Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 11 de 11
Filtrar
Mais filtros

Base de dados
Tipo de documento
Intervalo de ano de publicação
1.
Vision Res ; 47(17): 2272-80, 2007 Aug.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17617434

RESUMO

Oculomotor behavior contributes importantly to visual search. Saccadic eye movements can direct the fovea to potentially interesting parts of the visual field. Ensuing stable fixations enables the visual system to analyze those parts. The visual system may use fixation duration and saccadic amplitude as optimizers for visual search performance. Here we investigate whether the time courses of fixation duration and saccade amplitude depend on the subject's knowledge of the search stimulus, in particular target conspicuity. We analyzed 65,000 saccades and fixations in a search experiment for (possibly camouflaged) military vehicles of unknown type and size. Mean saccade amplitude decreased and mean fixation duration increased gradually as a function of the ordinal saccade and fixation number. In addition we analyzed 162,000 saccades and fixations recorded during a search experiment in which the location of the target was the only unknown. Whether target conspicuity was constant or varied appeared to have minor influence on the time courses of fixation duration and saccade amplitude. We hypothesize an intrinsic coarse-to-fine strategy for visual search that is even used when such a strategy is not optimal.


Assuntos
Fixação Ocular/fisiologia , Movimentos Sacádicos/fisiologia , Adulto , Feminino , Área de Dependência-Independência , Humanos , Masculino , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos/fisiologia , Estimulação Luminosa/métodos , Desempenho Psicomotor , Fatores de Tempo
2.
Vision Res ; 40(24): 3415-26, 2000.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11058738

RESUMO

Tasks such as reading or visual search consist of series of saccades. We have investigated to what extent saccades that are made within a series of self-paced movements are influenced by preceding movements. The present paper concerns an analysis of the duration of the fixations preceding saccades. We tested human subjects in a paradigm where they had to fixate two to four targets in a fixed order as fast as they could. We found that fixations before so-called 'return saccades' (saccades returning to the previously fixated position) are considerably longer (up to 40%) than other fixations. This phenomenon, which we call 'Inhibition of Saccade Return' (ISR), is present when return and regular saccades are mixed in one trial, and seems to be reset after each saccade. ISR is strongest at the previously fixated target, and decreases gradually from there. The radius of the area where ISR is found is about 4 degrees. The relation between ISR and 'Inhibition of Return' of spatial attention [Posner & Cohen, 1984] is discussed, as well as the neurophysiological basis of ISR.


Assuntos
Inibição Neural/fisiologia , Movimentos Sacádicos/fisiologia , Adulto , Feminino , Fixação Ocular/fisiologia , Humanos , Masculino , Psicofísica , Fatores de Tempo
3.
Vision Res ; 39(8): 1567-75, 1999 Apr.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10343822

RESUMO

The present study concerns the dynamics of multiple fixation search. We tried to gain insight into: (1) how the peripheral and foveal stimulus affect fixation duration; and (2) how fixation duration affects the peripheral target selection for saccades. We replicated the non-corroborating results of Luria and Strauss (1975) ('Eye movements during search for coded and uncoded targets', Perception and Psychophysics 17, 303-308) (saccades were selective), and Zelinsky (1996) (Using eye movements to assess the selectivity of search movements. Vision research 36(14), 2177-2187) (saccades were not selective), by manipulating the critical features for peripheral selection and discrimination separately. We found search to be more selective and efficient when the selection task was easy or when fixations were long-lasting. Remarkably, subjects did not increase their fixation durations when the peripheral selection task was more difficult. Only the discrimination task affected the fixation duration. This implies that the time available for peripheral target selection is determined mainly by the discrimination task. The results of the present experiment suggest that, besides the difficulty of the peripheral selection task, fixation duration is an important factor determining the selection of potential targets for eye movements.


Assuntos
Movimentos Sacádicos/fisiologia , Campos Visuais , Adulto , Fixação Ocular , Fóvea Central , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos/fisiologia , Fatores de Tempo
4.
Vision Res ; 38(9): 1295-302, 1998 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9666997

RESUMO

To investigate whether fixation durations are adjusted to the duration of a foveal analysis task, we designed a search task in which each stimulus element yielded information about the position of the target. We asked subjects to look for the target by making eye movements in the direction indicated by each stimulus element. We explicitly asked the subjects to make the eye movements in the correct direction, but they did not always do this. They made only 65-80% of the eye movements in directions indicated by the stimulus elements. From these results we conclude that fixation durations are not solely determined by the immediate visual stimulus and that subjects encounter difficulties when trying to increase fixation durations to values that would enable them to direct saccades accurately. In a second experiment we shortened the presentation time in order to provide an incentive for the subjects to speed up search. Shortening the presentation time did not affect fixation duration. Therefore, we suggest that fixation duration is controlled by a mechanism that uses estimations of the foveal analysis time of previous fixated stimulus elements.


Assuntos
Fixação Ocular , Movimentos Sacádicos , Adulto , Fóvea Central/fisiologia , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Fatores de Tempo , Percepção Visual/fisiologia
5.
Vision Res ; 41(25-26): 3505-11, 2001.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11718791

RESUMO

It has been suggested that attention can disambiguate stimuli that have equal motion energy in opposite directions (e.g. a counterphasing grating), such that a clear motion direction is perceived. The direction of this movement is determined by the observer and can be changed at will. Assuming that the responses of front-end motion detectors are equal for the two opponent directions, it has been proposed that the unambiguous motion perceived with attentive tracking arises from an independent mechanism that monitors the shifts of attention directed to the moving feature of interest. However, while perceiving motion under attentive tracking conditions, observers often report a strong impression that they are making eye movements. In this study, we investigated whether systematic eye movements are present during attentive tracking and, as a result, could be responsible for the subjective experience of movement. We had observers track an object in smooth motion, apparent motion and ambiguous motion, either with eye movements or with attention. The results show that there are negligible eye movements during attentive tracking, which are neither systematic nor correlated with the stimulus. Given that neither eye movements nor retinal image motion can account for subjectively perceived motion, as well as the absence of any other plausible explanation, we find it tempting evidence for an earlier suggestion that the percept of movement must arise from a specialized mechanism.


Assuntos
Atenção/fisiologia , Movimentos Oculares/fisiologia , Percepção de Movimento/fisiologia , Humanos , Masculino
6.
J Vis ; 1(2): 55-79, 2001.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12678602

RESUMO

Rotating a surface about a horizontal axis alters the retinal horizontal-shear disparities. Opposed torsional eye movements (cyclovergence) also change horizontal shear. If there were no compensation for the horizontal disparities created by cyclovergence, slant estimates would be erroneous. We asked whether compensation for cyclovergence occurs, and, if it does, whether it occurs by use of an extraretinal cyclovergence signal, by use of vertical-shear disparities, or by use of both signals. In four experiments, we found that compensation is nearly veridical when vertical-shear disparities are available and easily measured. When they are not available or easily measured, no compensation occurs. Thus, the visual system does not seem to use an extraretinal cyclovergence signal in stereoscopic slant estimation. We also looked for evidence of an extraretinal cyclovergence signal in a visual direction task and found none. We calculated the statistical reliabilities of slant-from-disparity and slant-from-texture estimates and found that the more reliable of the two means of estimation varies significantly with distance and slant. Finally, we examined how slant about a horizontal axis might be estimated when the eyes look eccentrically.


Assuntos
Percepção de Profundidade/fisiologia , Orientação , Percepção Visual/fisiologia , Convergência Ocular/fisiologia , Humanos , Disparidade Visual/fisiologia , Visão Binocular/fisiologia
7.
Transl Psychiatry ; 2: e115, 2012 May 15.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22832959

RESUMO

Recent rodent research has shown that the basolateral amygdala (BLA) inhibits unconditioned, or innate, fear. It is, however, unknown whether the BLA acts in similar ways in humans. In a group of five subjects with a rare genetic syndrome, that is, Urbach-Wiethe disease (UWD), we used a combination of structural and functional neuroimaging, and established focal, bilateral BLA damage, while other amygdala sub-regions are functionally intact. We tested the translational hypothesis that these BLA-damaged UWD-subjects are hypervigilant to facial expressions of fear, which are prototypical innate threat cues in humans. Our data indeed repeatedly confirm fear hypervigilance in these UWD subjects. They show hypervigilant responses to unconsciously presented fearful faces in a modified Stroop task. They attend longer to the eyes of dynamically displayed fearful faces in an eye-tracked emotion recognition task, and in that task recognize facial fear significantly better than control subjects. These findings provide the first direct evidence in humans in support of an inhibitory function of the BLA on the brain's threat vigilance system, which has important implications for the understanding of the amygdala's role in the disorders of fear and anxiety.


Assuntos
Tonsila do Cerebelo/fisiopatologia , Ansiedade/fisiopatologia , Dano Encefálico Crônico/fisiopatologia , Medo/fisiologia , Proteinose Lipoide de Urbach e Wiethe/fisiopatologia , Inibição Neural/fisiologia , Adulto , Ansiedade/genética , Ansiedade/patologia , Atenção , Dano Encefálico Crônico/genética , Dano Encefálico Crônico/patologia , Mapeamento Encefálico , Calcinose/genética , Calcinose/patologia , Calcinose/fisiopatologia , Discriminação Psicológica/fisiologia , Dominância Cerebral/fisiologia , Emoções/fisiologia , Expressão Facial , Feminino , Humanos , Interpretação de Imagem Assistida por Computador , Proteinose Lipoide de Urbach e Wiethe/genética , Proteinose Lipoide de Urbach e Wiethe/patologia , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Fatores de Crescimento Neural , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos/fisiologia , Reconhecimento Psicológico/fisiologia , Valores de Referência , Teste de Stroop , Estimulação Subliminar
8.
Exp Brain Res ; 170(3): 387-402, 2006 Apr.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16328267

RESUMO

Five experiments are presented, providing empirical support of the hypothesis that the sensory phenomenon of lateral masking may explain many well-known visual search phenomena that are commonly assumed to be governed by cognitive attentional mechanisms. Experiment I showed that when the same visual arrays are used in visual search and in lateral masking experiments, the factors (1) number of distractors, (2) distractor density, and (3) search type (conjunction vs disjunction) have the same effect on search times as they have on lateral masking scores. Experiment II showed that when the number of distractors and eccentricity is kept constant in a search task, the effect of reducing density (which reduces the lateral masking potential of distractors on the target) is to strongly reduce the disjunction-conjunction difference. In experiment III, the lateral masking potential of distractors on a target was measured with arrays that typically yield asymmetric search times in visual search studies (a Q among Os vs. an O among Qs). The lateral masking scores showed the same asymmetry. Experiment IV was a visual search study with such asymmetric search arrays in which the number of distractors and eccentricity was kept constant, while manipulating density. Reducing density (i.e., reducing lateral masking) produced a strong reduction of the asymmetry effect. Finally in experiment V, we showed that the data from experiment IV cannot be explained due to a difference between a fine and a coarse grain attentional mechanism. Taken together with eye movement data and error scores from experiment II and with similar findings from the literature, these results suggest that the sensory mechanism of lateral masking could well be a very important (if not the main) factor causing many of the well-known effects that are traditionally attributed to higher level cognitive or attentional mechanisms in visual search.


Assuntos
Atenção , Movimentos Oculares , Lateralidade Funcional , Mascaramento Perceptivo/fisiologia , Visão Ocular/fisiologia , Aprendizagem por Discriminação , Humanos , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos , Estimulação Luminosa/métodos , Psicofísica , Tempo de Reação
9.
Percept Psychophys ; 58(7): 969-76, 1996 Oct.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8920834

RESUMO

To obtain insight into the control of fixation duration during visual search, we had 4 subjects perform simple search tasks in which we systematically varied the discriminability of the target. The experiment was carried out under two conditions. Under the first condition (blocked), the discriminability of the target was kept constant during a session. Under the second condition (mixed), the discriminability of the target varied per trial. Under the blocked condition, fixation duration increased with decreasing discriminability. For 2 subjects, we found much shorter fixation durations in difficult trials with the mixed condition than in difficult trials with the blocked condition. Overall, the subjects fixated the target, continued to search, and then went back to the target in 5%-55% of the correct trials. In these trials, the result of the analysis of the foveal target was not used for preparing the next saccade. The results support a preprogramming model of the control of fixation duration. In a simple search task, control of fixation duration appears to be indirect.


Assuntos
Atenção , Aprendizagem por Discriminação , Fixação Ocular , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos , Adulto , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Orientação , Psicofísica , Tempo de Reação , Movimentos Sacádicos
10.
J Neurophysiol ; 83(5): 2757-75, 2000 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10805674

RESUMO

Cyclovergence is a simultaneously occurring cyclorotation of the two eyes in opposite directions. Cyclovergence can be elicited visually by opposite cyclorotation of the two eyes' images. It also can occur in conjunction with horizontal vergence and vertical version in a stereotyped manner as described by the extended Listing's law (or L2). We manipulated L2-related and visually evoked cyclovergence independently, using stereoscopic images of three-dimensional (3D) scenes. During pursuit in the midsagittal plane, cyclovergence followed L2. The amount of L2-related cyclovergence during pursuit varied between subjects. Each pursuit trial was repeated three times. Two of the three trials had additional image rotation to visually evoke cyclovergence. We could separate the L2-related and visual components of cyclovergence by subtraction of the cyclovergence response in matched trials that differed only in the image rotation that was applied during pursuit. This indicates that visual and L2-related contributions to cyclovergence add linearly, suggesting the presence of two independent systems. Visually evoked cyclovergence gains were characteristic for a given subject, little affected by visual stimulus parameters, and usually low (0.1-0.5) when a static target was fixated. Gain and phase lag of the visually evoked cyclovergence during vertical pursuit was comparable with that during fixation of a static target. The binocular orientations are in better agreement to orientations predicted by L2 then would be predicted by nulling of the cyclodisparities. On the basis of our results, we suggest that visually driven and L2-related cyclovergence are independent of each other and superimpose linearly.


Assuntos
Convergência Ocular/fisiologia , Percepção de Profundidade/fisiologia , Fixação Ocular/fisiologia , Adulto , Calibragem , Eletroculografia/instrumentação , Eletroculografia/métodos , Humanos , Masculino , Óptica e Fotônica , Acompanhamento Ocular Uniforme/fisiologia , Processamento de Sinais Assistido por Computador
11.
Exp Brain Res ; 129(4): 615-28, 1999 Dec.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10638435

RESUMO

When we move along we frequently look around. How quickly and accurately can we gaze in the direction of heading? We studied the temporal aspects of heading perception in expanding and contracting patterns simulating self-motion. Center of flow (CF) eccentricity was 15 degrees. Subjects had to indicate the CF by making a saccade to it. A temporal constraint on the response time was introduced, because stimuli were presented briefly (1 s). On average, subjects needed two saccades to find the CF. Initial saccades covered about 50-60% of the distance between the fixation point and the CF. Subjects underestimated the eccentricity of the CF. The systematic radial error ranged from -2.4 degrees to -4.9 degrees. The systematic tangential error was small (about 0.5 degree). The variable radial error ranged from 2.7 degrees to 4.6 degrees. We found a relation between saccade onset time and saccade endpoint error. Saccade endpoint error decreased with increasing saccade onset time, suggesting that saccades were often fired before the heading processing had been completed. From the saccade onset times, saccade endpoint errors and an estimate for the saccadic dead time (interval prior to the saccade during which modification is impossible 70 ms), we estimated the heading processing time (HPT 0.43 s). In three out of four subjects, HPT was longer for trials simulating backward movement than for trials simulating forward movement. For each saccade we determined whether it reduced the distance error. The second saccade reduced the error more effectively per time unit than the initial saccade. On the basis of this finding, we suggest that visual processing that occurs during the saccadic dead time of the first saccade is used in the preparation of the second saccade.


Assuntos
Percepção de Movimento/fisiologia , Movimentos Sacádicos/fisiologia , Adulto , Fixação Ocular/fisiologia , Humanos , Masculino , Matemática , Orientação/fisiologia , Estimulação Luminosa , Tempo de Reação/fisiologia , Volição/fisiologia
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA