Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 92
Filtrar
1.
J Int Neuropsychol Soc ; 30(4): 328-338, 2024 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37860873

RESUMO

OBJECTIVE: Increased intraindividual variability (IIV) of cognitive performance is a marker of cognitive decline in older adults. Whether computerized cognitive training (CCT) and aerobic exercise counteracts cognitive decline by reducing IIV is unknown. We investigated the effects of CCT with or without aerobic exercise on IIV in older adults. METHODS: This was a secondary analysis of an 8-week randomized controlled trial. Older adults (aged 65-85 years) were randomized to CCT alone (n = 41), CCT with aerobic exercise (n = 41), or an active control group (n = 42). The CCT group trained using the Fit Brains® platform 3×/week for 1 hr (plus 3×/week of home-based training). The CCT with aerobic exercise group received 15 min of walking plus 45 min of Fit Brains® 3×/week (plus 3×/week of home-based training). The control group received sham exercise and cognitive training (3×/week for 1 hr). We computed reaction time IIV from the Dimensional Change Card Sort Test, Flanker Inhibitory Control and Attention Test (Flanker), and Pattern Comparison Processing Speed Test (PACPS). RESULTS: Compared with the control group, IIV reduced in a processing speed task (PACPS) following CCT alone (mean difference [95% confidence interval]: -0.144 [-0.255 to -0.034], p < 0.01) and CCT with aerobic exercise (-0.113 [-0.225 to -0.001], p < 0.05). Attention (Flanker congruent) IIV was reduced only after CCT with aerobic exercise (-0.130 [-0.242 to -0.017], p < 0.05). CONCLUSIONS: A CCT program promoted cognitive health via reductions in IIV of cognitive performance and combining it with aerobic exercise may result in broader benefits.


Assuntos
Cognição , Disfunção Cognitiva , Humanos , Idoso , Exercício Físico/psicologia , Caminhada , Terapia por Exercício/métodos
2.
Neuroimage ; 236: 118089, 2021 08 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33882347

RESUMO

White matter hyperintensities (WMH) are a prominent feature of cerebral small vessel disease and are associated with cognitive impairment. These deficits in cognition may be caused by the disruption of large-scale functional networks due to the presence of WMHs. However, knowledge regarding the relevance of these lesions on functional networks remains inconclusive. These inconsistencies may derive from issues with interpreting functional imaging data from clinical populations. Lesion network mapping is a technique that allows the overlaying of lesions from a patient population to the functional connectivity of a human connectome derived from healthy adults. This allows researchers to identify functional networks that would be disrupted in a healthy population should the WMHs seen in cerebral small vessel disease be present. We hypothesized that the extent to which these functional networks are disrupted by WMHs is associated with cognitive performance in older adults with cerebral small vessel disease. This cross-sectional study combined baseline data from four studies to create a total sample of 164 older adults (aged ≥55) from metropolitan Vancouver with cerebral small vessel disease. Using lesion network mapping, we assessed the percentage overlap between voxels functionally connected with both the WMHs (lesion network) and five common functional networks: (1) visual; (2) dorsal attention; (3) ventral attention; (4) sensorimotor; and (5) frontoparietal. Cognition was assessed using: (1) Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA); (2) Stroop Colour Word Test (3-2); (3) Trail Making Tests (Part B-A); and (4) Digit Symbol Substitution Test. A One-Way ANOVA and Tukey post-hoc tests were performed to identify the functional networks with greatest percentage overlap with the lesion network. Partial correlations controlling for age were used to analyse whether the extent of the overlap between the lesion and functional networks was associated with poorer cognition. The visual, ventral attention, and frontoparietal networks had significantly greater overlap with the lesion network. After controlling for multiple comparisons, level of lesion network overlap with both the sensorimotor network (p<.001) and ventral attention network (p <. 001) was significantly correlated with MoCA score. Thus, the greater the disruption to the sensorimotor and ventral attention networks, the poorer the global cognition. Our results reveal that the visual, ventral attention, and frontoparietal networks are most vulnerable to disruptions stemming from WMHs. Additionally, we identified that disruption to the sensorimotor and ventral attention networks, as a result of WMHs, may underlie deficits in global cognition in older adults with cerebral small vessel disease.


Assuntos
Doenças de Pequenos Vasos Cerebrais , Disfunção Cognitiva , Conectoma , Estudos Transversais , Função Executiva/fisiologia , Leucoaraiose , Rede Nervosa , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Doenças de Pequenos Vasos Cerebrais/diagnóstico por imagem , Doenças de Pequenos Vasos Cerebrais/patologia , Doenças de Pequenos Vasos Cerebrais/fisiopatologia , Disfunção Cognitiva/diagnóstico por imagem , Disfunção Cognitiva/patologia , Disfunção Cognitiva/fisiopatologia , Feminino , Humanos , Leucoaraiose/diagnóstico por imagem , Leucoaraiose/patologia , Leucoaraiose/fisiopatologia , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Rede Nervosa/diagnóstico por imagem , Rede Nervosa/patologia , Rede Nervosa/fisiopatologia , Testes Neuropsicológicos
3.
BMC Cancer ; 20(1): 711, 2020 Jul 31.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32736542

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: Up to 75% of women diagnosed with breast cancer report chemotherapy-related cognitive changes (CRCC) during treatment, including decreased memory, attention, and processing speed. Though CRCC negatively impacts everyday functioning and reduces overall quality of life in women diagnosed with breast cancer, effective interventions to prevent and/or manage CRCC are elusive. Consequently, women seldom receive advice on how to prevent or manage CRCC. Aerobic exercise is associated with improved cognitive functioning in healthy older adults and adults with cognitive impairments. Accordingly, it holds promise as an intervention to prevent and/or manage CRCC. However, evidence from randomized controlled trials (RCTs) supporting a beneficial effect of aerobic exercise on CRCC is limited. The primary aim of the ACTIVATE trial is to evaluate the impact of supervised aerobic exercise on CRCC in women receiving chemotherapy for breast cancer. METHODS: The ACTIVATE trial is a two-arm, two-centre RCT. Women diagnosed with stage I-III breast cancer and awaiting neo-adjuvant or adjuvant chemotherapy are recruited from hospitals in Ottawa (Ontario) and Vancouver (British Columbia), Canada. Recruits are randomized to the intervention group (aerobic exercise during chemotherapy) or the wait-list control group (usual care during chemotherapy and aerobic exercise post-chemotherapy). The primary outcome is cognitive functioning as measured by a composite cognitive summary score (COGSUM) of several neuropsychological tests. Secondary outcomes are self-reported cognitive functioning, quality of life, and brain structure and functioning (measured by magnetic resonance imaging (MRI)/functional MRI and electroencephalography). Assessments take place pre-chemotherapy (pre-intervention), mid-way through chemotherapy (mid-intervention/mid-wait period), end of chemotherapy (post-intervention/post-wait period; primary endpoint), 16-weeks post-chemotherapy, and at 1-year post-baseline. DISCUSSION: Aerobic exercise is a promising intervention for preventing and/or managing CRCC and enhancing quality of life among women diagnosed with breast cancer. The ACTIVATE trial tests several novel hypotheses, including that aerobic exercise can prevent and/or mitigate CRCC and that this effect is mediated by the timing of intervention delivery (i.e., during versus post-chemotherapy). Findings may support prescribing exercise during (or post-) chemotherapy for breast cancer and elucidate the potential role of aerobic exercise as a management strategy for CRCC in women with early-stage breast cancer. TRIAL REGISTRATION: The trial was registered with the ClinicalTrials.gov database ( NCT03277898 ) on September 11, 2017.


Assuntos
Neoplasias da Mama/tratamento farmacológico , Transtornos Cognitivos/terapia , Cognição/efeitos dos fármacos , Exercício Físico/fisiologia , Antineoplásicos/efeitos adversos , Transtornos Cognitivos/diagnóstico , Transtornos Cognitivos/prevenção & controle , Feminino , Humanos , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Seleção de Pacientes , Qualidade de Vida , Tamanho da Amostra , Autorrelato , Listas de Espera
4.
Eur J Neurosci ; 48(12): 3534-3551, 2018 12.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30151944

RESUMO

The ability to actively suppress, or gate, irrelevant sensory information is required for safe and efficient walking in sensory-rich environments. Both motor attention and motor planning alter somatosensory evoked potentials (SEPs) in healthy adults. This study's aim was to examine the effect of motor attention on processing of irrelevant somatosensory information during plantar flexion motor planning after stroke. Thirteen healthy older adults and 11 individuals with stroke participated. Irrelevant tibial nerve stimulation was delivered while SEPs were recorded over Cz, overlaying the leg portion of the sensorimotor cortex at the vertex of the head. Three conditions were tested in both legs: (1) Rest, (2) Attend To, and (3) Attend Away from the stimulated limb. In conditions 2 and 3, relevant vibration cued voluntary plantar flexion movements of the stimulated (Attend To) or non-stimulated (Attend Away) leg. SEP amplitudes were averaged during motor planning per condition. Individuals with stroke did not show attention-mediated gating of the N40 component associated with irrelevant somatosensory information during motor planning. It may be that dysfunction in pathways connecting to area 3b explains the lack of attention-mediated gating of the N40. Also, attention-mediated gating during motor planning explained significant and unique variance in a measure of community balance and mobility combined with response time. Thus, the ability to gate irrelevant somatosensory information appears important for stepping in both older adults and after stroke. Our data suggest that therapies that direct motor attention could positively impact walking after stroke.


Assuntos
Potenciais Somatossensoriais Evocados/fisiologia , Movimento/fisiologia , Córtex Somatossensorial/fisiologia , Acidente Vascular Cerebral/fisiopatologia , Adulto , Idoso , Atenção/fisiologia , Estimulação Elétrica/métodos , Eletroencefalografia , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Nervo Mediano/fisiopatologia , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Tempo de Reação , Nervo Tibial/fisiopatologia
5.
Exp Brain Res ; 233(5): 1503-10, 2015 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25700669

RESUMO

Although migraine is traditionally categorized as a primary headache disorder, the condition is also associated with abnormalities in visual attentional function in between headache events. Namely, relative to controls, migraineurs show both a heightened sensitivity to nominally unattended visual events, as well as decreased habituation responses at sensory and post-sensory (cognitive) levels. Here we used event-related potentials (ERPs) to examine whether cortical hypersensitivities in migraineurs extend to mind wandering, or periods of time wherein we transiently attenuate the processing of external stimulus inputs as our thoughts drift away from the on-going task at hand. Participants performed a sustained attention to response task while they were occasionally queried as to their attentional state-either "on-task" or "mind wandering." We then analyzed the ERP responses to task-relevant stimuli as a function of whether they immediately preceded an on-task versus mind wandering report. We found that despite the commonly reported heightened visual sensitivities in our migraine group, they nevertheless manifest a reduced cognitive response during periods of mind wandering relative to on-task attentional states, as measured via amplitude changes in the P3 ERP component. This suggests that our capacity to attenuate the processing of external stimulus inputs during mind wandering is not necessarily impaired by the class of cortical hypersensitivities characteristic of the interictal migraine brain.


Assuntos
Atenção/fisiologia , Conscientização/fisiologia , Encéfalo/fisiopatologia , Potenciais Evocados Visuais/fisiologia , Transtornos de Enxaqueca/fisiopatologia , Adulto , Eletroencefalografia , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Estimulação Luminosa , Desempenho Psicomotor , Tempo de Reação/fisiologia , Pensamento/fisiologia , Adulto Jovem
6.
J Cogn Neurosci ; 26(3): 635-44, 2014 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24168216

RESUMO

Our ability to make decisions is predicated upon our knowledge of the outcomes of the actions available to us. Reinforcement learning theory posits that actions followed by a reward or punishment acquire value through the computation of prediction errors-discrepancies between the predicted and the actual reward. A multitude of neuroimaging studies have demonstrated that rewards and punishments evoke neural responses that appear to reflect reinforcement learning prediction errors [e.g., Krigolson, O. E., Pierce, L. J., Holroyd, C. B., & Tanaka, J. W. Learning to become an expert: Reinforcement learning and the acquisition of perceptual expertise. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21, 1833-1840, 2009; Bayer, H. M., & Glimcher, P. W. Midbrain dopamine neurons encode a quantitative reward prediction error signal. Neuron, 47, 129-141, 2005; O'Doherty, J. P. Reward representations and reward-related learning in the human brain: Insights from neuroimaging. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 14, 769-776, 2004; Holroyd, C. B., & Coles, M. G. H. The neural basis of human error processing: Reinforcement learning, dopamine, and the error-related negativity. Psychological Review, 109, 679-709, 2002]. Here, we used the brain ERP technique to demonstrate that not only do rewards elicit a neural response akin to a prediction error but also that this signal rapidly diminished and propagated to the time of choice presentation with learning. Specifically, in a simple, learnable gambling task, we show that novel rewards elicited a feedback error-related negativity that rapidly decreased in amplitude with learning. Furthermore, we demonstrate the existence of a reward positivity at choice presentation, a previously unreported ERP component that has a similar timing and topography as the feedback error-related negativity that increased in amplitude with learning. The pattern of results we observed mirrored the output of a computational model that we implemented to compute reward prediction errors and the changes in amplitude of these prediction errors at the time of choice presentation and reward delivery. Our results provide further support that the computations that underlie human learning and decision-making follow reinforcement learning principles.


Assuntos
Encéfalo/fisiologia , Tomada de Decisões/fisiologia , Aprendizagem/fisiologia , Reforço Psicológico , Adolescente , Adulto , Simulação por Computador , Eletroencefalografia , Potenciais Evocados , Feminino , Jogo de Azar , Humanos , Masculino , Modelos Neurológicos , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Recompensa , Análise e Desempenho de Tarefas , Fatores de Tempo , Adulto Jovem
7.
Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci ; 14(1): 286-96, 2014 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23900749

RESUMO

Mind wandering reduces both the sensory and cognitive processing of affectively neutral stimuli, but whether it also modulates the processing of affectively salient stimuli remains unclear. In particular, we examined whether mind wandering attenuates one's sensitivity to observing mild pain in others. In the first experiment, we recorded event-related potentials (ERPs) as participants viewed images of hands in either painful or neutral situations, while being prompted at random intervals to report whether their thoughts were on task or mind wandering. We found that the brain's later response to painful images was significantly reduced immediately preceding "mind-wandering" versus "on-task" reports, as measured via amplitude decreases in a frontal-central positivity beginning approximately 300 ms poststimulus. In a second, control experiment using behavioral measures, we wanted to confirm whether the subjective sense of pain observed in others does in fact decrease during mind wandering. Accordingly, we asked participants to rate how painful the hand images looked on a 5-point Likert scale, again while taking reports of their mind-wandering states at unpredictable intervals. Consistent with our ERP data, we found that the ratings for painful images were significantly reduced immediately preceding mind-wandering reports. Additional control analyses suggested that the effect could not simply be ascribed to general habituation in the affective response to painful images over time. Collectively, our findings demonstrate that mind wandering can directly modulate the cortical processing of affectively salient stimulus inputs, serving in this case to reduce sensitivity to the physical discomfort of others.


Assuntos
Atenção/fisiologia , Encéfalo/fisiologia , Empatia/fisiologia , Percepção da Dor/fisiologia , Percepção Social , Percepção Visual/fisiologia , Eletroencefalografia , Potenciais Evocados , Feminino , Mãos , Humanos , Masculino , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Estimulação Luminosa , Fatores de Tempo , Adulto Jovem
8.
Brain Cogn ; 92C: 32-38, 2014 12.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25463137

RESUMO

An emerging theory proposes that visual attention operates in parallel at two distinct time scales - a shorter one (<1s) associated with moment-to-moment orienting of selective visuospatial attention, and a longer one (>10s) associated with more global aspects of attention-to-task. Given their parallel nature, here we examined whether these comparatively slower fluctuations in task-related attention show the same visual field asymmetry - namely, a right visual field bias - as often reported for selective visual-spatial attention. Participants performed a target detection task at fixation while event-related potentials (ERP) time-locked to task-irrelevant visual probes presented in the left and right visual fields were recorded. At random intervals, participants were asked to report whether they were "on-task" or "mind wandering". Our results demonstrated that sensory attenuation during periods of "mind wandering" relative to "on-task", as measured by the visual P1 ERP component at electrodes sites contralateral to the stimulus, was only observed for probes presented in the left visual field. In contrast, the magnitude of sensory gain in the right visual field was insensitive to whether participants were "on-task" or "mind wandering". Taken together, our results support the notion that task-related attention at longer time scales and spatial attention at shorter time scales affect the same underlying mechanism in visual cortex.

9.
Conscious Cogn ; 26: 51-63, 2014 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24674757

RESUMO

Recent research has shown that mind wandering recruits executive resources away from the external task towards inner thoughts. No studies however have determined whether executive functions are drawn away in a unitary manner during mind wandering episodes, or whether there is variation in specific functions impacted. Accordingly, we examined whether mind wandering differentially modulates three core executive functions-response inhibition, updating of working memory, and mental set shifting. In three experiments, participants performed one of these three executive function tasks and reported their attentional state as either on-task or mind wandering at random intervals. We found that mind wandering led to poorer performance in the response inhibition and working memory tasks, but not the set-shifting task. These findings suggest that mind wandering does not recruit executive functions in a monolithic manner. Rather, it appears to selectively engage certain executive functions, which may reflect the adaptive maintenance of ongoing task performance.


Assuntos
Atenção/fisiologia , Função Executiva/fisiologia , Inibição Psicológica , Memória de Curto Prazo/fisiologia , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Adulto Jovem
10.
J Alzheimers Dis Rep ; 8(1): 817-831, 2024.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38910947

RESUMO

Background: Slower walking is associated with changes in cortical volume and thickness. Computerized cognitive training (CCT) and exercise improve cortical volume and thickness and thus, may promote gait speed. Slowing of gait is predictive of Alzheimer's disease. Objective: To examine: 1) the effect of CCT, with or without physical exercise, on cortical volume and thickness and; 2) the association of changes in cortical volume and thickness with changes in gait speed. Methods: A subset of 124 adults (n = 53), aged 65-85 years, enrolled in an 8-week randomized controlled trial and completed T1-weighted MRI and 4-meter walk at baseline and 8 weeks. Participants were randomized to: 1) active control (BAT; n = 19); 2) CCT (n = 17); or 3) CCT preceded by exercise (Ex-CCT; n = 17). Change in cortical volume and thickness were assessed and compared across all groups using Freesurfer. RESULTS: BAT versus CCT increased left rostral middle frontal gyrus volume (p  = 0.027) and superior temporal gyrus thickness (p = 0.039). Ex-CCT versus CCT increased left cuneus thickness (p < 0.001) and right post central gyrus thickness (p = 0.005), and volume (p < 0.001). Ex-CCT versus BAT increased left (p = 0.001) and right (p = 0.020) superior parietal gyri thickness. There were no significant between-group differences in gait speed (p > 0.175). Increased left superior parietal volume (p = 0.036, r = 0.340) and thickness (p = 0.002, r = 0.348), right post central volume (p = .017, r = 0.341) and thickness (p = 0.001, r = 0.348), left banks of superior temporal sulcus thickness (p = 0.002, r = 0.356), and left precuneus thickness (p < 0.001, r = 0.346) were associated with increased gait speed. CONCLUSIONS: CCT with physical exercise, but not CCT alone, improves cortical volume and thickness in older adults. These changes may contribute to the maintenance of gait speed in aging.

11.
J Cogn Neurosci ; 25(6): 952-60, 2013 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23448525

RESUMO

Mind wandering is a natural, transient state wherein our neurocognitive systems become temporarily decoupled from the external sensory environment as our thoughts drift away from the current task at hand. Yet despite the ubiquity of mind wandering in everyday human life, we rarely seem impaired in our ability to adaptively respond to the external environment when mind wandering. This suggests that despite widespread neurocognitive decoupling during mind wandering states, we may nevertheless retain some capacity to attentionally monitor external events. But what specific capacities? In Experiment 1, using traditional performance measures, we found that both volitional and automatic forms of visual-spatial attentional orienting were significantly attenuated when mind wandering. In Experiment 2, however, ERPs revealed that, during mind wandering states, there was a relative preservation of sensitivity to deviant or unexpected sensory events, as measured via the auditory N1 component. Taken together, our findings suggest that, although some selective attentional processes may be subject to down-regulation during mind wandering, we may adaptively compensate for these neurocognitively decoupled states by maintaining automatic deviance-detection functions.


Assuntos
Atenção/fisiologia , Conscientização/fisiologia , Encéfalo/fisiologia , Eletroencefalografia/métodos , Adaptação Psicológica/fisiologia , Adulto , Eletroencefalografia/instrumentação , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Adulto Jovem
12.
Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci ; 13(4): 803-13, 2013 Dec.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23702836

RESUMO

Are we humans drawn to the forbidden? From jumbo-sized soft drinks to illicit substances, the influence of prohibited ownership on subsequent demand has made this question a pressing one. We know that objects that we ourselves own have a heightened psychological saliency, relative to comparable objects that are owned by others, but do these kinds of effects extend from self-owned to "forbidden" objects? To address this question, we developed a modified version of the Turk shopping paradigm in which "purchased" items were assigned to various recipients. Participants sorted everyday objects labeled as "self-owned", "other-owned," and either "forbidden to oneself" (Experiment 1) or "forbidden to everyone" (Experiment 2). Subsequent surprise recognition memory tests revealed that forbidden objects with high (Experiment 1) but not with low (Experiment 2) self-relevance were recognized as well as were self-owned objects, and better than other-owned objects. In a third and final experiment, we used event-related potentials (ERPs) to determine whether self-owned and self-forbidden objects, which showed a common memory advantage, are in fact treated the same at a neurocognitive-affective level. We found that both object types were associated with enhanced cognitive analysis, relative to other-owned objects, as measured by the P300 ERP component. However, we also found that self-forbidden objects uniquely triggered an enhanced response preceding the P300, in an ERP component (the N2) that is sensitive to more rapid, affect-related processing. Our findings thus suggest that, whereas self-forbidden objects share a common cognitive signature with self-owned objects, they are unique in being identified more quickly at a neurocognitive level.


Assuntos
Atenção/fisiologia , Memória/fisiologia , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos/fisiologia , Adolescente , Adulto , Análise de Variância , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Estimulação Luminosa , Tempo de Reação/fisiologia , Adulto Jovem
13.
Front Psychol ; 14: 1075953, 2023.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36925597

RESUMO

Inattention to one's on-going task leads to well-documented cognitive, behavioral, and physiological consequences. At the same time, the reliable association between mind-wandering and negative mood has suggested that there are affective consequences to task inattention as well. We examined this potential relationship between inattention and mood in the following study. Six hundred and fifty-five participants completed self-report questionnaires related to inattentive thinking (i.e., attentional lapses, daydreaming, mindfulness, rumination, reflection, worry, postevent processing, inattentiveness, and counterfactual thinking), a questionnaire about depressive symptoms, and a questionnaire about anxiety symptoms. First, an exploratory factor analysis was conducted to identify potential underlying constructs of types of inattentive thinking. Using ordinary least squares extraction and Oblimin rotation, a three-factor model demonstrated suitable fit, broadly representing mind-wandering/inattentive consequences, repetitive negative thinking, and reflective/introspective thinking. Second, after eliminating measures that did not strongly load on any factor, structural equation modeling was conducted and found that the relationship between mind-wandering and depression was partially explained by repetitive negative thinking, whereas the relationship between mind-wandering and anxiety was fully explained by repetitive negative thinking. The present findings suggest that understanding how inattentive thoughts are interrelated not only influences mood and affect but also reveals important considerations of intentionality, executive functioning, and qualitative styles of these thoughts.

14.
Am J Lifestyle Med ; 17(2): 258-275, 2023.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36896037

RESUMO

One new case of dementia is detected every 4 seconds and no effective drug therapy exists. Effective behavioural strategies to promote healthy cognitive ageing are thus essential. Three behaviours related to cognitive health which we all engage in daily are physical activity, sedentary behaviour and sleep. These time-use activity behaviours are linked to cognitive health in a complex and dynamic relationship not yet fully elucidated. Understanding how each of these behaviours is related to each other and cognitive health will help determine the most practical and effective lifestyle strategies for promoting healthy cognitive ageing. In this review, we discuss methods and analytical approaches to best investigate how these time-use activity behaviours are related to cognitive health. We highlight four key recommendations for examining these relationships such that researchers should include measures which (1) are psychometrically appropriate; (2) can specifically answer the research question; (3) include objective and subjective estimates of the behaviour and (4) choose an analytical method for modelling the relationships of time-use activity behaviours with cognitive health which is appropriate for their research question.

15.
Neuropsychologia ; 180: 108480, 2023 02 10.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36621593

RESUMO

Our attention often drifts away from the ongoing task to task-unrelated thoughts, a phenomenon commonly referred to as mind wandering. Ample studies dedicated to delineating its electrophysiological correlates have revealed distinct event-related potentials (ERP) and spectral patterns associated with mind wandering. It remains less clear whether the complexity of the electroencephalography (EEG) changes when our minds wander, a metric that captures the predictability of the time series at varying timescales. Accordingly, this study investigated whether mind wandering impacts EEG signal complexity. We further explored whether such effects differ across timescales, and change in a context-dependent manner as indexed by global and local levels of processing. To address this, we recorded participants' EEG while they completed Navon's global and local processing task and occasionally reported whether they were on-task or mind wandering throughout the task. We found that brain signal complexity as indexed by multiscale entropy decreased at medium timescales in centro-parietal regions and increased at coarse timescales in anterior and posterior regions during mind wandering, as compared to the on-task state, for global processing. Moreover, global processing showed increased complexity at fine to medium timescales compared to local processing. Finally, behavioral performance revealed a context-dependent effect in accuracy measures, with mind wandering showing lower accuracy compared to the on-task state only during the local condition. Taken together, these results indicate that changes in brain signal complexity across timescales may be an important feature of mind wandering.


Assuntos
Encéfalo , Eletroencefalografia , Humanos , Entropia , Encéfalo/fisiologia , Atenção/fisiologia , Potenciais Evocados
16.
BMC Neurol ; 12: 126, 2012 Oct 30.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23110387

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: Maintaining cognitive function is essential for healthy aging and to function autonomously within society. White matter lesions (WMLs) are associated with reduced cognitive function in older adults. However, whether their anatomical location moderates these associations is not well-established. This review systematically evaluates peer-reviewed evidence on the role of anatomical location in the association between WMLs and cognitive function. METHODS: In accordance with the preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analysis (PRISMA) statement, databases of EMBASE, PUBMED, MEDLINE, and CINAHL, and reference lists of selected papers were searched. We limited our search results to adults aged 60 years and older, and studies published in the English language from 2000 to 2011. Studies that investigated the association between cognitive function and WML location were included. Two independent reviewers extracted: 1) study characteristics including sample size, sample characteristic, and study design; 2) WML outcomes including WML location, WML quantification method (scoring or volume measurement), strength of the MRI magnet in Tesla, and MRI sequence used for WML detection; and 3) cognitive function outcomes including cognitive tests for two cognitive domains of memory and executive function/processing speed. RESULTS: Of the 14 studies included, seven compared the association of subcortical versus periventricular WMLs with cognitive function. Seven other studies investigated the association between WMLs in specific brain regions (e.g., frontal, parietal lobes) and cognitive function. Overall, the results show that a greater number of studies have found an association between periventricular WMLs and executive function/processing speed, than subcortical WMLs. However, whether WMLs in different brain regions have a differential effect on cognitive function remains unclear. CONCLUSIONS: Evidence suggests that periventricular WMLs may have a significant negative impact on cognitive abilities of older adults. This finding may be influenced by study heterogeneity in: 1) MRI sequences, WML quantification methods, and neuropsychological batteries; 2) modifying effect of cardiovascular risk factors; and 3) quality of studies and lack of sample size calculation.


Assuntos
Encéfalo/patologia , Transtornos Cognitivos/diagnóstico , Transtornos Cognitivos/epidemiologia , Imagem de Tensor de Difusão/estatística & dados numéricos , Leucoencefalopatias/diagnóstico , Leucoencefalopatias/epidemiologia , Fibras Nervosas Mielinizadas/patologia , Fatores Etários , Idoso , Comorbidade , Humanos , Incidência , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Fatores de Risco
17.
Anat Sci Educ ; 15(6): 1074-1085, 2022 Nov.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34694737

RESUMO

The use of mixed reality in science education has been increasing and as such it has become more important to understand how information is learned in these virtual environments. Spatial ability is important in many learning contexts, but especially in neuroanatomy education where learning the locations and spatial relationships between brain regions is paramount. It is currently unclear what role spatial ability plays in mixed reality learning environments, and whether it is different compared to traditional physical environments. To test this, a learning experiment was conducted where students learned neuroanatomy using both mixed reality and a physical plastic model of a brain (N = 27). Spatial ability was assessed and analyzed to determine its effect on performance across the two learning modalities. The results showed that spatial ability facilitated learning in mixed reality (ß = 0.21, P = 0.003), but not when using a plastic model (ß = 0.08, P = 0.318). A non-significant difference was observed between the modalities in terms of knowledge test performance (d = 0.39, P = 0.052); however, mixed reality was more engaging (d = 0.59, P = 0.005) and learners were more confident in the information they learned compared to using a physical model (d = 0.56, P = 0.007). Overall, these findings suggest that spatial ability is more relevant in virtual learning environments, where the ability to manipulate and interact with an object is diminished or abstracted through a virtual user interface.


Assuntos
Anatomia , Realidade Aumentada , Navegação Espacial , Humanos , Interface Usuário-Computador , Anatomia/educação , Aprendizagem
18.
Neurobiol Aging ; 109: 166-175, 2022 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34740078

RESUMO

White matter hyperintensities (WMH) are associated with greater falls risk and slow gait speed. Whether these deficits are caused by the disruption of large-scale functional networks remains inconclusive. Further, physical activity moderates the association between WMHs and falls, but whether this extends to the disruption of functional networks remains unknown. One hundred and sixty-four adults (>55 years old) were included in this study. Using lesion network mapping, we identified significant correlations between the percentage of WMH-related disruption of the dorsal attention network and Physiological Profile Assessment (PPA) score (r = 0.24, p < 0.01); and between disruption of both the sensorimotor (r = 0.23, p < 0.01) and ventral attention networks (r = 0.21, p = 0.01) with foam sway. There were no significant associations with floor sway or gait speed. Physical activity moderated the association between the dorsal attention network and PPA score (p = 0.045). Thus, future research should investigate whether physical activity should be recommended in the clinical management of older adults with cerebral small vessel disease.


Assuntos
Acidentes por Quedas , Rede Nervosa/patologia , Substância Branca/patologia , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Atenção/fisiologia , Exercício Físico/fisiologia , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Rede Nervosa/fisiopatologia , Risco , Córtex Sensório-Motor/fisiologia , Córtex Sensório-Motor/fisiopatologia , Velocidade de Caminhada , Substância Branca/fisiopatologia
19.
J Alzheimers Dis ; 90(2): 553-563, 2022.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36155502

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: White matter hyperintensities (WMH) are associated with impaired cognition and increased falls risk. Resistance training (RT) is a promising intervention to reduce WMH progression, improve executive functions, and reduce falls. However, the underlying neurobiological process by which RT improves executive functions and falls risk remain unclear. We hypothesized that: 1) RT reduces the level of WMH-related disruption to functional networks; and 2) reduced disruption to the sensorimotor and attention networks will be associated with improved executive function and reduced falls risk. OBJECTIVE: Investigate the impact of 52 weeks of RT on WMH-related disruption to functional networks. METHODS: Thirty-two older females (65-75 years) were included in this exploratory analysis of a 52-week randomized controlled trial. Participants received either twice-weekly RT or balance and tone training (control). We used lesion network mapping to assess changes in WMH-related disruption to the sensorimotor, dorsal attention, and ventral attention networks. Executive function was measured using the Stroop Colour-Word Test. Falls risk was assessed using the Physiological Profile Assessment (PPA) and the foam sway test. RESULTS: RT significantly reduced the level of WMH-related disruption to the sensorimotor network (p = 0.012). Reduced disruption to the dorsal attention network was associated with improvements in Stroop performance (r = 0.527, p = 0.030). Reduced disruption to the ventral attention network was associated with reduced PPA score (r = 0.485, p = 0.049)Conclusion:RT may be a promising intervention to mitigate WMH-related disruption to the sensorimotor network. Additionally, reducing disruption to the dorsal and ventral attention networks may contribute to improved executive function and reduced falls risk respectively.


Assuntos
Disfunção Cognitiva , Treinamento Resistido , Substância Branca , Humanos , Feminino , Idoso , Substância Branca/diagnóstico por imagem , Substância Branca/patologia , Função Executiva/fisiologia , Acidentes por Quedas/prevenção & controle , Disfunção Cognitiva/patologia , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética , Cognição/fisiologia
20.
J Cogn Neurosci ; 23(2): 460-70, 2011 Feb.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20146593

RESUMO

Top-down control of visual sensory cortex has long been tied to the orienting of visual spatial attention on a rapid, moment-to-moment basis. Here, we examined whether sensory responses in visual cortex are also modulated by natural and comparatively slower fluctuations in whether or not one is paying attention to the task at hand. Participants performed a simple visual discrimination task at fixation as the ERPs to task-irrelevant probes in the upper visual periphery were recorded. At random intervals, participants were stopped and asked to report on their attentional state at the time of stoppage-either "on-task" or "off-task." ERPs to the probes immediately preceding these subjective reports were then examined as a function of whether attention was in an on-task versus off-task state. We found that sensory-evoked responses to the probes were significantly attenuated during off-task relative to on-task states, as measured by the visual P1 ERP component. In two additional experiments, we replicated this effect while (1) finding that off-task sensory attenuation extends to the auditory domain, as measured by the auditory N1 ERP component, and (2) eliminating state-dependent shifts in general arousal as a possible explanation for the effects. Collectively, our findings suggest that sensory gain control in cortex is yoked to the natural ebb and flow in how much attention we pay to the current task over time.


Assuntos
Atenção/fisiologia , Cognição/fisiologia , Potenciais Evocados Auditivos/fisiologia , Potenciais Evocados Visuais/fisiologia , Córtex Visual/fisiologia , Campos Visuais/fisiologia , Estimulação Acústica/métodos , Mapeamento Encefálico , Eletroencefalografia/métodos , Feminino , Lateralidade Funcional/fisiologia , Humanos , Masculino , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Estimulação Luminosa/métodos , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Tempo de Reação/fisiologia , Percepção Espacial/fisiologia , Fatores de Tempo
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA