Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 9 de 9
Filtrar
Mais filtros

Base de dados
Tipo de documento
Intervalo de ano de publicação
1.
Cell ; 177(4): 999-1009.e10, 2019 05 02.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31051108

RESUMO

What specific features should visual neurons encode, given the infinity of real-world images and the limited number of neurons available to represent them? We investigated neuronal selectivity in monkey inferotemporal cortex via the vast hypothesis space of a generative deep neural network, avoiding assumptions about features or semantic categories. A genetic algorithm searched this space for stimuli that maximized neuronal firing. This led to the evolution of rich synthetic images of objects with complex combinations of shapes, colors, and textures, sometimes resembling animals or familiar people, other times revealing novel patterns that did not map to any clear semantic category. These results expand our conception of the dictionary of features encoded in the cortex, and the approach can potentially reveal the internal representations of any system whose input can be captured by a generative model.


Assuntos
Rede Nervosa/fisiologia , Lobo Temporal/fisiologia , Percepção Visual/fisiologia , Algoritmos , Animais , Córtex Cerebral/fisiologia , Macaca mulatta/fisiologia , Masculino , Neurônios/metabolismo , Neurônios/fisiologia
2.
Bioinformatics ; 39(1)2023 01 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36519840

RESUMO

SUMMARY: We present vembrane as a command line variant call format (VCF)/binary call format (BCF) filtering tool that consolidates and extends the filtering functionality of previous software to meet any imaginable filtering use case. Vembrane exposes the VCF/BCF file type specification and its inofficial extensions by the annotation tools VEP and SnpEff as Python data structures. vembrane filter enables filtration by Python expressions, requiring only basic knowledge of the Python programming language. vembrane table allows users to generate tables from subsets of annotations or functions thereof. Finally, it is fast, by using pysam and relying on lazy evaluation. AVAILABILITY AND IMPLEMENTATION: Source code and installation instructions are available at github.com/vembrane/vembrane (doi: 10.5281/zenodo.7003981). SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION: Supplementary data are available at Bioinformatics online.


Assuntos
Variação Genética , Transtornos Mentais , Humanos , Software , Linguagens de Programação
3.
J Neurophysiol ; 129(5): 1021-1044, 2023 05 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36947884

RESUMO

A central goal of systems neuroscience is to understand how populations of sensory neurons encode and relay information to the rest of the brain. Three key quantities of interest are 1) how mean neural activity depends on the stimulus (sensitivity), 2) how neural activity (co)varies around the mean (noise correlations), and 3) how predictive these variations are of the subject's behavior (choice probability). Previous empirical work suggests that both choice probability and noise correlations are affected by task training, with decision-related information fed back to sensory areas and aligned to neural sensitivity on a task-by-task basis. We used Utah arrays to record activity from populations of primary visual cortex (V1) neurons from two macaque monkeys that were trained to switch between two coarse orientation-discrimination tasks. Surprisingly, we find no evidence for significant trial-by-trial changes in noise covariance between tasks, nor do we find a consistent relationship between neural sensitivity and choice probability, despite recording from well-tuned task-sensitive neurons, many of which were histologically confirmed to be in supragranular V1, and despite behavioral evidence that the monkeys switched their strategy between tasks. Thus our data at best provide weak support for the hypothesis that trial-by-trial task-switching induces changes to noise correlations and choice probabilities in V1. However, our data agree with a recent finding of a single "choice axis" across tasks. They also raise the intriguing possibility that choice-related signals in early sensory areas are less indicative of task learning per se and instead reflect perceptual learning that occurs in highly overtrained subjects.NEW & NOTEWORTHY Converging evidence suggests that decision processes affect sensory neural activity, and this has informed numerous theories of neural processing. We set out to replicate and extend previous results on decision-related information and noise correlations in V1 of macaque monkeys. However, in our data, we find little evidence for a number of expected effects. Our null results therefore call attention to differences in task training, stimulus design, recording, and analysis techniques between our and prior studies.


Assuntos
Córtex Visual , Animais , Córtex Visual/fisiologia , Macaca mulatta/fisiologia , Aprendizagem , Neurônios/fisiologia , Neurônios Aferentes
4.
J Neurosci ; 37(3): 648-659, 2017 01 18.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28100746

RESUMO

Neurons in primate inferotemporal cortex (IT) are clustered into patches of shared image preferences. Functional imaging has shown that these patches are activated by natural categories (e.g., faces, body parts, and places), artificial categories (numerals, words) and geometric features (curvature and real-world size). These domains develop in the same cortical locations across monkeys and humans, which raises the possibility of common innate mechanisms. Although these commonalities could be high-level template-based categories, it is alternatively possible that the domain locations are constrained by low-level properties such as end-stopping, eccentricity, and the shape of the preferred images. To explore this, we looked for correlations among curvature preference, receptive field (RF) end-stopping, and RF eccentricity in the ventral stream. We recorded from sites in V1, V4, and posterior IT (PIT) from six monkeys using microelectrode arrays. Across all visual areas, we found a tendency for end-stopped sites to prefer curved over straight contours. Further, we found a progression in population curvature preferences along the visual hierarchy, where, on average, V1 sites preferred straight Gabors, V4 sites preferred curved stimuli, and many PIT sites showed a preference for curvature that was concave relative to fixation. Our results provide evidence that high-level functional domains may be mapped according to early rudimentary properties of the visual system. SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT: The macaque occipitotemporal cortex contains clusters of neurons with preferences for categories such as faces, body parts, and places. One common question is how these clusters (or "domains") acquire their cortical position along the ventral stream. We and other investigators previously established an fMRI-level correlation among these category domains, retinotopy, and curvature preferences: for example, in inferotemporal cortex, face- and curvature-preferring domains show a central visual field bias whereas place- and rectilinear-preferring domains show a more peripheral visual field bias. Here, we have found an electrophysiological-level explanation for the correlation among domain preference, curvature, and retinotopy based on neuronal preference for short over long contours, also called end-stopping.


Assuntos
Percepção de Forma/fisiologia , Orientação/fisiologia , Estimulação Luminosa/métodos , Córtex Visual/fisiologia , Vias Visuais/fisiologia , Potenciais de Ação/fisiologia , Animais , Previsões , Macaca mulatta , Masculino
5.
J Neurosci ; 31(29): 10437-44, 2011 Jul 20.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21775589

RESUMO

Perceptual stability requires the integration of information across eye movements. We first tested the hypothesis that motion signals are integrated by neurons whose receptive fields (RFs) do not move with the eye but stay fixed in the world. Specifically, we measured the RF properties of neurons in the middle temporal area (MT) of macaques (Macaca mulatta) during the slow phase of optokinetic nystagmus. Using a novel method to estimate RF locations for both spikes and local field potentials, we found that the location on the retina that changed spike rates or local field potentials did not change with eye position; RFs moved with the eye. Second, we tested the hypothesis that neurons link information across eye positions by remapping the retinal location of their RFs to future locations. To test this, we compared RF locations during leftward and rightward slow phases of optokinetic nystagmus. We found no evidence for remapping during slow eye movements; the RF location was not affected by eye-movement direction. Together, our results show that RFs of MT neurons and the aggregate activity reflected in local field potentials are yoked to the eye during slow eye movements. This implies that individual MT neurons do not integrate sensory information from a single position in the world across eye movements. Future research will have to determine whether such integration, and the construction of perceptual stability, takes place in the form of a distributed population code in eye-centered visual cortex or is deferred to downstream areas.


Assuntos
Mapeamento Encefálico , Nistagmo Optocinético/fisiologia , Córtex Visual/fisiologia , Campos Visuais/fisiologia , Potenciais de Ação/fisiologia , Animais , Macaca mulatta , Masculino , Método de Monte Carlo , Neurônios/fisiologia , Estimulação Luminosa/métodos , Tempo de Reação , Valores de Referência , Análise de Regressão , Córtex Visual/citologia , Vias Visuais/fisiologia
6.
Front Genet ; 13: 867018, 2022.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35711922

RESUMO

Occurrence of extra-chromosomal circular DNA is a phenomenon frequently observed in tumor cells, and the presence of such DNA has been recognized as a marker of adverse outcome across cancer types. We here describe a computational workflow for identification of DNA circles from long-read sequencing data. The workflow is implemented based on the Snakemake workflow management system. Its key step uses a graph-theoretic approach to identify putative circular fragments validated on simulated reads. We then demonstrate robustness of our approach using nanopore sequencing of selectively enriched circular DNA by highly sensitive and specific recovery of plasmids and the mitochondrial genome, which is the only circular DNA in normal human cells. Finally, we show that the workflow facilitates detection of larger circular DNA fragments containing extrachromosomal copies of the MYCN oncogene and the respective breakpoints, which is a potentially useful application in disease monitoring of several cancer types.

7.
Front Syst Neurosci ; 11: 13, 2017.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28377700

RESUMO

How we perceive the world as stable despite the frequent disruptions of the retinal image caused by eye movements is one of the fundamental questions in sensory neuroscience. Seemingly convergent evidence points towards a mechanism which dynamically updates representations of visual space in anticipation of a movement (Wurtz, 2008). In particular, receptive fields (RFs) of neurons, predominantly within oculomotor and attention related brain structures (Duhamel et al., 1992; Walker et al., 1995; Umeno and Goldberg, 1997), are thought to "remap" to their future, post-movement location prior to an impending eye movement. New studies (Neupane et al., 2016a,b) report observations on RF dynamics at the time of eye movements of neurons in area V4. These dynamics are interpreted as being largely dominated by a remapping of RFs. Critically, these observations appear at odds with a previous study reporting a different type of RF dynamics within the same brain structure (Tolias et al., 2001), consisting of a shrinkage and shift of RFs towards the movement target. Importantly, RFs have been measured with different techniques in those studies. Here, we measured V4 RFs comparable to Neupane et al. (2016a,b) and observe a shrinkage and shift of RFs towards the movement target when analyzing the immediate stimulus response (Zirnsak et al., 2014). When analyzing the late stimulus response (Neupane et al., 2016a,b), we observe RF shifts resembling remapping. We discuss possible causes for these shifts and point out important issues which future studies on RF dynamics need to address.

8.
Vision Res ; 111(Pt B): 161-9, 2015 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25449335

RESUMO

Most approaches to visual prostheses have focused on the retina, and for good reasons. The earlier that one introduces signals into the visual system, the more one can take advantage of its prodigious computational abilities. For methods that make use of microelectrodes to introduce electrical signals, however, the limited density and volume occupying nature of the electrodes place severe limits on the image resolution that can be provided to the brain. In this regard, non-retinal areas in general, and the primary visual cortex in particular, possess one large advantage: "magnification factor" (MF)-a value that represents the distance across a sheet of neurons that represents a given angle of the visual field. In the foveal representation of primate primary visual cortex, the MF is enormous-on the order of 15-20 mm/deg in monkeys and humans, whereas on the retina, the MF is limited by the optical design of the eye to around 0.3m m/deg. This means that, for an electrode array of a given density, a much higher-resolution image can be introduced into V1 than onto the retina (or any other visual structure). In addition to this tremendous advantage in resolution, visual cortex is plastic at many different levels ranging from a very local ability to learn to better detect electrical stimulation to higher levels of learning that permit human observers to adapt to radical changes to their visual inputs. We argue that the combination of the large magnification factor and the impressive ability of the cerebral cortex to learn to recognize arbitrary patterns, might outweigh the disadvantages of bypassing earlier processing stages and makes V1 a viable option for the restoration of vision.


Assuntos
Córtex Cerebral/fisiologia , Plasticidade Neuronal/fisiologia , Percepção Visual/fisiologia , Próteses Visuais , Animais , Potenciais Evocados Visuais/fisiologia , Fóvea Central/fisiologia , Haplorrinos , Humanos , Campos Visuais/fisiologia , Vias Visuais/fisiologia
9.
Front Syst Neurosci ; 8: 239, 2014.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25565992

RESUMO

The detection of visual motion requires temporal delays to compare current with earlier visual input. Models of motion detection assume that these delays reside in separate classes of slow and fast thalamic cells, or slow and fast synaptic transmission. We used a data-driven modeling approach to generate a model that instead uses recurrent network dynamics with a single, fixed temporal integration window to implement the velocity computation. This model successfully reproduced the temporal response dynamics of a population of motion sensitive neurons in macaque middle temporal area (MT) and its constituent parts matched many of the properties found in the motion processing pathway (e.g., Gabor-like receptive fields (RFs), simple and complex cells, spatially asymmetric excitation and inhibition). Reverse correlation analysis revealed that a simplified network based on first and second order space-time correlations of the recurrent model behaved much like a feedforward motion energy (ME) model. The feedforward model, however, failed to capture the full speed tuning and direction selectivity properties based on higher than second order space-time correlations typically found in MT. These findings support the idea that recurrent network connectivity can create temporal delays to compute velocity. Moreover, the model explains why the motion detection system often behaves like a feedforward ME network, even though the anatomical evidence strongly suggests that this network should be dominated by recurrent feedback.

SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA