Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 112
Filtrar
1.
Ergonomics ; 67(3): 288-304, 2024 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37267092

RESUMO

The present study examined the impact of individual differences, attention, and memory deficits on distracted driving. Drivers with ADHD are more susceptible to distraction which results in more frequent collisions, violations, and licence suspensions. Consequently, the present investigation had 36 participants complete preliminary questionnaires, memory tasks, workload indices, and four, 4-min simulated driving scenarios to evaluate such impact. It was hypothesised ADHD diagnosis, type of cellular distraction, and traffic density would each differentially and substantively impact driving performance. Results indicated traffic density and distraction type significantly affected the objective driving facets measured, as well as subjective and secondary task performance. ADHD diagnosis directly impacted secondary task performance. Results further showed significant interactions between distraction type and traffic density on both brake pressure and steering wheel angle negatively impacting lateral and horizontal vehicle control. Altogether, these findings provide substantial empirical evidence for the deleterious effect of cellphone use on driving performance.Practitioner summary: This study examined how ADHD diagnosis, traffic density, and distraction type affect driver behaviour. Participants completed driving behaviour questionnaires, memory tasks, workload indices, and driving scenarios. Results showed that ADHD diagnosis impacted secondary task performance, while traffic and distractions significantly impacted driving performance as well secondary task performance and workload.


Assuntos
Transtorno do Deficit de Atenção com Hiperatividade , Telefone Celular , Direção Distraída , Humanos , Individualidade , Carga de Trabalho
2.
Ergonomics ; : 1-16, 2024 Jul 17.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39016161

RESUMO

As the UK's Chartered Institute of Ergonomics and Human Factors (CIEHF) celebrates its 75th anniversary, it is worth reflecting on our discipline's contribution, current state, and critical future endeavours. We present the perspectives of 18 EHF professionals who were asked to respond to five questions regarding the impact of EHF, contemporary challenges, and future directions. Co-authors were in agreement that EHF's impact has been only limited to date and that critical issues require resolution, such as increasing the number of suitably qualified practitioners, resolving the research-practice gap, and increasing awareness of EHF and its benefits. Frequently discussed future directions include advanced emerging technologies such as artificial intelligence, the development of new EHF methods, and enhancing the quality and reach of education and training. The majority felt there will be a need for EHF in 75 years; however, many noted that our methods will need to adapt to meet new needs.Practitioner statement: This article provides the perspectives of 18 Ergonomics and Human Factors (EHF) professionals on the impact of EHF, contemporary challenges and critical future directions, and changes that are necessary to ensure EHF remains relevant in future. As such, it provides important guidance on future EHF research and practice.

3.
J Integr Neurosci ; 22(3): 62, 2023 May 08.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37258434

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: With the advent of portable neurophysiological methods, including electroencephalography, progress in studying brain activity during physical tasks has received considerable attention, predominantly in clinical exercise and sports studies. However, the neural signatures of physical tasks in everyday settings were less addressed. METHODS: Electroencephalography (EEG) indices are sensitive to fluctuations in the human brain, reflecting spontaneous brain activity with an excellent temporal resolution. OBJECTIVE: In this regard, this study attempts to systematically review the feasibility of using EEG indices to quantify human performance in various physical activities in both laboratory and real-world applications. A secondary goal was to examine the feasibility of using EEG indices for quantifying human performance during physical activities with mental tasks. The systematic review was conducted based on the updated Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses guidelines. RESULTS: Out of 81 studies, 64 task studies focused on quantifying human performance concerning physical activity, whereas 17 studies focused on quantifying human performance on physical activities associated with mental tasks. EEG studies have primarily relied on linear methods, including the power spectrum, followed by the amplitude of Event-related potential components, to evaluate human physical performance. The nonlinear methods were relatively less addressed in the literature. Most studies focused on assessing the brain activity associated with muscular fatigue tasks. The upper anatomical areas have been discussed in several occupational schemes. The studies addressing biomechanical loading on the torso and spine, which are the risk factors for musculoskeletal disorders, are less addressed. CONCLUSIONS: Despite the recent interest in investigating the neural mechanisms underlying human motor functioning, assessing the brain signatures of physical tasks performed in naturalistic settings is still limited.


Assuntos
Encéfalo , Eletroencefalografia , Humanos , Eletroencefalografia/métodos , Encéfalo/fisiologia , Potenciais Evocados , Exercício Físico/fisiologia , Atenção/fisiologia
4.
Psychol Res ; 85(4): 1439-1448, 2021 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32382882

RESUMO

The aim was to examine theories of bilingual inhibitory control superiority in the visual domain. In an ambiguous figure task, the ability to reverse (switch) interpretations (e.g., duck-rabbit) was examined in 3-5-year-olds bilinguals and monolinguals (N = 67). Bilingualism was no performance predictor in conceptual tasks (Droodle task, false belief task, ambiguous figures production task) that did not pose inhibitory demands. Bilinguals outperformed monolinguals in the ability to reverse, suggesting superior inhibitory capacity per se. Once reversal was experienced there was no difference in the time it took to reverse or reversal frequency between bilinguals and monolinguals. Bayesian analyses confirmed statistical result patterns. Findings support the established view of bilinguals' superior domain-general inhibitory control. This might be brought to bear by attending the environment differently.


Assuntos
Enganação , Discriminação Psicológica/fisiologia , Multilinguismo , Tempo de Reação/fisiologia , Reconhecimento Psicológico/fisiologia , Teorema de Bayes , Pré-Escolar , Comunicação , Feminino , Humanos , Idioma , Masculino , Psicolinguística
5.
Ergonomics ; 62(4): 575-592, 2019 Apr.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30523739

RESUMO

Witnesses may construct a composite face of a perpetrator using a computerised interface. Police practitioners guide witnesses through this unusual process, the goal being to produce an identifiable image. However, any changes a perpetrator makes to their external facial-features may interfere with this process. In Experiment 1, participants constructed a composite using a holistic interface one day after target encoding. Target faces were unaltered, or had altered external-features: (i) changed hair, (ii) external-features removed or (iii) naturally-concealed external-features (hair, ears, face-shape occluded by a hooded top). These manipulations produced composites with more error-prone internal-features: participants' familiar with a target's unaltered appearance less often provided a correct name. Experiment 2 applied external-feature alterations to composites of unaltered targets; although whole-face composites contained less error-prone internal-features, identification was impaired. Experiment 3 replicated negative effects of changing target hair on construction and tested a practical solution: selectively concealing hair and eyes improved identification. Practitioner Summary: The research indicates that when a target identity disguises or changes hair, this can lead to a witness (or victim) constructing a composite that is less readily identified. We assess a practical method to overcome this forensic issue. Abbreviation: GEE: Generalized Estimating Equations.


Assuntos
Face/anatomia & histologia , Reconhecimento Facial , Rememoração Mental , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos , Reconhecimento Psicológico , Adolescente , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Fotografação , Adulto Jovem
6.
Hum Factors ; 60(5): 597-609, 2018 08.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29986155

RESUMO

OBJECTIVE: This work assesses the efficacy of the "prevalence effect" as a form of cyberattack in human-automation teaming, using an email task. BACKGROUND: Under the prevalence effect, rare signals are more difficult to detect, even when taking into account their proportionally low occurrence. This decline represents diminished human capability to both detect and respond. As signal probability (SP) approaches zero, accuracy exhibits logarithmic decay. Cybersecurity, a context in which the environment is entirely artificial, provides an opportunity to manufacture conditions enhancing or degrading human performance, such as prevalence effects. Email cybersecurity prevalence effects have not previously been demonstrated, nor intentionally manipulated. METHOD: The Email Testbed (ET) provides a simulation of a clerical email work involving messages containing sensitive personal information. Using the ET, participants were presented with 300 email interactions and received cyberattacks at rates of either 1%, 5%, or 20%. RESULTS: Results demonstrated the existence and power of prevalence effects in email cybersecurity. Attacks delivered at a rate of 1% were significantly more likely to succeed, and the overall pattern of accuracy across declining SP exhibited logarithmic decay. APPLICATION: These findings suggest a "prevalence paradox" within human-machine teams. As automation reduces attack SP, the human operator becomes increasingly likely to fail in detecting and reporting attacks that remain. In the cyber realm, the potential to artificially inflict this state on adversaries, hacking the human operator rather than algorithmic defense, is considered. Specific and general information security design countermeasures are offered.


Assuntos
Segurança Computacional , Correio Eletrônico , Sistemas Homem-Máquina , Interface Usuário-Computador , Adulto , Humanos , Probabilidade , Adulto Jovem
7.
Hum Factors ; 59(1): 35-43, 2017 02.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28146675

RESUMO

OBJECTIVE: I explore the origins, theoretical underpinnings, applications, and importance of vigilance in a world ever more dominated by semiautomated, automated, and autonomous machines. BACKGROUND: The empirical genesis of vigilance is taken as a case study in the etiology of the application of the behavioral sciences to the human culture of technology. The subsequent taxonomic ordering and theoretical clarification of its causal antecedents are set in the overall context of contemporary human-machine systems research. METHOD: The methods exercised in this work are historical analysis and informational synthesis in combination with projected theoretical implications and impact. RESULTS: The profile of evolution of the concept of vigilance is clarified and cast in the light of critical events, such as the promulgation of the vigilance taxonomy, its linkage to attentional resource theory, and the recognition that the attendant performance decrement is as indicative of iatrogenic sources as it is a shortfall or limitation of the observer's processing capacity. CONCLUSION: Vigilance is alive and growing in importance. Understanding sustained attention will become ever more critical in the humanization of automation-dominated systems. APPLICATION: The application of vigilance is widespread and potentially ubiquitous for semiautomated, automated, and autonomous system interaction.


Assuntos
Nível de Alerta/fisiologia , Sistemas Homem-Máquina , Humanos
8.
Hum Factors ; 59(1): 28-34, 2017 02.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28146677

RESUMO

OBJECTIVE: To provide an evaluative overview of the life and contributions of Raja Parasuraman. BACKGROUND: From his earliest contributions in clarifying and explaining the problematic area of vigilance to his most recent interdisciplinary advances in understanding how genotype relates to behavior in complex technical environments, Raja Parasuraman was a giant of human factors and ergonomics. Our present exposition articulates and recounts his many contributions to our science and to science in general beyond the confines of our own discipline. METHOD: We use the history of scientific contributions, biographical analysis, and reported personal experience to accomplish our overall assessment of the man and his work. RESULTS: We conclude that Parasuraman's contributions were unique, substantive, and seminal, and will continue to influence our science for many years to come. APPLICATION: This work will serve as a record for those to come who look to make significant contributions to the goals, aims, and aspirations that we set ourselves in human factors and ergonomics in seeking to improve the human condition.


Assuntos
Neurociência Cognitiva/história , Ergonomia/história , Sistemas Homem-Máquina , História do Século XX , História do Século XXI , Humanos
9.
Int J Eat Disord ; 49(5): 507-18, 2016 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26996142

RESUMO

OBJECTIVE: A core feature of anorexia nervosa (AN) is an over-estimation of body size. Women with AN have a different pattern of eye-movements when judging bodies, but it is unclear whether this is specific to their diagnosis or whether it is found in anyone over-estimating body size. METHOD: To address this question, we compared the eye movement patterns from three participant groups while they carried out a body size estimation task: (i) 20 women with recovering/recovered anorexia (rAN) who had concerns about body shape and weight and who over-estimated body size, (ii) 20 healthy controls who had normative levels of concern about body shape and who estimated body size accurately (iii) 20 healthy controls who had normative levels of concern about body shape but who did over-estimate body size. RESULTS: Comparisons between the three groups showed that: (i) accurate body size estimators tended to look more in the waist region, and this was independent of clinical diagnosis; (ii) there is a pattern of looking at images of bodies, particularly viewing the upper parts of the torso and face, which is specific to participants with rAN but which is independent of accuracy in body size estimation. DISCUSSION: Since the over-estimating controls did not share the same body image concerns that women with rAN report, their over-estimation cannot be explained by attitudinal concerns about body shape and weight. These results suggest that a distributed fixation pattern is associated with over-estimation of body size and should be addressed in treatment programs. © 2016 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. (Int J Eat Disord 2016; 49:507-518).


Assuntos
Anorexia Nervosa/psicologia , Transtornos Dismórficos Corporais/psicologia , Tamanho Corporal , Anorexia Nervosa/diagnóstico , Anorexia Nervosa/fisiopatologia , Imagem Corporal , Peso Corporal , Estudos de Casos e Controles , Movimentos Oculares/fisiologia , Feminino , Fixação Ocular/fisiologia , Humanos , Adulto Jovem
10.
Hum Factors ; 58(8): 1143-1157, 2016 12.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27613827

RESUMO

OBJECTIVE: We examine how transitions in task demand are manifested in mental workload and performance in a dual-task setting. BACKGROUND: Hysteresis has been defined as the ongoing influence of demand levels prior to a demand transition. Authors of previous studies predominantly examined hysteretic effects in terms of performance. However, little is known about the temporal development of hysteresis in mental workload. METHOD: A simulated driving task was combined with an auditory memory task. Participants were instructed to prioritize driving or to prioritize both tasks equally. Three experimental conditions with low, high, and low task demands were constructed by manipulating the frequency of lane changing. Multiple measures of subjective mental workload were taken during experimental conditions. RESULTS: Contrary to our prediction, no hysteretic effects were found after the high- to low-demand transition. However, a hysteretic effect in mental workload was found within the high-demand condition, which degraded toward the end of the high condition. Priority instructions were not reflected in performance. CONCLUSION: Online assessment of both performance and mental workload demonstrates the transient nature of hysteretic effects. An explanation for the observed hysteretic effect in mental workload is offered in terms of effort regulation. APPLICATION: An informed arrival at the scene is important in safety operations, but peaks in mental workload should be avoided to prevent buildup of fatigue. Therefore, communication technologies should incorporate the historical profile of task demand.


Assuntos
Percepção Auditiva/fisiologia , Condução de Veículo , Função Executiva/fisiologia , Memória de Curto Prazo/fisiologia , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Adulto , Humanos
11.
Am J Psychol ; 129(2): 111-24, 2016.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27424414

RESUMO

Effects of sex and handedness on the perception of temporal durations from 1 to 20 s were studied. A total of 80 male and 40 female participants were divided equally into left-handed and right-handed subgroups. Using an empty interval production procedure, each person estimated durations of 1, 3, 7, and 20 s, respectively, 50 times each. The order of presentation was randomized across participants but yoked across the sexes in each of the respective handedness subgroups. Results showed significant sex differences but no effects for handedness. One important facet of this sex effect was expressed in a consistent intercept difference in the identified relationship that linked the log-linear size of the absolute error of estimation against the logarithmic magnitude of the target duration at which such error was recorded. This new finding provides a new descriptive, empirical relationship for time perception of brief temporal intervals. The potential methodological, evolutionary, and cognitive reasons for this lawful relationship are discussed.


Assuntos
Lateralidade Funcional/fisiologia , Percepção do Tempo/fisiologia , Adulto , Interpretação Estatística de Dados , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Fatores Sexuais , Adulto Jovem
12.
Law Hum Behav ; 40(2): 128-35, 2016 Apr.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26436334

RESUMO

Facial composite systems help eyewitnesses to show the appearance of criminals. However, likenesses created by unfamiliar witnesses will not be completely accurate, and people familiar with the target can find them difficult to identify. Faces are processed holistically; we explore whether this impairs identification of inaccurate composite images and whether recognition can be improved. In Experiment 1 (n = 64) an imaging technique was used to make composites of celebrity faces more accurate and identification was contrasted with the original composite images. Corrected composites were better recognized, confirming that errors in production of the likenesses impair identification. The influence of holistic face processing was explored by misaligning the top and bottom parts of the composites (cf. Young, Hellawell, & Hay, 1987). Misalignment impaired recognition of corrected composites but identification of the original, inaccurate composites significantly improved. This effect was replicated with facial composites of noncelebrities in Experiment 2 (n = 57). We conclude that, like real faces, facial composites are processed holistically: recognition is impaired because unlike real faces, composites contain inaccuracies and holistic face processing makes it difficult to perceive identifiable features. This effect was consistent across composites of celebrities and composites of people who are personally familiar. Our findings suggest that identification of forensic facial composites can be enhanced by presenting composites in a misaligned format.


Assuntos
Criminosos , Face , Rememoração Mental , Reconhecimento Psicológico , Adolescente , Adulto , Idoso , Feminino , Humanos , Processamento de Imagem Assistida por Computador , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Fotografação , Adulto Jovem
13.
Hum Factors ; 57(8): 1339-42, 2015 Dec.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26534852

RESUMO

The laudable effort by Strayer and his colleagues to derive a systematic method to assess forms of cognitive distraction in the automobile is beset by the problem of nonstationary in driver response capacity. At the level of the overall goal of driving, this problem conflates actual on-road behavior; characterized by underspecified task satisficing, with our own understandable, scientifically inspired aspiration for measuring deterministic performance optimization. Measures of response conceived under this latter imperative are, at best, only shadowy reflections of the actual phenomenological experience involved in real-world vehicle control. Whether we, as a research community, can resolve this issue remains uncertain. However, we believe we can mount a positive attack on what is arguably another equally important dimension of the collision problem.


Assuntos
Atenção , Automóveis , Acidentes de Trânsito , Condução de Veículo/psicologia , Cognição , Humanos
14.
Am J Psychol ; 128(1): 1-14, 2015.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26219170

RESUMO

It has been claimed that dreams are the royal road to the unconscious mind. The present work argues that dreams and associated brain states such as memory, attention, flow, and perhaps even consciousness itself arise from diverse conflicts over control of time in the brain. Dreams are the brain's offline efforts to distill projections of the future, while memory represents the vestiges of the past successes and survived failures of those and other conscious projections. Memory thus acts to inform and improve the prediction of possible future states through the use of conscious prospects (planning) and unconscious prospective memory (dreams). When successful, these prospects result in states of flow for conscious planning and déjà vu for its unconscious comparator. In consequence, and contrary to normal expectation, memory is overwhelmingly oriented to deal with the future. Consciousness is the comparable process operating in the present moment. Thus past, present, and future are homeomorphic with the parts of memory (episodic and autobiographical) that recall a personal past, consciousness, and the differing dimensions of prospective memory to plan for future circumstances, respectively. Dreaming (i.e., unconscious prospective memory), has the luxury to run multiple "what if" simulations of many possible futures, essentially offline. I explicate these propositions and their relations to allied constructs such as déjà vu and flow. More generally, I propose that what appear to us as a range of normal psychological experiences are actually manifestations of an ongoing pathological battle for control within the brain. The landscape of this conflict is time. I suggest that there are at least 3 general systems bidding for this control, and in the process of evolution, each system has individually conferred a sequentially increasing survival advantage, but only at the expense of a still incomplete functional integration. Through juxtaposition of these respective brain systems, I endeavor to resolve some fundamental paradoxes and conundrums expressed in the basic psychological and behavioral processes of sleep, consciousness, and memory. The implication of this conceptual framework for the overall conception of time is then briefly adumbrated.


Assuntos
Encéfalo/fisiologia , Estado de Consciência/fisiologia , Sonhos/fisiologia , Memória/fisiologia , Humanos
15.
Ergonomics ; 58(4): 548-64, 2015.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25831959

RESUMO

Traditional efforts to deal with the enormous problem of workplace safety have proved insufficient, as they have tended to neglect the broader sociotechnical environment that surrounds workers. Here, we advocate a sociotechnical systems approach that describes the complex multi-level system factors that contribute to workplace safety. From the literature on sociotechnical systems, complex systems and safety, we develop a sociotechnical model of workplace safety with concentric layers of the work system, socio-organisational context and the external environment. The future challenges that are identified through the model are highlighted. PRACTITIONER SUMMARY: Understanding the environmental, organisational and work system factors that contribute to workplace safety will help to develop more effective and integrated solutions to deal with persistent workplace safety problems. Solutions to improve workplace safety need to recognise the broad sociotechnical system and the respective interactions between the system elements and levels.


Assuntos
Saúde Ocupacional , Análise de Sistemas , Teoria de Sistemas , Humanos , Segurança , Local de Trabalho
16.
Ergonomics ; 58(1): 1-17, 2015.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25442818

RESUMO

Mental workload (MWL) is one of the most widely used concepts in ergonomics and human factors and represents a topic of increasing importance. Since modern technology in many working environments imposes ever more cognitive demands upon operators while physical demands diminish, understanding how MWL impinges on performance is increasingly critical. Yet, MWL is also one of the most nebulous concepts, with numerous definitions and dimensions associated with it. Moreover, MWL research has had a tendency to focus on complex, often safety-critical systems (e.g. transport, process control). Here we provide a general overview of the current state of affairs regarding the understanding, measurement and application of MWL in the design of complex systems over the last three decades. We conclude by discussing contemporary challenges for applied research, such as the interaction between cognitive workload and physical workload, and the quantification of workload 'redlines' which specify when operators are approaching or exceeding their performance tolerances.


Assuntos
Ergonomia , Carga de Trabalho/psicologia , Cognição , Humanos , Análise e Desempenho de Tarefas
17.
Cogn Neuropsychiatry ; 19(6): 554-69, 2014.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25101966

RESUMO

INTRODUCTION: Existing eye-tracking literature has shown that both adults and children with autism spectrum disorders (ASD) show fewer and slower fixations on faces. Despite this reduced saliency and processing of other faces, recognition of their own face is reported to be more "typical" in nature. This study uses eye-tracking to explore the typicality of gaze patterns when children with ASD attend their own faces compared to other familiar and unfamiliar faces. METHODS: Eye-tracking methodology was used to explore fixation duration and time taken to fixate on the Eye and Mouth regions of familiar, unfamiliar and Self Faces. Twenty-one children with ASD (9-16 years) were compared to typically developing matched groups. RESULTS: There were no significant differences between children with ASD and typically matched groups for fixation patterns to the Eye and Mouth areas of all face types (familiar, unfamiliar and self). Correlational analyses showed that attention to the Eye area of unfamiliar and Self Faces was related to socio-communicative ability in children with ASD. CONCLUSIONS: Levels of socio-communicative ability in children with ASD were related to gaze patterns on unfamiliar and Self Faces, but not familiar faces. This lack of relationship between ability and attention to familiar faces may indicate that children across the autism spectrum are able to fixate these faces in a similar way. The implications for these findings are discussed.


Assuntos
Atenção , Transtornos Globais do Desenvolvimento Infantil/psicologia , Face , Reconhecimento Psicológico , Adolescente , Adulto , Estudos de Casos e Controles , Criança , Comunicação , Olho , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Boca , Comportamento Social
18.
Sci Justice ; 54(3): 215-27, 2014 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24796951

RESUMO

Eyewitnesses are often invited to construct a facial composite, an image created of the person they saw commit a crime that is used by law enforcement to locate criminal suspects. In the current paper, the effectiveness of composite images was investigated from traditional feature systems (E-FIT and PRO-fit), where participants (face constructors) selected individual features to build the face, and a more recent holistic system (EvoFIT), where they 'evolved' a composite by repeatedly selecting from arrays of complete faces. Further participants attempted to name these composites when seen as an unaltered image, or when blurred, rotated, linearly stretched or converted to a photographic negative. All of the manipulations tested reduced correct naming of the composites overall except (i) for a low level of blur, for which naming improved for holistic composites but reduced for feature composites, and (ii) for 100% linear stretch, for which a substantial naming advantage was observed. Results also indicated that both featural (facial elements) and configural (feature spacing) information were useful for recognition in both types of composite system, but highly-detailed information was more accurate in the feature-based than in the holistic method. The naming advantage of linear stretch was replicated using a forensically more-practical procedure with observers viewing an unaltered composite sideways. The work is valuable to police practitioners and designers of facial-composite systems.


Assuntos
Identificação Biométrica/métodos , Face/anatomia & histologia , Fotografação , Adolescente , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Adulto Jovem
19.
Cogn Res Princ Implic ; 9(1): 41, 2024 Jun 21.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38902539

RESUMO

The human face is commonly used for identity verification. While this task was once exclusively performed by humans, technological advancements have seen automated facial recognition systems (AFRS) integrated into many identification scenarios. Although many state-of-the-art AFRS are exceptionally accurate, they often require human oversight or involvement, such that a human operator actions the final decision. Previously, we have shown that on average, humans assisted by a simulated AFRS (sAFRS) failed to reach the level of accuracy achieved by the same sAFRS alone, due to overturning the system's correct decisions and/or failing to correct sAFRS errors. The aim of the current study was to investigate whether participants' trust in automation was related to their performance on a one-to-one face matching task when assisted by a sAFRS. Participants (n = 160) completed a standard face matching task in two phases: an unassisted baseline phase, and an assisted phase where they were shown the identification decision (95% accurate) made by a sAFRS prior to submitting their own decision. While most participants improved with sAFRS assistance, those with greater relative trust in automation achieved larger gains in performance. However, the average aided performance of participants still failed to reach that of the sAFRS alone, regardless of trust status. Nonetheless, further analysis revealed a small sample of participants who achieved 100% accuracy when aided by the sAFRS. Our results speak to the importance of considering individual differences when selecting employees for roles requiring human-algorithm interaction, including identity verification tasks that incorporate facial recognition technologies.


Assuntos
Reconhecimento Facial Automatizado , Automação , Confiança , Humanos , Masculino , Feminino , Adulto , Adulto Jovem , Reconhecimento Facial/fisiologia , Algoritmos
20.
Cortex ; 172: 159-184, 2024 03.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38330779

RESUMO

Despite severe everyday problems recognising faces, some individuals with developmental prosopagnosia (DP) can achieve typical accuracy scores on laboratory face recognition tests. To address this, studies sometimes also examine response times (RTs), which tend to be longer in DPs relative to control participants. In the present study, 24 potential (according to self-report) DPs and 110 age-matched controls completed the Cambridge Face and Bicycle Memory Tests, old new faces task, and a famous faces test. We used accuracy and the Balanced Integration Score (BIS), a measure that adjusts accuracy for RTs, to classify our sample at the group and individual levels. Subjective face recognition ability was assessed using the PI20 questionnaire and semi structured interviews. Fifteen DPs showed a major impairment using BIS compared with only five using accuracy alone. Logistic regression showed that a model incorporating the BIS measures was the most sensitive for classifying DP and showed highest area under the curve (AUC). Furthermore, larger between-group effect sizes were observed for a derived global (averaged) memory measure calculated using BIS versus accuracy alone. BIS is thus an extremely sensitive novel measure for attenuating speed-accuracy trade-offs that can otherwise mask impairment measured only by accuracy in DP.


Assuntos
Reconhecimento Facial , Prosopagnosia , Humanos , Prosopagnosia/diagnóstico , Reconhecimento Facial/fisiologia , Autorrelato , Inquéritos e Questionários , Tempo de Reação , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos/fisiologia
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA