Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 127
Filtrar
1.
Nat Rev Neurosci ; 24(7): 431-450, 2023 07.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37253949

RESUMO

Artificial neural networks (ANNs) inspired by biology are beginning to be widely used to model behavioural and neural data, an approach we call 'neuroconnectionism'. ANNs have been not only lauded as the current best models of information processing in the brain but also criticized for failing to account for basic cognitive functions. In this Perspective article, we propose that arguing about the successes and failures of a restricted set of current ANNs is the wrong approach to assess the promise of neuroconnectionism for brain science. Instead, we take inspiration from the philosophy of science, and in particular from Lakatos, who showed that the core of a scientific research programme is often not directly falsifiable but should be assessed by its capacity to generate novel insights. Following this view, we present neuroconnectionism as a general research programme centred around ANNs as a computational language for expressing falsifiable theories about brain computation. We describe the core of the programme, the underlying computational framework and its tools for testing specific neuroscientific hypotheses and deriving novel understanding. Taking a longitudinal view, we review past and present neuroconnectionist projects and their responses to challenges and argue that the research programme is highly progressive, generating new and otherwise unreachable insights into the workings of the brain.


Assuntos
Encéfalo , Redes Neurais de Computação , Humanos , Encéfalo/fisiologia
2.
Nat Rev Neurosci ; 23(6): 361-375, 2022 06.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35444305

RESUMO

Mapping human brain function is a long-standing goal of neuroscience that promises to inform the development of new treatments for brain disorders. Early maps of human brain function were based on locations of brain damage or brain stimulation that caused a functional change. Over time, this approach was largely replaced by technologies such as functional neuroimaging, which identify brain regions in which activity is correlated with behaviours or symptoms. Despite their advantages, these technologies reveal correlations, not causation. This creates challenges for interpreting the data generated from these tools and using them to develop treatments for brain disorders. A return to causal mapping of human brain function based on brain lesions and brain stimulation is underway. New approaches can combine these causal sources of information with modern neuroimaging and electrophysiology techniques to gain new insights into the functions of specific brain areas. In this Review, we provide a definition of causality for translational research, propose a continuum along which to assess the relative strength of causal information from human brain mapping studies and discuss recent advances in causal brain mapping and their relevance for developing treatments.


Assuntos
Encefalopatias , Neurociências , Encéfalo/fisiologia , Mapeamento Encefálico/métodos , Humanos , Neuroimagem/métodos
3.
PLoS Comput Biol ; 19(9): e1011484, 2023 09.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37768890

RESUMO

The brain learns representations of sensory information from experience, but the algorithms by which it does so remain unknown. One popular theory formalizes representations as inferred factors in a generative model of sensory stimuli, meaning that learning must improve this generative model and inference procedure. This framework underlies many classic computational theories of sensory learning, such as Boltzmann machines, the Wake/Sleep algorithm, and a more recent proposal that the brain learns with an adversarial algorithm that compares waking and dreaming activity. However, in order for such theories to provide insights into the cellular mechanisms of sensory learning, they must be first linked to the cell types in the brain that mediate them. In this study, we examine whether a subtype of cortical interneurons might mediate sensory learning by serving as discriminators, a crucial component in an adversarial algorithm for representation learning. We describe how such interneurons would be characterized by a plasticity rule that switches from Hebbian plasticity during waking states to anti-Hebbian plasticity in dreaming states. Evaluating the computational advantages and disadvantages of this algorithm, we find that it excels at learning representations in networks with recurrent connections but scales poorly with network size. This limitation can be partially addressed if the network also oscillates between evoked activity and generative samples on faster timescales. Consequently, we propose that an adversarial algorithm with interneurons as discriminators is a plausible and testable strategy for sensory learning in biological systems.


Assuntos
Interneurônios , Aprendizagem , Aprendizagem/fisiologia , Encéfalo , Algoritmos , Sono
4.
J Exp Biol ; 225(6)2022 03 15.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35142362

RESUMO

Healthy young adults have a most preferred walking speed, step length and step width that are close to energetically optimal. However, people can choose to walk with a multitude of different step lengths and widths, which can vary in both energy expenditure and preference. Here, we further investigated step length-width preferences and their relationship to energy expenditure. In line with a growing body of research, we hypothesized that people's preferred stepping patterns would not be fully explained by metabolic energy expenditure. To test this hypothesis, we used a two-alternative forced-choice paradigm. Fifteen participants walked on an oversized treadmill. Each trial, participants performed two prescribed stepping patterns and then chose the pattern they preferred. Over time, we adapted the choices such that there was 50% chance of choosing one pattern over another (equally preferred). If people's preferences are based solely on metabolic energy expenditure, then these equally preferred stepping patterns should have equal energy expenditure. In contrast, we found that energy expenditure differed across equally preferred step length-width patterns (P<0.001). On average, longer steps with higher energy expenditure were preferred over shorter and wider steps with lower energy expenditure (P<0.001). We also asked participants to rank a set of shorter, wider and longer steps from most preferred to least preferred, and from most energy expended to least energy expended. Only 7/15 participants had the same rankings for their preferences and perceived energy expenditure. Our results suggest that energy expenditure is not the only factor influencing a person's conscious gait choices.


Assuntos
Marcha , Caminhada , Fenômenos Biomecânicos , Metabolismo Energético , Teste de Esforço , Humanos , Adulto Jovem
5.
Cereb Cortex ; 30(3): 1957-1973, 2020 03 14.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31647525

RESUMO

Prior knowledge about our environment influences our actions. How does this knowledge evolve into a final action plan and how does the brain represent this? Here, we investigated this question in the monkey oculomotor system during self-guided search of natural scenes. In the frontal eye field (FEF), we found a subset of neurons, "Early neurons," that contain information about the upcoming saccade long before it is executed, often before the previous saccade had even ended. Crucially, much of this early information did not relate to the actual saccade that would eventually be selected. Rather, it related to prior information about the probabilities of possible upcoming saccades based on the presaccade fixation location. Nearer to the time of saccade onset, a greater proportion of these neurons' activities related to the saccade selection, although prior information continued to influence activity throughout. A separate subset of FEF neurons, "Late neurons," only represented the final action plan near saccade onset and not prior information. Our results demonstrate how, across the population of FEF neurons, prior information evolves into definitive saccade plans.


Assuntos
Atenção/fisiologia , Lobo Frontal/fisiologia , Memória/fisiologia , Campos Visuais/fisiologia , Percepção Visual/fisiologia , Potenciais de Ação/fisiologia , Animais , Fixação Ocular/fisiologia , Neurônios/fisiologia , Estimulação Luminosa/métodos , Tempo de Reação/fisiologia
6.
J Med Internet Res ; 23(9): e22844, 2021 09 03.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34477562

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: The assessment of behaviors related to mental health typically relies on self-report data. Networked sensors embedded in smartphones can measure some behaviors objectively and continuously, with no ongoing effort. OBJECTIVE: This study aims to evaluate whether changes in phone sensor-derived behavioral features were associated with subsequent changes in mental health symptoms. METHODS: This longitudinal cohort study examined continuously collected phone sensor data and symptom severity data, collected every 3 weeks, over 16 weeks. The participants were recruited through national research registries. Primary outcomes included depression (8-item Patient Health Questionnaire), generalized anxiety (Generalized Anxiety Disorder 7-item scale), and social anxiety (Social Phobia Inventory) severity. Participants were adults who owned Android smartphones. Participants clustered into 4 groups: multiple comorbidities, depression and generalized anxiety, depression and social anxiety, and minimal symptoms. RESULTS: A total of 282 participants were aged 19-69 years (mean 38.9, SD 11.9 years), and the majority were female (223/282, 79.1%) and White participants (226/282, 80.1%). Among the multiple comorbidities group, depression changes were preceded by changes in GPS features (Time: r=-0.23, P=.02; Locations: r=-0.36, P<.001), exercise duration (r=0.39; P=.03) and use of active apps (r=-0.31; P<.001). Among the depression and anxiety groups, changes in depression were preceded by changes in GPS features for Locations (r=-0.20; P=.03) and Transitions (r=-0.21; P=.03). Depression changes were not related to subsequent sensor-derived features. The minimal symptoms group showed no significant relationships. There were no associations between sensor-based features and anxiety and minimal associations between sensor-based features and social anxiety. CONCLUSIONS: Changes in sensor-derived behavioral features are associated with subsequent depression changes, but not vice versa, suggesting a directional relationship in which changes in sensed behaviors are associated with subsequent changes in symptoms.


Assuntos
Depressão , Smartphone , Adulto , Ansiedade/diagnóstico , Ansiedade/epidemiologia , Transtornos de Ansiedade , Depressão/diagnóstico , Depressão/epidemiologia , Feminino , Humanos , Estudos Longitudinais , Masculino
7.
J Neuroeng Rehabil ; 18(1): 124, 2021 08 10.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34376199

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: Falls are a leading cause of accidental deaths and injuries worldwide. The risk of falling is especially high for individuals suffering from balance impairments. Retrospective surveys and studies of simulated falling in lab conditions are frequently used and are informative, but prospective information about real-life falls remains sparse. Such data are essential to address fall risks and develop fall detection and alert systems. Here we present the results of a prospective study investigating a proof-of-concept, smartphone-based, online system for fall detection and notification. METHODS: The system uses the smartphone's accelerometer and gyroscope to monitor the participants' motion, and falls are detected using a regularized logistic regression. Data on falls and near-fall events (i.e., stumbles) is stored in a cloud server and fall-related variables are logged onto a web portal developed for data exploration, including the event time and weather, fall probability, and the faller's location and activity before the fall. RESULTS: In total, 23 individuals with an elevated risk of falling carried the phones for 2070 days in which the model classified 14,904,000 events. The system detected 27 of the 37 falls that occurred (sensitivity = 73.0 %) and resulted in one false alarm every 46 days (specificity > 99.9 %, precision = 37.5 %). 42.2 % of the events falsely classified as falls were validated as stumbles. CONCLUSIONS: The system's performance shows the potential of using smartphones for fall detection and notification in real-life. Apart from functioning as a practical fall monitoring instrument, this system may serve as a valuable research tool, enable future studies to scale their ability to capture fall-related data, and help researchers and clinicians to investigate real-falls.


Assuntos
Acidentes por Quedas , Smartphone , Humanos , Sistemas On-Line , Estudos Prospectivos , Estudos Retrospectivos
8.
Nucleic Acids Res ; 46(13): e78, 2018 07 27.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29718339

RESUMO

DNA polymerase fidelity is affected by both intrinsic properties and environmental conditions. Current strategies for measuring DNA polymerase error rate in vitro are constrained by low error subtype sensitivity, poor scalability, and lack of flexibility in types of sequence contexts that can be tested. We have developed the Magnification via Nucleotide Imbalance Fidelity (MagNIFi) assay, a scalable next-generation sequencing assay that uses a biased deoxynucleotide pool to quantitatively shift error rates into a range where errors are frequent and hence measurement is robust, while still allowing for accurate mapping to error rates under typical conditions. This assay is compatible with a wide range of fidelity-modulating conditions, and enables high-throughput analysis of sequence context effects on base substitution and single nucleotide deletion fidelity using a built-in template library. We validate this assay by comparing to previously established fidelity metrics, and use it to investigate neighboring sequence-mediated effects on fidelity for several DNA polymerases. Through these demonstrations, we establish the MagNIFi assay for robust, high-throughput analysis of DNA polymerase fidelity.


Assuntos
DNA Polimerase Dirigida por DNA/metabolismo , Sequenciamento de Nucleotídeos em Larga Escala/métodos , Análise de Sequência de DNA/métodos , Desoxirribonucleotídeos/metabolismo
9.
J Neurophysiol ; 122(1): 389-397, 2019 07 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31091169

RESUMO

During sensorimotor tasks, subjects use sensory feedback but also prior information. It is often assumed that the sensorimotor prior is just given by the experiment and that the details for acquiring this prior (e.g., the effector) are irrelevant. However, recent research has suggested that the construction of priors is nontrivial. To test if the sensorimotor details matter for the construction of a prior, we designed two tasks that differ only in the effectors that subjects use to indicate their estimate. For both a typical reaching setting and an atypical wrist rotation setting, prior and feedback uncertainty matter as quantitatively predicted by Bayesian statistics. However, in violation of simple Bayesian models, the importance of the prior differs across effectors. Subjects overly rely on their prior in the typical reaching case compared with the wrist case. The brain is not naively Bayesian with a single and veridical prior. NEW & NOTEWORTHY Traditional Bayesian models often assume that we learn statistics of movements and use the information as a prior to guide subsequent movements. The effector is merely a reporting modality for information processing. We asked subjects to perform a visuomotor learning task with different effectors (finger or wrist). Surprisingly, we found that prior information is used differently between the effectors, suggesting that learning of the prior is related to the movement context such as the effector involved or that naive models of Bayesian behavior need to be extended.


Assuntos
Modelos Neurológicos , Destreza Motora , Córtex Sensório-Motor/fisiologia , Análise e Desempenho de Tarefas , Adulto , Teorema de Bayes , Feminino , Mãos/inervação , Mãos/fisiologia , Humanos , Masculino , Percepção Visual
10.
J Neurophysiol ; 121(1): 61-73, 2019 01 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30379603

RESUMO

Whether one is delicately placing a contact lens on the surface of the eye or lifting a heavy weight from the floor, the motor system must produce a wide range of forces under different dynamical loads. How does the motor cortex, with neurons that have a limited activity range, function effectively under these widely varying conditions? In this study, we explored the interaction of activity in primary motor cortex (M1) and muscles (electromyograms, EMGs) of two male rhesus monkeys for wrist movements made during three tasks requiring different dynamical loads and forces. Despite traditionally providing adequate predictions in single tasks, in our experiments, a single linear model failed to account for the relation between M1 activity and EMG across conditions. However, a model with a gain parameter that increased with the target force remained accurate across forces and dynamical loads. Surprisingly, this model showed that a greater proportion of EMG changes were explained by the nonlinear gain than the linear mapping from M1. In addition to its theoretical implications, the strength of this nonlinearity has important implications for brain-computer interfaces (BCIs). If BCI decoders are to be used to control movement dynamics (including interaction forces) directly, they will need to be nonlinear and include training data from broad data sets to function effectively across tasks. Our study reinforces the need to investigate neural control of movement across a wide range of conditions to understand its basic characteristics as well as translational implications. NEW & NOTEWORTHY We explored the motor cortex-to-electromyogram (EMG) mapping across a wide range of forces and loading conditions, which we found to be highly nonlinear. A greater proportion of EMG was explained by a nonlinear gain than a linear mapping. This nonlinearity allows motor cortex to control the wide range of forces encountered in the real world. These results unify earlier observations and inform the next-generation brain-computer interfaces that will control movement dynamics and interaction forces.


Assuntos
Eletromiografia , Contração Isométrica/fisiologia , Córtex Motor/fisiologia , Movimento/fisiologia , Músculo Esquelético/fisiologia , Potenciais de Ação , Animais , Interfaces Cérebro-Computador , Eletrodos Implantados , Modelos Lineares , Macaca mulatta , Masculino , Neurônios/fisiologia , Dinâmica não Linear , Processamento de Sinais Assistido por Computador , Torque , Punho/fisiologia
11.
J Comput Neurosci ; 45(3): 173-191, 2018 12.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30294750

RESUMO

Prominent models of spike trains assume only one source of variability - stochastic (Poisson) spiking - when stimuli and behavior are fixed. However, spike trains may also reflect variability due to internal processes such as planning. For example, we can plan a movement at one point in time and execute it at some arbitrary later time. Neurons involved in planning may thus share an underlying time course that is not precisely locked to the actual movement. Here we combine the standard Linear-Nonlinear-Poisson (LNP) model with Dynamic Time Warping (DTW) to account for shared temporal variability. When applied to recordings from macaque premotor cortex, we find that time warping considerably improves predictions of neural activity. We suggest that such temporal variability is a widespread phenomenon in the brain which should be modeled.


Assuntos
Potenciais de Ação/fisiologia , Modelos Neurológicos , Neurônios/fisiologia , Dinâmica não Linear , Animais , Humanos , Distribuição de Poisson , Fatores de Tempo
12.
PLoS Comput Biol ; 13(1): e1005142, 2017 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28081134

RESUMO

Perception is seen as a process that utilises partial and noisy information to construct a coherent understanding of the world. Here we argue that the experience of pain is no different; it is based on incomplete, multimodal information, which is used to estimate potential bodily threat. We outline a Bayesian inference model, incorporating the key components of cue combination, causal inference, and temporal integration, which highlights the statistical problems in everyday perception. It is from this platform that we are able to review the pain literature, providing evidence from experimental, acute, and persistent phenomena to demonstrate the advantages of adopting a statistical account in pain. Our probabilistic conceptualisation suggests a principles-based view of pain, explaining a broad range of experimental and clinical findings and making testable predictions.


Assuntos
Modelos Neurológicos , Modelos Estatísticos , Percepção da Dor/fisiologia , Teorema de Bayes , Humanos , Dor/fisiopatologia
13.
PLoS Comput Biol ; 13(5): e1005483, 2017 05.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28459860

RESUMO

Using a DNA polymerase to record intracellular calcium levels has been proposed as a novel neural recording technique, promising massive-scale, single-cell resolution monitoring of large portions of the brain. This technique relies on local storage of neural activity in strands of DNA, followed by offline analysis of that DNA. In simple implementations of this scheme, the time when each nucleotide was written cannot be determined directly by post-hoc DNA sequencing; the timing data must be estimated instead. Here, we use a Dynamic Time Warping-based algorithm to perform this estimation, exploiting correlations between neural activity and observed experimental variables to translate DNA-based signals to an estimate of neural activity over time. This algorithm improves the parallelizability of traditional Dynamic Time Warping, allowing several-fold increases in computation speed. The algorithm also provides a solution to several critical problems with the molecular recording paradigm: determining recording start times and coping with DNA polymerase pausing. The algorithm can generally locate DNA-based records to within <10% of a recording window, allowing for the estimation of unobserved incorporation times and latent neural tunings. We apply our technique to an in silico motor control neuroscience experiment, using the algorithm to estimate both timings of DNA-based data and the directional tuning of motor cortical cells during a center-out reaching task. We also use this algorithm to explore the impact of polymerase characteristics on system performance, determining the precision of a molecular recorder as a function of its kinetic and error-generating properties. We find useful ranges of properties for DNA polymerase-based recorders, providing guidance for future protein engineering attempts. This work demonstrates a useful general extension to dynamic alignment algorithms, as well as direct applications of that extension toward the development of molecular recorders, providing a necessary stepping stone for future biological work.


Assuntos
Algoritmos , Biologia Computacional/métodos , DNA , Nucleotídeos , Cálcio/análise , Cálcio/metabolismo , Simulação por Computador , DNA/análise , DNA/química , DNA/metabolismo , DNA Polimerase Dirigida por DNA/metabolismo , Modelos Biológicos , Neurônios/metabolismo , Neurociências , Nucleotídeos/análise , Nucleotídeos/metabolismo , Análise de Célula Única , Fatores de Tempo
14.
J Neurophysiol ; 117(2): 728-737, 2017 02 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27881721

RESUMO

Each of our movements is selected from any number of alternative movements. Some studies have shown evidence that the central nervous system (CNS) chooses to make the specific movements that are least affected by motor noise. Previous results showing that the CNS has a natural tendency to minimize the effects of noise make the direct prediction that if the relationship between movements and noise were to change, the specific movements people learn to make would also change in a predictable manner. Indeed, this has been shown for well-practiced movements such as reaching. Here, we artificially manipulated the relationship between movements and visuomotor noise by adding noise to a motor task in a novel redundant geometry such that there arose a single control policy that minimized the noise. This allowed us to see whether, for a novel motor task, people could learn the specific control policy that minimized noise or would need to employ other compensation strategies to overcome the added noise. As predicted, subjects were able to learn movements that were biased toward the specific ones that minimized the noise, suggesting not only that the CNS can learn to minimize the effects of noise in a novel motor task but also that artificial visuomotor noise can be a useful tool for teaching people to make specific movements. Using noise as a teaching signal promises to be useful for rehabilitative therapies and movement training with human-machine interfaces. NEW & NOTEWORTHY: Many theories argue that we choose to make the specific movements that minimize motor noise. Here, by changing the relationship between movements and noise, we show that people actively learn to make movements that minimize noise. This not only provides direct evidence for the theories of noise minimization but presents a way to use noise to teach specific movements to improve rehabilitative therapies and human-machine interface control.


Assuntos
Aprendizagem/fisiologia , Movimento/fisiologia , Ruído , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Adulto , Análise de Variância , Feminino , Generalização Psicológica , Humanos , Masculino , Adulto Jovem
15.
J Med Internet Res ; 19(4): e118, 2017 04 18.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28420605

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: Sleep is a critical aspect of people's well-being and as such assessing sleep is an important indicator of a person's health. Traditional methods of sleep assessment are either time- and resource-intensive or suffer from self-reporting biases. Recently, researchers have started to use mobile phones to passively assess sleep in individuals' daily lives. However, this work remains in its early stages, having only examined relatively small and homogeneous populations in carefully controlled contexts. Thus, it remains an open question as to how well mobile device-based sleep monitoring generalizes to larger populations in typical use cases. OBJECTIVE: The aim of this study was to assess the ability of machine learning algorithms to detect the sleep start and end times for the main sleep period in a 24-h cycle using mobile devices in a diverse sample. METHODS: We collected mobile phone sensor data as well as daily self-reported sleep start and end times from 208 individuals (171 females; 37 males), diverse in age (18-66 years; mean 39.3), education, and employment status, across the United States over 6 weeks. Sensor data consisted of geographic location, motion, light, sound, and in-phone activities. No specific instructions were given to the participants regarding phone placement. We used random forest classifiers to develop both personalized and global predictors of sleep state from the phone sensor data. RESULTS: Using all available sensor features, the average accuracy of classifying whether a 10-min segment was reported as sleep was 88.8%. This is somewhat better than using the time of day alone, which gives an average accuracy of 86.9%. The accuracy of the model considerably varied across the participants, ranging from 65.1% to 97.3%. We found that low accuracy in some participants was due to two main factors: missing sensor data and misreports. After correcting for these, the average accuracy increased to 91.8%, corresponding to an average median absolute deviation (MAD) of 38 min for sleep start time detection and 36 min for sleep end time. These numbers are close to the range reported by previous research in more controlled situations. CONCLUSIONS: We find that mobile phones provide adequate sleep monitoring in typical use cases, and that our methods generalize well to a broader population than has previously been studied. However, we also observe several types of data artifacts when collecting data in uncontrolled settings. Some of these can be resolved through corrections, but others likely impose a ceiling on the accuracy of sleep prediction for certain subjects. Future research will need to focus more on the understanding of people's behavior in their natural settings in order to develop sleep monitoring tools that work reliably in all cases for all people.


Assuntos
Telefone Celular/instrumentação , Polissonografia/instrumentação , Sono/fisiologia , Adolescente , Adulto , Idoso , Coleta de Dados , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Polissonografia/métodos , Adulto Jovem
16.
J Med Internet Res ; 19(4): e143, 2017 04 28.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30578218

RESUMO

[This corrects the article DOI: 10.2196/jmir.6821.].

17.
J Vis ; 17(3): 12, 2017 03 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28355625

RESUMO

There are three prominent factors that can predict human visual-search behavior in natural scenes: the distinctiveness of a location (salience), similarity to the target (relevance), and features of the environment that predict where the object might be (context). We do not currently know how well these factors are able to predict macaque visual search, which matters because it is arguably the most popular model for asking how the brain controls eye movements. Here we trained monkeys to perform the pedestrian search task previously used for human subjects. Salience, relevance, and context models were all predictive of monkey eye fixations and jointly about as precise as for humans. We attempted to disrupt the influence of scene context on search by testing the monkeys with an inverted set of the same images. Surprisingly, the monkeys were able to locate the pedestrian at a rate similar to that for upright images. The best predictions of monkey fixations in searching inverted images were obtained by rotating the results of the model predictions for the original image. The fact that the same models can predict human and monkey search behavior suggests that the monkey can be used as a good model for understanding how the human brain enables natural-scene search.


Assuntos
Movimentos Oculares/fisiologia , Fixação Ocular/fisiologia , Macaca mulatta/fisiologia , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos/fisiologia , Animais , Meio Ambiente , Feminino , Humanos , Modelos Teóricos
18.
Behav Brain Sci ; 40: e272, 2017 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29342693

RESUMO

Lake et al. suggest that current AI systems lack the inductive biases that enable human learning. However, Lake et al.'s proposed biases may not directly map onto mechanisms in the developing brain. A convergence of fields may soon create a correspondence between biological neural circuits and optimization in structured architectures, allowing us to systematically dissect how brains learn.


Assuntos
Cognição , Aprendizagem , Encéfalo , Humanos , Pensamento
19.
J Neurophysiol ; 116(2): 645-57, 2016 08 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27169506

RESUMO

When a saccade is expected to result in a reward, both neural activity in oculomotor areas and the saccade itself (e.g., its vigor and latency) are altered (compared with when no reward is expected). As such, it is unclear whether the correlations of neural activity with reward indicate a representation of reward beyond a movement representation; the modulated neural activity may simply represent the differences in motor output due to expected reward. Here, to distinguish between these possibilities, we trained monkeys to perform a natural scene search task while we recorded from the frontal eye field (FEF). Indeed, when reward was expected (i.e., saccades to the target), FEF neurons showed enhanced responses. Moreover, when monkeys accidentally made eye movements to the target, firing rates were lower than when they purposively moved to the target. Thus, neurons were modulated by expected reward rather than simply the presence of the target. We then fit a model that simultaneously included components related to expected reward and saccade parameters. While expected reward led to shorter latency and higher velocity saccades, these behavioral changes could not fully explain the increased FEF firing rates. Thus, FEF neurons appear to encode motivational factors such as reward expectation, above and beyond the kinematic and behavioral consequences of imminent reward.


Assuntos
Lobo Frontal/fisiologia , Neurônios/fisiologia , Recompensa , Movimentos Sacádicos/fisiologia , Campos Visuais/fisiologia , Potenciais de Ação/fisiologia , Animais , Feminino , Lobo Frontal/citologia , Modelos Lineares , Macaca mulatta , Tempo de Reação/fisiologia , Estatísticas não Paramétricas
20.
J Neurophysiol ; 116(3): 1328-43, 2016 09 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27250912

RESUMO

When we search for visual objects, the features of those objects bias our attention across the visual landscape (feature-based attention). The brain uses these top-down cues to select eye movement targets (spatial selection). The frontal eye field (FEF) is a prefrontal brain region implicated in selecting eye movements and is thought to reflect feature-based attention and spatial selection. Here, we study how FEF facilitates attention and selection in complex natural scenes. We ask whether FEF neurons facilitate feature-based attention by representing search-relevant visual features or whether they are primarily involved in selecting eye movement targets in space. We show that search-relevant visual features are weakly predictive of gaze in natural scenes and additionally have no significant influence on FEF activity. Instead, FEF activity appears to primarily correlate with the direction of the upcoming eye movement. Our result demonstrates a concrete need for better models of natural scene search and suggests that FEF activity during natural scene search is explained primarily by spatial selection.


Assuntos
Atenção/fisiologia , Movimentos Oculares/fisiologia , Percepção Espacial/fisiologia , Percepção Visual/fisiologia , Potenciais de Ação , Animais , Área Sob a Curva , Medições dos Movimentos Oculares , Feminino , Modelos Lineares , Macaca mulatta , Microeletrodos , Modelos Neurológicos , Atividade Motora/fisiologia , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Estimulação Luminosa , Curva ROC
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA