Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 17 de 17
Filtrar
1.
Hum Brain Mapp ; 40(15): 4457-4469, 2019 10 15.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31313467

RESUMO

As a person reads, the brain performs complex operations to create higher order semantic representations from individual words. While these steps are effortless for competent readers, we are only beginning to understand how the brain performs these actions. Here, we explore lexical semantics using magnetoencephalography (MEG) recordings of people reading adjective-noun phrases presented one word at a time. We track the neural representation of single word representations over time, through different brain regions. Our results reveal two novel findings: (a) a neural representation of the adjective is present during noun presentation, but this representation is different from that observed during adjective presentation and (b) the neural representation of adjective semantics observed during adjective reading is reactivated after phrase reading, with remarkable consistency. We also note that while the semantic representation of the adjective during the reading of the adjective is very distributed, the later representations are concentrated largely to temporal and frontal areas previously associated with composition. Taken together, these results paint a picture of information flow in the brain as phrases are read and understood.


Assuntos
Mapeamento Encefálico , Compreensão/fisiologia , Leitura , Semântica , Adulto , Córtex Cerebral/fisiologia , Feminino , Humanos , Magnetoencefalografia , Fatores de Tempo
2.
BMC Public Health ; 13: 940, 2013 Oct 08.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24103508

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: Mathematical and computational models provide valuable tools that help public health planners to evaluate competing health interventions, especially for novel circumstances that cannot be examined through observational or controlled studies, such as pandemic influenza. The spread of diseases like influenza depends on the mixing patterns within the population, and these mixing patterns depend in part on local factors including the spatial distribution and age structure of the population, the distribution of size and composition of households, employment status and commuting patterns of adults, and the size and age structure of schools. Finally, public health planners must take into account the health behavior patterns of the population, patterns that often vary according to socioeconomic factors such as race, household income, and education levels. RESULTS: FRED (a Framework for Reconstructing Epidemic Dynamics) is a freely available open-source agent-based modeling system based closely on models used in previously published studies of pandemic influenza. This version of FRED uses open-access census-based synthetic populations that capture the demographic and geographic heterogeneities of the population, including realistic household, school, and workplace social networks. FRED epidemic models are currently available for every state and county in the United States, and for selected international locations. CONCLUSIONS: State and county public health planners can use FRED to explore the effects of possible influenza epidemics in specific geographic regions of interest and to help evaluate the effect of interventions such as vaccination programs and school closure policies. FRED is available under a free open source license in order to contribute to the development of better modeling tools and to encourage open discussion of modeling tools being used to evaluate public health policies. We also welcome participation by other researchers in the further development of FRED.


Assuntos
Controle de Doenças Transmissíveis/métodos , Simulação por Computador , Influenza Humana/epidemiologia , Influenza Humana/transmissão , Modelos Teóricos , Software , Adolescente , Adulto , Idoso , Censos , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Estados Unidos , Adulto Jovem
3.
Neuroimage ; 62(1): 451-63, 2012 Aug 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22565201

RESUMO

We present a methodological approach employing magnetoencephalography (MEG) and machine learning techniques to investigate the flow of perceptual and semantic information decodable from neural activity in the half second during which the brain comprehends the meaning of a concrete noun. Important information about the cortical location of neural activity related to the representation of nouns in the human brain has been revealed by past studies using fMRI. However, the temporal sequence of processing from sensory input to concept comprehension remains unclear, in part because of the poor time resolution provided by fMRI. In this study, subjects answered 20 questions (e.g. is it alive?) about the properties of 60 different nouns prompted by simultaneous presentation of a pictured item and its written name. Our results show that the neural activity observed with MEG encodes a variety of perceptual and semantic features of stimuli at different times relative to stimulus onset, and in different cortical locations. By decoding these features, our MEG-based classifier was able to reliably distinguish between two different concrete nouns that it had never seen before. The results demonstrate that there are clear differences between the time course of the magnitude of MEG activity and that of decodable semantic information. Perceptual features were decoded from MEG activity earlier in time than semantic features, and features related to animacy, size, and manipulability were decoded consistently across subjects. We also observed that regions commonly associated with semantic processing in the fMRI literature may not show high decoding results in MEG. We believe that this type of approach and the accompanying machine learning methods can form the basis for further modeling of the flow of neural information during language processing and a variety of other cognitive processes.


Assuntos
Mapeamento Encefálico/métodos , Compreensão/fisiologia , Magnetoencefalografia/métodos , Rede Nervosa/fisiologia , Semântica , Percepção Visual/fisiologia , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Adulto Jovem
4.
Front Psychol ; 13: 894405, 2022.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35693512

RESUMO

Rhythm is key to language acquisition. Across languages, rhythmic features highlight fundamental linguistic elements of the sound stream and structural relations among them. A sensitivity to rhythmic features, which begins in utero, is evident at birth. What is less clear is whether rhythm supports infants' earliest links between language and cognition. Prior evidence has documented that for infants as young as 3 and 4 months, listening to their native language (English) supports the core cognitive capacity of object categorization. This precocious link is initially part of a broader template: listening to a non-native language from the same rhythmic class as (e.g., German, but not Cantonese) and to vocalizations of non-human primates (e.g., lemur, Eulemur macaco flavifrons, but not birds e.g., zebra-finches, Taeniopygia guttata) provide English-acquiring infants the same cognitive advantage as does listening to their native language. Here, we implement a machine-learning (ML) approach to ask whether there are acoustic properties, available on the surface of these vocalizations, that permit infants' to identify which vocalizations are candidate links to cognition. We provided the model with a robust sample of vocalizations that, from the vantage point of English-acquiring 4-month-olds, either support object categorization (English, German, lemur vocalizations) or fail to do so (Cantonese, zebra-finch vocalizations). We assess (a) whether supervised ML classification models can distinguish those vocalizations that support cognition from those that do not, and (b) which class(es) of acoustic features (including rhythmic, spectral envelope, and pitch features) best support that classification. Our analysis reveals that principal components derived from rhythm-relevant acoustic features were among the most robust in supporting the classification. Classifications performed using temporal envelope components were also robust. These new findings provide in principle evidence that infants' earliest links between vocalizations and cognition may be subserved by their perceptual sensitivity to rhythmic and spectral elements available on the surface of these vocalizations, and that these may guide infants' identification of candidate links to cognition.

5.
Neural Netw ; 137: 63-74, 2021 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33556802

RESUMO

As deep neural net architectures minimize loss, they accumulate information in a hierarchy of learned representations that ultimately serve the network's final goal. Different architectures tackle this problem in slightly different ways, but all create intermediate representational spaces built to inform their final prediction. Here we show that very different neural networks trained on two very different tasks build knowledge representations that display similar underlying patterns. Namely, we show that the representational spaces of several distributional semantic models bear a remarkable resemblance to several Convolutional Neural Network (CNN) architectures (trained for image classification). We use this information to explore the network behavior of CNNs (1) in pretrained models, (2) during training, and (3) during adversarial attacks. We use these findings to motivate several applications aimed at improving future research on CNNs. Our work illustrates the power of using one model to explore another, gives new insights into the function of CNN models, and provides a framework for others to perform similar analyses when developing new architectures. We show that one neural network model can provide a window into understanding another.


Assuntos
Aprendizado de Máquina , Web Semântica , Segurança Computacional , Semântica
6.
Brain Behav ; 11(8): e2234, 2021 08.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34129727

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: As we learn a new nonnative language (L2), we begin to build a new map of concepts onto orthographic representations. Eventually, L2 can conjure as rich a semantic representation as our native language (L1). However, the neural processes for mapping a new orthographic representation to a familiar meaning are not well understood or characterized. METHODS: Using electroencephalography and an artificial language that maps symbols to English words, we show that it is possible to use machine learning models to detect a newly formed semantic mapping as it is acquired. RESULTS: Through a trial-by-trial analysis, we show that we can detect when a new semantic mapping is formed. Our results show that, like word meaning representations evoked by a L1, the localization of the newly formed neural representations is highly distributed, but the representation may emerge more slowly after the onset of the symbol. Furthermore, our mapping of word meanings to symbols removes the confound of the semantics to the visual characteristics of the stimulus, a confound that has been difficult to disentangle previously. CONCLUSION: We have shown that the L1 semantic representation conjured by a newly acquired L2 word can be detected using decoding techniques, and we give the first characterization of the emergence of that mapping. Our work opens up new possibilities for the study of semantic representations during L2 learning.


Assuntos
Multilinguismo , Semântica , Eletroencefalografia , Humanos , Idioma , Desenvolvimento da Linguagem
7.
PLoS One ; 16(3): e0242754, 2021.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33661954

RESUMO

In language, stored semantic representations of lexical items combine into an infinitude of complex expressions. While the neuroscience of composition has begun to mature, we do not yet understand how the stored representations evolve and morph during composition. New decoding techniques allow us to crack open this very hard question: we can train a model to recognize a representation in one context or time-point and assess its accuracy in another. We combined the decoding approach with magnetoencephalography recorded during a picture naming task to investigate the temporal evolution of noun and adjective representations during speech planning. We tracked semantic representations as they combined into simple two-word phrases, using single words and two-word lists as non-combinatory controls. We found that nouns were generally more decodable than adjectives, suggesting that noun representations were stronger and/or more consistent across trials than those of adjectives. When training and testing across contexts and times, the representations of isolated nouns were recoverable when those nouns were embedded in phrases, but not so if they were embedded in lists. Adjective representations did not show a similar consistency across isolated and phrasal contexts. Noun representations in phrases also sustained over time in a way that was not observed for any other pairing of word class and context. These findings offer a new window into the temporal evolution and context sensitivity of word representations during composition, revealing a clear asymmetry between adjectives and nouns. The impact of phrasal contexts on the decodability of nouns may be due to the nouns' status as head of phrase-an intriguing hypothesis for future research.


Assuntos
Encéfalo/fisiologia , Semântica , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Magnetoencefalografia , Masculino , Psicolinguística
8.
Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci ; 375(1791): 20180531, 2020 02 03.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31840577

RESUMO

The temporal generalization method (TGM) is a data analysis technique that can be used to test if the brain's representation for particular stimuli (e.g. sounds, images) is maintained, or if it changes as a function of time (King J-R, Dehaene S. 2014 Characterizing the dynamics of mental representations: the temporal generalization method. Trends Cogn. Sci.18, 203-210. (doi:10.1016/j.tics.2014.01.002)). The TGM involves training models to predict the stimuli or condition using a time window from a recording of brain activity, and testing the resulting models at all possible time windows. This is repeated for all possible training windows to create a full matrix of accuracy for every combination of train/test window. The results of a TGM indicate when brain activity patterns are consistent (i.e. the trained model performs well even when tested on a different time window), and when they are inconsistent, allowing us to track neural representations over time. The TGM has been used to study the representation of images and sounds during a variety of tasks, but has been less readily applied to studies of language. Here, we give an overview of the method itself, discuss how the TGM has been used to analyse two studies of language in context and explore how the TGM could be applied to further our understanding of semantic composition. This article is part of the theme issue 'Towards mechanistic models of meaning composition'.


Assuntos
Encéfalo/fisiologia , Idioma , Animais , Compreensão , Potenciais Evocados , Previsões , Humanos , Magnetoencefalografia , Semântica , Lobo Temporal/fisiologia
9.
Neural Netw ; 131: 103-114, 2020 Nov.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32771841

RESUMO

The current state-of-the-art object recognition algorithms, deep convolutional neural networks (DCNNs), are inspired by the architecture of the mammalian visual system, and are capable of human-level performance on many tasks. As they are trained for object recognition tasks, it has been shown that DCNNs develop hidden representations that resemble those observed in the mammalian visual system (Razavi and Kriegeskorte, 2014; Yamins and Dicarlo, 2016; Gu and van Gerven, 2015; Mcclure and Kriegeskorte, 2016). Moreover, DCNNs trained on object recognition tasks are currently among the best models we have of the mammalian visual system. This led us to hypothesize that teaching DCNNs to achieve even more brain-like representations could improve their performance. To test this, we trained DCNNs on a composite task, wherein networks were trained to: (a) classify images of objects; while (b) having intermediate representations that resemble those observed in neural recordings from monkey visual cortex. Compared with DCNNs trained purely for object categorization, DCNNs trained on the composite task had better object recognition performance and are more robust to label corruption. Interestingly, we found that neural data was not required for this process, but randomized data with the same statistical properties as neural data also boosted performance. While the performance gains we observed when training on the composite task vs the "pure" object recognition task were modest, they were remarkably robust. Notably, we observed these performance gains across all network variations we studied, including: smaller (CORNet-Z) vs larger (VGG-16) architectures; variations in optimizers (Adam vs gradient descent); variations in activation function (ReLU vs ELU); and variations in network initialization. Our results demonstrate the potential utility of a new approach to training object recognition networks, using strategies in which the brain - or at least the statistical properties of its activation patterns - serves as a teacher signal for training DCNNs.


Assuntos
Modelos Neurológicos , Redes Neurais de Computação , Reconhecimento Automatizado de Padrão/métodos , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos , Animais , Haplorrinos , Reconhecimento Automatizado de Padrão/normas , Córtex Visual/fisiologia
10.
Int J Popul Data Sci ; 5(4): 1716, 2020.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35516163

RESUMO

Introduction: The COVID-19 pandemic had clear impacts on mental health. Social media presents an opportunity for assessing mental health at the population level. Objectives: 1) Identify and describe language used on social media that is associated with discourse about depression. 2) Describe the associations between identified language and COVID-19 incidence over time across several geographies. Methods: We create a word embedding based on the posts in Reddit's /r/Depression and use this word embedding to train representations of active authors. We contrast these authors against a control group and extract keywords that capture differences between the two groups. We filter these keywords for face validity and to match character limits of an information retrieval system, Elasticsearch. We retrieve all geo-tagged posts on Twitter from April 2019 to June 2021 from Seattle, Sydney, Mumbai, and Toronto. The tweets are scored with BM25 using the keywords. We call this score rDD. We compare changes in average score over time with case counts from the pandemic's beginning through June 2021. Results: We observe a pattern in rDD across all cities analyzed: There is an increase in rDD near the start of the pandemic which levels off over time. However, in Mumbai we also see an increase aligned with a second wave of cases. Conclusions: Our results are concordant with other studies which indicate that the impact of the pandemic on mental health was highest initially and was followed by recovery, largely unchanged by subsequent waves. However, in the Mumbai data we observed a substantial rise in rDD with a large second wave. Our results indicate possible un-captured heterogeneity across geographies, and point to a need for a better understanding of this differential impact on mental health.

11.
Bioinformatics ; 24(21): 2512-7, 2008 Nov 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18728042

RESUMO

MOTIVATION: Each protein performs its functions within some specific locations in a cell. This subcellular location is important for understanding protein function and for facilitating its purification. There are now many computational techniques for predicting location based on sequence analysis and database information from homologs. A few recent techniques use text from biological abstracts: our goal is to improve the prediction accuracy of such text-based techniques. We identify three techniques for improving text-based prediction: a rule for ambiguous abstract removal, a mechanism for using synonyms from the Gene Ontology (GO) and a mechanism for using the GO hierarchy to generalize terms. We show that these three techniques can significantly improve the accuracy of protein subcellular location predictors that use text extracted from PubMed abstracts whose references are recorded in Swiss-Prot.


Assuntos
Biologia Computacional/métodos , Publicações Periódicas como Assunto , Proteínas/análise , Software , Vocabulário Controlado , Indexação e Redação de Resumos , Classificação/métodos , Bases de Dados de Proteínas , Genes , Proteínas/química , Proteínas/genética , PubMed
12.
Nucleic Acids Res ; 33(Database issue): D147-53, 2005 Jan 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15608166

RESUMO

PA-GOSUB (Proteome Analyst: Gene Ontology Molecular Function and Subcellular Localization) is a publicly available, web-based, searchable and downloadable database that contains the sequences, predicted GO molecular functions and predicted subcellular localizations of more than 107,000 proteins from 10 model organisms (and growing), covering the major kingdoms and phyla for which annotated proteomes exist (http://www.cs.ualberta.ca/~bioinfo/PA/GOSUB). The PA-GOSUB database effectively expands the coverage of subcellular localization and GO function annotations by a significant factor (already over five for subcellular localization, compared with Swiss-Prot v42.7), and more model organisms are being added to PA-GOSUB as their sequenced proteomes become available. PA-GOSUB can be used in three main ways. First, a researcher can browse the pre-computed PA-GOSUB annotations on a per-organism and per-protein basis using annotation-based and text-based filters. Second, a user can perform BLAST searches against the PA-GOSUB database and use the annotations from the homologs as simple predictors for the new sequences. Third, the whole of PA-GOSUB can be downloaded in either FASTA or comma-separated values (CSV) formats.


Assuntos
Bases de Dados de Proteínas , Proteínas/química , Proteômica , Sequência de Aminoácidos , Animais , Inteligência Artificial , Humanos , Camundongos , Modelos Animais , Proteínas/análise , Proteínas/genética , Proteínas/fisiologia , Homologia de Sequência de Aminoácidos
13.
Nucleic Acids Res ; 32(Web Server issue): W365-71, 2004 Jul 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15215412

RESUMO

Proteome Analyst (PA) (http://www.cs.ualberta.ca/~bioinfo/PA/) is a publicly available, high-throughput, web-based system for predicting various properties of each protein in an entire proteome. Using machine-learned classifiers, PA can predict, for example, the GeneQuiz general function and Gene Ontology (GO) molecular function of a protein. In addition, PA is currently the most accurate and most comprehensive system for predicting subcellular localization, the location within a cell where a protein performs its main function. Two other capabilities of PA are notable. First, PA can create a custom classifier to predict a new property, without requiring any programming, based on labeled training data (i.e. a set of examples, each with the correct classification label) provided by a user. PA has been used to create custom classifiers for potassium-ion channel proteins and other general function ontologies. Second, PA provides a sophisticated explanation feature that shows why one prediction is chosen over another. The PA system produces a Naïve Bayes classifier, which is amenable to a graphical and interactive approach to explanations for its predictions; transparent predictions increase the user's confidence in, and understanding of, PA.


Assuntos
Proteoma/química , Proteômica , Software , Internet , Proteínas/classificação , Proteínas/fisiologia , Reprodutibilidade dos Testes , Análise de Sequência de Proteína
14.
Proc Conf Assoc Comput Linguist Meet ; 2014: 489-499, 2014 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26166940

RESUMO

Vector space models (VSMs) represent word meanings as points in a high dimensional space. VSMs are typically created using a large text corpora, and so represent word semantics as observed in text. We present a new algorithm (JNNSE) that can incorporate a measure of semantics not previously used to create VSMs: brain activation data recorded while people read words. The resulting model takes advantage of the complementary strengths and weaknesses of corpus and brain activation data to give a more complete representation of semantics. Evaluations show that the model 1) matches a behavioral measure of semantics more closely, 2) can be used to predict corpus data for unseen words and 3) has predictive power that generalizes across brain imaging technologies and across subjects. We believe that the model is thus a more faithful representation of mental vocabularies.

15.
PLoS One ; 9(11): e112575, 2014.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25426840

RESUMO

Story understanding involves many perceptual and cognitive subprocesses, from perceiving individual words, to parsing sentences, to understanding the relationships among the story characters. We present an integrated computational model of reading that incorporates these and additional subprocesses, simultaneously discovering their fMRI signatures. Our model predicts the fMRI activity associated with reading arbitrary text passages, well enough to distinguish which of two story segments is being read with 74% accuracy. This approach is the first to simultaneously track diverse reading subprocesses during complex story processing and predict the detailed neural representation of diverse story features, ranging from visual word properties to the mention of different story characters and different actions they perform. We construct brain representation maps that replicate many results from a wide range of classical studies that focus each on one aspect of language processing and offer new insights on which type of information is processed by different areas involved in language processing. Additionally, this approach is promising for studying individual differences: it can be used to create single subject maps that may potentially be used to measure reading comprehension and diagnose reading disorders.


Assuntos
Encéfalo/fisiologia , Compreensão/fisiologia , Rememoração Mental/fisiologia , Leitura , Adolescente , Adulto , Atenção , Encéfalo/anatomia & histologia , Mapeamento Encefálico , Feminino , Humanos , Processamento de Imagem Assistida por Computador , Idioma , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética , Masculino
16.
Big Data ; 2(4): 216-29, 2014 Dec.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27442756

RESUMO

Given a simple noun such as apple, and a question such as "Is it edible?," what processes take place in the human brain? More specifically, given the stimulus, what are the interactions between (groups of) neurons (also known as functional connectivity) and how can we automatically infer those interactions, given measurements of the brain activity? Furthermore, how does this connectivity differ across different human subjects? In this work, we show that this problem, even though originating from the field of neuroscience, can benefit from big data techniques; we present a simple, novel good-enough brain model, or GeBM in short, and a novel algorithm Sparse-SysId, which are able to effectively model the dynamics of the neuron interactions and infer the functional connectivity. Moreover, GeBM is able to simulate basic psychological phenomena such as habituation and priming (whose definition we provide in the main text). We evaluate GeBM by using real brain data. GeBM produces brain activity patterns that are strikingly similar to the real ones, where the inferred functional connectivity is able to provide neuroscientific insights toward a better understanding of the way that neurons interact with each other, as well as detect regularities and outliers in multisubject brain activity measurements.

17.
Science ; 361(6402): 618, 2018 Aug 10.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30093600
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA