia , Comportamento de Ajuda , Reforço Psicológico , Adolescente , Adulto , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Transtorno da Personalidade Antissocial/psicologia , Feminino , Humanos , Aprendizagem/fisiologia , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Modelos Psicológicos , Recompensa , Adulto Jovem
18.
Biochim Biophys Acta Biomembr ; 1863(12): 183771, 2021 12 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34499883

RESUMO

The bacteriophage infection cycle is terminated at a predefined time to release the progeny virions via a robust lytic system composed of holin, endolysin, and spanin proteins. Holin is the timekeeper of this process. Pinholin S21 is a prototype holin of phage Φ21, which determines the timing of host cell lysis through the coordinated efforts of pinholin and antipinholin. However, mutations in pinholin and antipinholin play a significant role in modulating the timing of lysis depending on adverse or favorable growth conditions. Earlier studies have shown that single point mutations of pinholin S21 alter the cell lysis timing, a proxy for pinholin function as lysis is also dependent on other lytic proteins. In this study, continuous wave electron paramagnetic resonance (CW-EPR) power saturation and double electron-electron resonance (DEER) spectroscopic techniques were used to directly probe the effects of mutations on the structure and conformational changes of pinholin S21 that correlate with pinholin function. DEER and CW-EPR power saturation data clearly demonstrate that increased hydrophilicity induced by residue mutations accelerate the externalization of antipinholin transmembrane domain 1 (TMD1), while increased hydrophobicity prevents the externalization of TMD1. This altered hydrophobicity is potentially accelerating or delaying the activation of pinholin S21. It was also found that mutations can influence intra- or intermolecular interactions in this system, which contribute to the activation of pinholin and modulate the cell lysis timing. This could be a novel approach to analyze the mutational effects on other holin systems, as well as any other membrane protein in which mutation directly leads to structural and conformational changes.


Assuntos
Bacteriófagos/genética , Endopeptidases/genética , Proteínas de Membrana/genética , Proteínas Virais/genética , Vírion/genética , Bacteriófagos/química , Transporte Biológico , Morte Celular/genética , Endopeptidases/química , Proteínas de Membrana/química , Mutação/genética , Proteínas Virais/química , Vírion/química
19.
Cortex ; 138: 24-37, 2021 05.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33677325

RESUMO

Human decisions are susceptible to biases, but establishing causal roles of brain areas has proved to be difficult. Here we studied decision biases in 17 people with unilateral medial prefrontal cortex damage and a rare patient with bilateral ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC) lesions. Participants learned to choose which of two options was most likely to win, and then bet money on the outcome. Thus, good performance required not only selecting the best option, but also the amount to bet. Healthy people were biased by their previous bet, as well as by the unchosen option's value. Unilateral medial prefrontal lesions reduced these biases, leading to more rational decisions. Bilateral vmPFC lesions resulted in more strategic betting, again with less bias from the previous trial, paradoxically improving performance overall. Together, the results suggest that vmPFC normally imposes contextual biases, which in healthy people may actually be suboptimal in some situations.


Assuntos
Tomada de Decisões , Jogo de Azar , Viés , Encéfalo , Humanos , Córtex Pré-Frontal/diagnóstico por imagem
20.
Cortex ; 132: 41-50, 2020 11.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32919108

RESUMO

It has recently been proposed that short-term memory (STM) binding deficits might be an important feature of Alzheimer's disease (AD), providing a potential avenue for earlier detection of this disorder. By contrast, work in Parkinson's disease (PD), using different tasks, has suggested that the STM impairment in this condition is characterised by increased random guessing, possibly due to fluctuating attention. In the present study, to establish whether a misbinding impairment is present in sporadic late-onset AD (LOAD) and increased guessing is a feature of PD, we compared the performance of these patient groups to two control populations: healthy age-matched controls and individuals with subjective cognitive impairment (SCI) with comparable recruitment history as patients. All participants performed a sensitive task of STM that required high resolution retention of object-location bindings. This paradigm also enabled us to explore the underlying sources of error contributing to impaired STM in patients with LOAD and PD using computational modelling of response error. Patients with LOAD performed significantly worse than other groups on this task. Importantly their impaired memory was associated with increased misbinding errors. This was in contrast to patients with PD who made significantly more guessing responses. These findings therefore provide additional support for the presence of two doubly dissociable signatures of STM deficit in AD and PD, with binding impairment in AD and increased random guessing characterising the STM deficit in PD. The task used to measure memory precision here provides an easy-to-administer assessment of STM that is sensitive to the different types of deficit in AD and PD and hence has the potential to inform clinical practice.


Assuntos
Doença de Alzheimer , Disfunção Cognitiva , Doença de Parkinson , Doença de Alzheimer/complicações , Humanos , Transtornos da Memória/etiologia , Memória de Curto Prazo , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Doença de Parkinson/complicações
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA