RESUMO
Circle theory predicts most offenders will reside within the smallest circle that encloses a pattern of offense locations, which defines the offender's crime range. Those who reside within the crime range reflect a marauder decision pattern, with the remainder reflecting a commuter decision pattern. Random simulations of marauders were conducted to determine the failure rate of the smallest circle to correctly identify a marauder decision pattern. Failure rates were higher as the number of offenses was reduced, but were systematic and predictable. Moreover, the published rates of the marauder vs commuter patterns correspond well with the predicted failure rate. A method for estimating the true size of an offender's criminal territory is presented and it is suggested that the vast majority of offenders are likely to be adopting a marauder decision pattern, and that true commuters may be far less than indicated by the circle test.
RESUMO
Musicians undergo extensive training which enhances established neural links between auditory and motor areas of the brain. Long-term training develops, strengthens and enables flexibility in these connections allowing proficiency in performance. Previous research has indicated that passive listening of trained music results in the recruitment of premotor areas. It has been argued that this sound-action representation may rely on activity in mirror neuron systems and that these systems are heavily dependent on actual sensorimotor experience. Action observation studies using electroencephalography have associated changes in mu rhythm activity with the mirror neuron system in the visuomotor domain. We aimed to investigate similar effects in the audiomotor domain. We utilised a mu suppression method in our action-listening study to detect involuntary motor coactivation when pianists passively listened to piano melodies. Wavelet analysis revealed sensorimotor mu rhythm suppression while pianists listened passively to piano melodies. Thus, we show that this spectral analysis method can also be used to demonstrate that auditory stimuli can activate the human mirror neuron system when sounds are linked to actions. Mu suppression could be a useful index for further research on action representation and training-induced plasticity.
Assuntos
Percepção Auditiva/fisiologia , Ondas Encefálicas/fisiologia , Sincronização de Fases em Eletroencefalografia/fisiologia , Atividade Motora/fisiologia , Música , Córtex Sensório-Motor/fisiologia , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Adulto JovemRESUMO
When deciding if a rotated object would face to the left or to the right, if imagined at the upright, mental rotation is typically assumed to be carried out through the shortest angular distance to the upright prior to determining the direction of facing. However, the response time functions for left- and right-facing objects are oppositely asymmetric, which is not consistent with the standard explanation. Using Searle and Hamm's individual differences adaption of Kung and Hamm's Mixture Model, the current study compares the predicted response time functions derived when assuming that objects are rotated through the shortest route to the upright with the predicted response time functions derived when assuming that objects are rotated in the direction they face. The latter model provides a better fit to the majority of the individual data. This allows us to conclude that, when deciding if rotated objects would face to the left or to the right if imagined at the upright, mental rotation is carried out in the direction that the objects face and not necessarily in the shortest direction to the upright. By comparing results for mobile and immobile object sets we can also conclude that semantic information regarding the mobility of an object does not appear to influence the speed of mental rotation, but it does appear to influence pre-rotation processes and the likelihood of employing a mental rotation strategy.
Assuntos
Imaginação , Individualidade , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos/fisiologia , Tempo de Reação/fisiologia , Percepção Espacial/fisiologia , Adolescente , Adulto , Humanos , Rotação , Adulto JovemRESUMO
A growing body of literature has explored the influence of physical activity on brain structure and function. While the mechanisms of this relationship remain largely speculative, recent research suggests that one of the effects of physical exercise is an increase in synaptic long-term potentiation (LTP). This has not yet been explored directly in humans due to the difficulty of measuring LTP non-invasively. However, we have previously established that LTP-like changes in visual-evoked potentials (VEPs) can be measured in humans. Here, we investigated whether physical fitness status affects the degree of visual sensory LTP. Using a self-report measure of physical activity, participants were split into two groups: a high-activity group, and a low-activity group. LTP was measured and compared between the two groups using the previously established electroencephalography-LTP paradigm, which assesses the degree to which the N1b component of the VEP elicited by a sine grating is potentiated (enhanced) following a rapid "tetanic" presentation of that grating. Both groups demonstrated increased negativity in the amplitude of the N1b component of the VEP immediately after presentation of the visual "tetanus," indicating potentiation. However, after a 30-min rest period, the N1b for the high-activity group remained potentiated while the N1b for the low-activity group returned to baseline. This study presents the first evidence for the impact of self-reported levels of physical activity on LTP in humans, and sheds light on potential neurological mechanisms underlying the relationship between physical fitness and cognition.
Assuntos
Encéfalo/fisiologia , Potenciais Evocados Visuais/fisiologia , Potenciação de Longa Duração/fisiologia , Atividade Motora/fisiologia , Adulto , Análise de Variância , Mapeamento Encefálico , Eletroencefalografia , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Estimulação Luminosa , Inquéritos e Questionários , Adulto JovemRESUMO
Automatic attentional capture by a salient distractor can be prevented by spatial attentional control settings (ACSs) (e.g., Yantis and Jonides in J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 16:121-134, 1990). Earlier, converging evidence for a spatial ACS (Eason et al. 1969) was found in event-related potentials (ERPs). In these studies, the ACS was defined by a single target-relevant location. In an extension, Ishigami et al. (Vis Cogn 17:431-456, 2009) demonstrated a successful ACS in performance that was based on multiple (two) target-relevant locations. The purpose of the current study is to seek converging evidence from ERPs for a spatial ACS defined by multiple (two) target-relevant locations, using the methods in Ishigami et al. (Vis Cogn 17:431-456, 2009). Any one of four figure-8s brightened uninformatively (cue) before presentation of a digit target calling for a speeded identification (2 or 5). A spatial ACS was encouraged because in different blocks, the digit targets appeared only on the horizontal or vertical axis. Performance was more impaired following the invalid-attended cues than following invalid-unattended cues, consistent with Ishigami et al. (Vis Cogn 17:431-456, 2009) and verifying a successful spatial ACS. The direction of attention significantly affected the visual evoked potentials (VEPs) elicited by otherwise identical cues: the amplitudes of early VEPs were greater when the location the cue was presented in was target-relevant than when the location was target-irrelevant. These results re-affirm that attentional capture by irrelevant salient stimuli can be modulated by spatial ACSs defined by multiple target locations in performance and provide converging evidence from ERPs for the previously established behavioral findings.
Assuntos
Atenção/fisiologia , Sinais (Psicologia) , Potenciais Evocados Visuais/fisiologia , Estimulação Luminosa/métodos , Percepção Espacial/fisiologia , Adolescente , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Adulto JovemRESUMO
Mental rotation is thought to underlie the increase in response times (RTs) for deciding whether rotated letters are normal or mirrored versions. However, mental rotation predicts a linear increase in RTs, whereas the mirror/normal letter discrimination task typically produces a curved function. Recently, Kung and Hamm suggested that this curved function results from a mixture of trials in which mental rotation is employed and trials in which it is not. The mixture ratio may vary between individuals, with some individuals relying more on mental rotation than others. There is no factor in the Kung and Hamm model that reflects such individual differences. In the present study, we suggest that a possible exponent parameter could be added to the Kung and Hamm model to capture individual differences in the mixture ratio. This exponent parameter appears to capture an individual characteristic since the value obtained correlates between the mirror/normal letter task and a left/right object facing task. The development of a quantity that represents the mixture ratio will aid further testing of processes involved in the visual imagery system.
Assuntos
Discriminação Psicológica/fisiologia , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos/fisiologia , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Individualidade , Masculino , Modelos Teóricos , Testes Psicológicos , Rotação , Adulto JovemRESUMO
The degree to which cognitive resources are shared in the processing of musical pitch and lexical tones remains uncertain. Testing Mandarin amusics on their categorical perception of Mandarin lexical tones may provide insight into this issue. In the present study, a group of 15 amusic Mandarin speakers identified and discriminated Mandarin tones presented as continua in separate blocks. The tonal continua employed were from a high-level tone to a mid-rising tone and from a high-level tone to a high-falling tone. The two tonal continua were made in the contexts of natural speech and of nonlinguistic analogues. In contrast to the controls, the participants with amusia showed no improvement for discrimination pairs that crossed the classification boundary for either speech or nonlinguistic analogues, indicating a lack of categorical perception. The lack of categorical perception of Mandarin tones in the amusic group shows that the pitch deficits in amusics may be domain-general, and this suggests that the processing of musical pitch and lexical tones may share certain cognitive resources and/or processes (Patel 2003, 2008, 2012).
Assuntos
Transtornos da Percepção Auditiva/fisiopatologia , Percepção da Altura Sonora/fisiologia , Percepção da Fala/fisiologia , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Discriminação da Altura Tonal/fisiologia , Adulto JovemRESUMO
This study embedded attentional cues in the study phase of an item-method directed forgetting task. We used an unpredictive onset cue (Experiment 1), a predictive onset cue (Experiment 2), or a predictive central cue (Experiments 3-6) to direct attention to the left or right. In Experiments 1-5, this was followed by a pink or blue study word that required a speeded colour discrimination; in Experiment 6, it was followed by a pink or blue word or nonword that required a lexical decision. Each study word was followed by an instruction to Remember or Forget. A yes-no recognition test confirmed better recognition of to-be-remembered words than to-be-forgotten words; a cueing effect confirmed the effectiveness of predictive cues in allocating attentional resources. There was, however, no evidence that the directed forgetting effect differed for attended and unattended words: Encoding depends more on the memory intention formed after a study word has disappeared than on the availability of processing resources when that word first appears.
Assuntos
Intenção , Rememoração Mental , Sinais (Psicologia) , Humanos , Memória , Reconhecimento PsicológicoRESUMO
BACKGROUND: Functional imaging studies of people with focal hand dystonia (FHD) have indicated abnormal activity in sensorimotor brain regions. Few studies however, have examined FHD during movements that do not provoke symptoms of the disorder. It is possible, therefore, that any differences between FHD and controls are confounded by activity due to the occurrence of symptoms. Thus, in order to characterise impairments in patients with FHD during movements that do not induce dystonic symptoms, we investigated the neural correlates of externally paced finger tapping movements. METHODS: Functional MRI (fMRI) was used to compare patients with FHD to controls with respect to activation in networks modulated by task complexity and hand used to perform simple and complex tapping movements. RESULTS: In the 'complexity network,' patients with FHD showed significantly less activity relative to controls in posterior parietal cortex, medial supplementary motor area (SMA), anterior putamen and cerebellum. In the 'hand network,' patients with FHD showed less activation than controls in primary motor (M1) and somatosensory (S1) cortices, SMA and cerebellum. Conjunction analysis revealed that patients with FHD demonstrated reduced activation in the majority of combined network regions (M1, S1 and cerebellum). CONCLUSION: Dysfunction in FHD is widespread in both complexity and hand networks, and impairments are demonstrated even when performing tasks that do not evoke dystonic symptoms. These results suggest that such impairments are inherent to, rather than symptomatic of, the disorder.
Assuntos
Distonia/fisiopatologia , Potenciais Somatossensoriais Evocados/fisiologia , Mãos/fisiopatologia , Transtornos Psicomotores/fisiopatologia , Córtex Somatossensorial/fisiopatologia , Adulto , Toxinas Botulínicas Tipo A/uso terapêutico , Distonia/tratamento farmacológico , Feminino , Lateralidade Funcional/fisiologia , Humanos , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Fármacos Neuromusculares/uso terapêutico , Índice de Gravidade de DoençaRESUMO
Rotated mirror/normal letter discriminations are thought to require mental rotation in order to determine the direction of facing of the stimulus. The response time (RT) function over orientation tends to be curved, rather than the linear function found for other mental rotation tasks. The present study investigated the possibility that the curved RT function is a result of a mixture of trials requiring and not requiring mental rotation. The results suggested that the frequency of mental rotation is also a linear function of stimulus orientation. Moreover, the relationship between an individual's rate of plane rotation and the mean difference in RT between mirror and normal stimuli was replicated, supporting the suggestion that mirrored stimuli are flipped after they are spun (Hamm, Johnson, & Corballis, 2004). On the basis of the present findings, the entire RT function can be modeled by using only the mean RTs for upright and inverted stimuli.
Assuntos
Aprendizagem por Discriminação , Linguística , Rotação , Percepção Visual , Adolescente , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Tempo de Reação , Adulto JovemRESUMO
OBJECTIVE: To examine whether or not university mathematics students semantically process gestures depicting mathematical functions (mathematical gestures) similarly to the way they process action gestures and sentences. Semantic processing was indexed by the N400 effect. RESULTS: The N400 effect elicited by words primed with mathematical gestures (e.g. "converging" and "decreasing") was the same in amplitude, latency and topography as that elicited by words primed with action gestures (e.g. drive and lift), and that for terminal words of sentences. SIGNIFICANCE AND CONCLUSION: Findings provide a within-subject demonstration that the topographies of the gesture N400 effect for both action and mathematical words are indistinguishable from that of the standard language N400 effect. This suggests that mathematical function words are processed by the general language semantic system and do not appear to involve areas involved in other mathematical concepts (e.g. numerosity).
Assuntos
Encéfalo/fisiologia , Potenciais Evocados/fisiologia , Gestos , Matemática , Estimulação Acústica , Análise de Variância , Distribuição de Qui-Quadrado , Eletroencefalografia , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Estimulação Luminosa , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Tempo de Reação/fisiologia , Processamento de Sinais Assistido por Computador , Percepção da Fala/fisiologia , Percepção Visual/fisiologia , Adulto JovemRESUMO
The flanker task developed by B. A. Eriksen and C. W. Eriksen (1974) is a reliable method of showing the effect of flanking stimuli (flankers) on responses to central targets. When flankers map to the same or different responses as the targets, response latencies reduce or increase, respectively, relative to neutral flankers. In Experiment 1, the authors present 2-dimensional line drawings of structurally distinct animals: 2 birds, 2 bugs, and 2 dogs. These pairs generated a reliable flanker compatibility effect (FCE) for both upright and inverted flankers. In Experiment 2, the authors present pairs of structurally similar animals: 2 dogs, 2 cows, and 2 horses. These produced a reliable FCE for only upright flankers. The results are discussed in line with structural categorisation of stimuli being invariant of object orientation.
Assuntos
Atenção , Comportamento de Escolha , Aprendizagem por Discriminação , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos , Desempenho Psicomotor , Adolescente , Adulto , Atenção/fisiologia , Comportamento de Escolha/fisiologia , Aprendizagem por Discriminação/fisiologia , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos/fisiologia , Estimulação Luminosa/métodos , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Tempo de Reação/fisiologia , Adulto JovemRESUMO
Illusory line motion (ILM) refers to perceived motion in a bar when it is presented all at once. Explanations for ILM include low-level visual accounts, visual attention, and object tracking. These explanations tend to arise from studies using different protocols to induce ILM, based on the assumption that the same illusion is being generated. Using real motion in the same and in the opposite direction as the ILM quantifies the illusions from all protocols as the area between response curves for the left- and right-side inducers. This common measure enables testing of the assumption that two display configurations result in the same illusion. If there is a common underlying cause, an individual who shows a strong illusion in one situation should show a strong illusion in the other, but illusions that arise through different systems should not correlate. This approach has differentiated ILM induced by a flash (flashILM) from ILM induced by matching the bar to an attribute of the inducing stimuli (transformational apparent motion, TAM). The former is thought to reflect attention, while the latter is thought to reflect object processing. Low-level visual explanations are often offered based on ILM that occurs when the bar is adjacent to only a single inducer (polarized gamma motion, PGM) rather than between two stimuli (flashILM and TAM). The present study replicates the independence of flashILM and TAM and shows that neither is related to PGM, suggesting that all three explanations for ILM are warranted and that the debates in the literature are conflating at least three different illusions.
Assuntos
Atenção/fisiologia , Percepção de Movimento/fisiologia , Ilusões Ópticas/fisiologia , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Estimulação Luminosa/métodos , Adulto JovemRESUMO
Illusory line motion (ILM) refers to the perception of motion in a line that is, in fact, presented in full at one time. One form of this illusion (flashILM) occurs when the line is presented between two objects following a brief luminance change in one of them and flashILM is thought to result from exogenous attention being captured by the flash. Exogenous attention fades with increasing delays, which predicts that flashILM should show a similar temporal pattern. Exogenous attention appears to follow flashILM to become more or less equally distributed along the line.The current study examines flashILM in order to test these predictions derived from the attentional explanation for flashILM and the results were consistent with them. The discussion then concludes with an exploratory analysis approach concerning states of consciousness and decision making and suggests a possible role for attention.
RESUMO
In an item-method directed forgetting task, attentional resources are withdrawn from forget item processing (e.g., Taylor & Fawcett in Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 73, 1790-1814, 2011). Taylor and Hamm (Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 78, 168-186, 2016) demonstrated that there is no corresponding increase in the proclivity for exogenous attention to be captured following a forget instruction. This means either that the attentional resources withdrawn from the forget item are reallocated immediately (and therefore not especially vulnerable to capture) or that it is not exogenous attention that is withdrawn. Given that endogenous attention is distinct from exogenous attention, we therefore extended the Taylor and Hamm study by using endogenous orienting rather than exogenous orienting. Words appeared individually in a peripheral location (Exp. 1) or in a central location (Exp. 2), followed by an instruction to either remember or forget. After a short (50-ms) or long (250-ms) interstimulus interval (ISI), a central cue (80% accurate) directed participants to allocate their attention to the left or right. This was followed by a discrimination target that appeared at a 1,000-ms cue-target stimulus onset asynchrony. A subsequent yes-no recognition test assessed memory for all study items. In both experiments, we observed better recognition of remember words than forget words-a directed forgetting effect. We also found a cueing effect, revealed as faster reaction times to discriminate cued targets than to discriminate uncued targets. There was not, however, an effect of memory instruction (and/or instruction-cue ISI) on the magnitude of this cueing effect. Thus, neither exogenous attention nor endogenous attention remains in an unengaged state following an instruction to forget.
Assuntos
Estimulação Acústica/métodos , Sinais (Psicologia) , Rememoração Mental/fisiologia , Orientação Espacial/fisiologia , Estimulação Luminosa/métodos , Adulto , Atenção/fisiologia , Humanos , Masculino , Memória/fisiologia , Psicofísica , Tempo de Reação/fisiologia , Adulto JovemRESUMO
Background: Long-term potentiation (LTP) is recognised as a core neuronal process underlying long-term memory. However, a direct relationship between LTP and human memory performance is yet to be demonstrated. The first aim of the current study was thus to assess the relationship between LTP and human long-term memory performance. With this also comes an opportunity to explore factors thought to mediate the relationship between LTP and long-term memory. The second aim of the current study was to explore the relationship between LTP and memory in groups differing with respect to brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) Val66Met; a single-nucleotide polymorphism (SNP) implicated in memory function. Methods: Participants were split into three genotype groups (Val/Val, Val/Met, Met/Met) and were presented with both an EEG paradigm for inducing LTP-like enhancements of the visually-evoked response, and a test of visual memory. Results: The magnitude of LTP 40 min after induction was predictive of long-term memory performance. Additionally, the BDNF Met allele was associated with both reduced LTP and reduced memory performance. Conclusions: The current study not only presents the first evidence for a relationship between sensory LTP and human memory performance, but also demonstrates how targeting this relationship can provide insight into factors implicated in variation in human memory performance. It is anticipated that this will be of utility to future clinical studies of disrupted memory function.
RESUMO
Binocular rivalry occurs when separate incompatible images are presented, one to each eye. The subjective experience of viewing these stimuli is not of a fused image, rather the perceiver experiences either one or the other percept alternating across time. Manipulation of rivalry transfer, through an abrupt focal contrast change, causes the emerging percept to radiate across the declining image. This spreading percept transition has been likened to a wave of neural excitation propagating across the excitable medium of the visual cortex [H.R. Wilson et al. (2001) Nature, 412, 907-910; S.H. Lee et al. (2005) Nat. Neurosci., 8, 22-23]. Electrophysiologically, scalp-recorded gamma oscillations (30-70 Hz) have been reported to persist for several hundred milliseconds in response to changes in the subjective experience induced by alterations in incoming sensory information (moving-bar stimuli). Here, we used electroencephalographic recordings in the human and binocular rivalry transition to assess the role of scalp-recorded gamma in purely subjective changes in the visual experience. Despite the protraction of perceptual binding over a 900 ms period, no deviation in the pattern of induced gamma was observed from that reported during the perceptual closure of static images. These results suggest that localized scalp-recorded gamma oscillations are dissociable from the subjective visual experience.
Assuntos
Atenção/fisiologia , Relógios Biológicos/fisiologia , Dominância Ocular/fisiologia , Visão Binocular/fisiologia , Percepção Visual/fisiologia , Adulto , Eletroencefalografia/métodos , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Estimulação Luminosa/métodos , Córtex Visual/fisiologiaRESUMO
Investigating the neural substrates of auditory processing of absolute pitch musicians has relevance for understanding the capabilities of the human brain for plasticity. Electroencephalography was used to examine the N1 of auditory-evoked potentials from absolute pitch musicians, nonabsolute pitch musicians, and nonmusicians during tone labeling tasks with and without presentation of a reference tone. Source localization using low-resolution electromagnetic tomography revealed that when labeling tones without a reference, absolute pitch musicians generated greater activity than nonabsolute pitch musicians in the left and right hemispheres. This suggests that when required to label tones without an external reference, absolute pitch musicians have the ability to recruit a greater network than nonabsolute pitch musicians or nonmusicians.
Assuntos
Córtex Auditivo/fisiologia , Eletroencefalografia , Potenciais Evocados Auditivos/fisiologia , Música , Percepção da Altura Sonora/fisiologia , Estimulação Acústica , Adulto , Mapeamento Encefálico , Feminino , Lateralidade Funcional/fisiologia , Humanos , Masculino , Vias Neurais , Localização de Som/fisiologiaRESUMO
Orientation sensitivity of the amplitude and latency of the P1 and the N1 was investigated while participants performed letter-digit category judgments and red-blue color judgments. These two tasks were used in order to ascertain whether the orientation effects reflect access to object identity, which would be necessary for category, but not color, judgments. Character misorientation significantly affected both the latency and the amplitude of the N1, but not the P1, component. The N1 amplitude increased gradually up to 90 degrees , then leveled off up to 150 degrees , and dipped somewhat for 180 degrees . The effect of orientation on N1 latency differed between the hemispheres, with a quadratic function characterizing the effect of orientation on the left, and linear and quadratic trends characterizing the effect on the right. The effects of orientation were attributed to perceptual learning rather than object recognition, and the hemispheric differences in N1 latency suggest feature-based processing in the left hemisphere and holistic processing in the right.
Assuntos
Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos/fisiologia , Percepção Espacial/fisiologia , Adulto , Testes de Percepção de Cores , Eletroencefalografia/estatística & dados numéricos , Feminino , Lateralidade Funcional , Humanos , Masculino , Neurônios/fisiologia , Estimulação Luminosa , Desempenho Psicomotor , Tempo de ReaçãoRESUMO
Participants indicated whether a small dot was located near the top or bottom pole of a rotated object. Response times increased as a function of object orientation more for top trials than for bottom trials. The interaction between orientation and response was shown to be due to a relationship between response times and the dot's height on the screen. The orientation effect was influenced, positively and negatively, by a vertical arrangement of the response keys depending on whether the upper or lower key was used for the top response. Horizontal key placement produced an intermediate orientation effect, with asymmetries of about 180 degrees depending on which hand was used for top responses. This task appears to reflect spatial stimulus-response compatibilities more than object processing.