Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 32
Filtrar
1.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 117(18): 9808-9814, 2020 05 05.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32300013

RESUMO

Subtle features of common language can imply to young children that scientists are a special and distinct kind of person-a way of thinking that can interfere with the development of children's own engagement with science. We conducted a large field experiment (involving 45 prekindergarten schools, 130 teachers, and over 1,100 children) to test if targeting subtle properties of language can increase science engagement in children's daily lives. Despite strong tendencies to describe scientists as a special kind of person (in a baseline control condition), brief video-based training changed the language that teachers used to introduce science to their students. These changes in language were powerful enough to predict children's science interest and behavior days later. Thus, subtle features of language shape children's beliefs and behaviors as they unfold in real world environments. Harnessing these mechanisms could promote science engagement in early childhood.


Assuntos
Desenvolvimento Infantil/fisiologia , Educação/normas , Idioma , Ciência/educação , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Instituições Acadêmicas , Estudantes/psicologia
2.
Health Rep ; 33(6): 3-16, 2022 06 15.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35876612

RESUMO

Background: Estimates of polypharmacy have primarily been derived from prescription claims, and less is known about the use of non-prescription medications (alone or in combination with prescription medications) across the frailty spectrum or by sex. Our objectives were to estimate the prevalence of polypharmacy (total, prescription, non-prescription, and concurrent prescription and non-prescription) overall, and by frailty, sex and broad age group. Data: Canadian Health Measures Survey, Cycle 5, 2016 to 2017. Methods: Among Canadians aged 40 to 79 years, all prescription and non-prescription medications used in the month prior to the survey were documented. Polypharmacy was defined as using five or more medications total (prescription and non-prescription), prescription only and non-prescription only. Concurrent prescription and non-prescription use was defined as two or more and three or more of each. Frailty was defined using a 31-item frailty index (FI) and categorized as non-frail (FI ≤ 0.1) and pre-frail or frail (FI > 0.1). Survey-weighted descriptive statistics were calculated overall and age standardized. Results: We analyzed 2,039 respondents, representing 16,638,026 Canadians (mean age of 56.9 years; 51% women). Overall, 52.4% (95% confidence interval [CI] = 47.3 to 57.4) were defined as pre-frail or frail. Age-standardized estimates of total polypharmacy, prescription polypharmacy and concurrent prescription and non-prescription medication use were significantly higher among pre-frail or frail versus non-frail adults (e.g., total polypharmacy: 64.1% versus 31.8%, respectively). Polypharmacy with non-prescription medications was common overall (20.5% [95% CI = 16.1 to 25.8]) and greater among women, but did not differ significantly by frailty. Interpretation: Polypharmacy and concurrent prescription and non-prescription medication use were common among Canadian adults, especially those who were pre-frail or frail. Our findings highlight the importance of considering non-prescribed medications when measuring the exposure to medications and the potential risk for adverse outcomes.


Assuntos
Fragilidade , Idoso , Canadá/epidemiologia , Feminino , Idoso Fragilizado , Fragilidade/epidemiologia , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Polimedicação , Prevalência
3.
Child Dev ; 92(4): e531-e547, 2021 07.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33511701

RESUMO

A problematic way to think about social categories is to essentialize them-to treat particular differences between people as marking fundamentally distinct social kinds. From where do these beliefs arise? Language that expresses generic claims about categories elicits some aspects of essentialism, but the scope of these effects remains unclear. This study (N = 204, ages 4.5-8 years, 73% White; recruited predominantly from the United States and the United Kingdom to participate online in 2019) found that generic language increases two critical aspects of essentialist thought: Beliefs that (a) category-related properties arise from intrinsic causal mechanisms and (b) category boundaries are inflexible. These findings have implications for understanding the spread of essentialist beliefs across communities and the development of intergroup behavior.


Assuntos
Idioma , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Humanos , Reino Unido , Estados Unidos
4.
Psychol Sci ; 30(3): 455-466, 2019 03.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30721119

RESUMO

The roots of gender disparities in science achievement take hold in early childhood. The present studies aimed to identify a modifiable feature of young children's environments that could be targeted to reduce gender differences in science behavior among young children. Four experimental studies with children ( N = 501) revealed that describing science in terms of actions ("Let's do science! Doing science means exploring the world!") instead of identities ("Let's be scientists! Scientists explore the world!") increased girls' subsequent persistence in new science games designed to illustrate the scientific method. These studies thus identified subtle but powerful linguistic cues that could be targeted to help reduce gender disparities in science engagement in early childhood.


Assuntos
Cognição/fisiologia , Linguística/tendências , Motivação/fisiologia , Ciência/educação , Estudantes/psicologia , Logro , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Sinais (Psicologia) , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Fatores Sexuais , Estudantes/estatística & dados numéricos
5.
Dev Sci ; 22(6): e12837, 2019 11.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31006163

RESUMO

Over the course of middle childhood, children's interest and beliefs about their own capacities for success in science often decline. This pernicious decline is especially evident among underrepresented groups, including girls, members of some racial and ethnic minorities, and children from lower socioeconomic backgrounds. The present research (N = 306, ages 6-11) found that while children lose interest and feelings of efficacy about their potential to "be scientists" across middle childhood, they maintain more robust interest and efficacy about "doing science." These patterns were confirmed in both longitudinal and cross-sectional analyses; effects were stable or increased across time and age. Mediation analyses revealed that the positive effect of action framing is partially accounted for by children's views that the group of people who do science is more inclusive than the category of scientists. These findings suggest that using action-focused language to encourage children in science is more inclusive and may lead to more science engagement across middle childhood than language that emphasizes scientists as an identity category. Implications for educational practices will be discussed.


Assuntos
Pessoal de Laboratório/psicologia , Idioma , Ciência , Logro , Criança , Estudos Transversais , Feminino , Humanos , Estudos Longitudinais , Masculino , Processos Mentais
6.
Dev Sci ; 21(1)2018 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28229563

RESUMO

Psychological essentialism is a pervasive conceptual bias to view categories as reflecting something deep, stable, and informative about their members. Scholars from diverse disciplines have long theorized that psychological essentialism has negative ramifications for inter-group relations, yet little previous empirical work has experimentally tested the social implications of essentialist beliefs. Three studies (N = 127, ages 4.5-6) found that experimentally inducing essentialist beliefs about a novel social category led children to share fewer resources with category members, but did not lead to the out-group dislike that defines social prejudice. These findings indicate that essentialism negatively influences some key components of inter-group relations, but does not lead directly to the development of prejudice.


Assuntos
Preconceito/psicologia , Percepção Social , Estereotipagem , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino
7.
Child Dev ; 89(1): 148-155, 2018 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28129455

RESUMO

Classifying people into categories not only helps humans simplify a complex social world but also contributes to stereotyping and discrimination. This research examines how social categorization develops by testing how language imbues with meaning otherwise arbitrary differences between people. Experimental studies (N = 129) with 2-year-olds showed that generic language-language that refers to abstract kinds-guides the development of social categorization. Toddlers learned a new category after hearing generic language about individuals who shared an arbitrary perceptual feature but not after hearing matched specific language, simple labels, or plural (but nongeneric) language about the same set of individuals. These findings show how subtle linguistic cues shape the development of social categorization.


Assuntos
Desenvolvimento Infantil/fisiologia , Formação de Conceito/fisiologia , Idioma , Percepção Social , Pré-Escolar , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino
8.
BMC Geriatr ; 18(1): 211, 2018 09 12.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30208884

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: The identification of contextual factors that modify associations between client frailty and their health and service use outcomes is essential for informed home health care and policy planning. Our objective was to examine variation in the associations between frailty and select 1-year health outcomes by caregiver distress and client sex among community-residing older care recipients. METHODS: We conducted a retrospective cohort study using linked population-based clinical and health administrative databases for all long-stay home care clients (n = 234,552) aged 66+ years assessed during April 2010-2013 in Ontario, Canada. Frailty was assessed using a previously validated 72-item frailty index (FI). Presence of caregiver distress was derived from clinical assessment items administered by trained home care assessors. Multivariable log-binomial regression models were used to examine variations in the associations between frailty and outcomes of interest (mortality, nursing home [NH] placement, all-cause and prolonged hospitalization) by caregiver distress, with further model stratification by client sex. RESULTS: Frailty prevalence varied little by sex (19.3% women, 19.9% men) despite significant sex-differences in clients' sociodemographic and health characteristics. In both sexes, frailty was significantly associated with all outcomes, particularly NH placement (RR = 3.84, 95%CI 3.75-3.93) and death (RR = 2.32, 95%CI 2.27-2.37), though risk ratios were greater for women. Caregiver distress was more common with increasing frailty and for male clients, and a significant independent predictor of NH placement and prolonged hospitalization in both sexes. The association between frailty and NH placement (but not other outcomes) varied by caregiver distress for both men and women (p < 0.001 interaction terms), showing a greater magnitude of association among clients without (vs. with) a distressed caregiver. CONCLUSIONS: As caregiver distress varies by client sex, represents a key driver of NH placement (even among relatively robust clients), and modifies the impact of other risk factors such as frailty, it should be routinely assessed. Further, sex-differences should be considered when developing and evaluating community-based services for older adults and their caregivers.


Assuntos
Cuidadores/psicologia , Idoso Fragilizado , Casas de Saúde , Estresse Psicológico/epidemiologia , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Análise de Variância , Feminino , Avaliação Geriátrica , Humanos , Masculino , Avaliação de Resultados em Cuidados de Saúde , Prevalência , Estudos Retrospectivos , Fatores de Risco , Fatores Sexuais
10.
J Child Lang ; 43(6): 1231-44, 2016 Nov.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26333824

RESUMO

Generic language ( Owls eat at night) expresses knowledge about categories and may represent a cognitively default mode of generalization. English-speaking children and adults more accurately recall generic than quantified sentences ( All owls eat at night) and tend to recall quantified sentences as generic. However, generics in English are shorter than quantified sentences, and may be better recalled for this reason. The present study provided a new test of the issue in Spanish, where generics are expressed with an additional linguistic element not found in certain quantified sentences ( Los búhos comen de noche 'Owls eat at night' [generic] vs. Muchos búhos comen de noche 'Many owls eat at night' [quantified]). Both preschoolers and adults recalled generics more accurately than quantified sentences, and quantified sentences were more often recalled as generic than the reverse. These findings provide strong additional evidence for generics as a cognitive default, in an understudied cultural context.


Assuntos
Generalização Psicológica , Desenvolvimento da Linguagem , Idioma , Linguística , Rememoração Mental , Semântica , Adolescente , Adulto , Criança , Linguagem Infantil , Pré-Escolar , Formação de Conceito , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Adulto Jovem
11.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 109(34): 13526-31, 2012 Aug 21.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22869722

RESUMO

Social essentialism entails the belief that certain social categories (e.g., gender, race) mark fundamentally distinct kinds of people. Essentialist beliefs have pernicious consequences, supporting social stereotyping and contributing to prejudice. How does social essentialism develop? In the studies reported here, we tested the hypothesis that generic language facilitates the cultural transmission of social essentialism. Two studies found that hearing generic language about a novel social category diverse for race, ethnicity, age, and sex led 4-y-olds and adults to develop essentialist beliefs about that social category. A third study documented that experimentally inducing parents to hold essentialist beliefs about a novel social category led them to produce more generic language when discussing the category with their children. Thus, generic language facilitates the transmission of essentialist beliefs about social categories from parents to children.


Assuntos
Cultura , Identificação Social , Estereotipagem , Adolescente , Adulto , Pré-Escolar , Cognição , Etnicidade , Feminino , Humanos , Idioma , Masculino , Preconceito , Comportamento Social , Percepção Social , Adulto Jovem
12.
Cogn Psychol ; 64(3): 186-214, 2012 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22225996

RESUMO

Generics are sentences such as "ravens are black" and "tigers are striped", which express generalizations concerning kinds. Quantified statements such as "all tigers are striped" or "most ravens are black" also express generalizations, but unlike generics, they specify how many members of the kind have the property in question. Recently, some theorists have proposed that generics express cognitively fundamental/default generalizations, and that quantified statements in contrast express cognitively more sophisticated generalizations (Gelman, 2010; Leslie, 2008). If this hypothesis is correct, then quantified statements may be remembered as generics. This paper presents four studies with 136 preschool children and 118 adults, demonstrating that adults and preschoolers alike tend to recall quantified statements as generics, thus supporting the hypothesis that generics express cognitively default generalizations.


Assuntos
Desenvolvimento Infantil/fisiologia , Formação de Conceito/fisiologia , Generalização Psicológica/fisiologia , Rememoração Mental/fisiologia , Adulto , Pré-Escolar , Medicina Baseada em Evidências , Humanos , Testes Psicológicos , Semântica , Adulto Jovem
13.
Child Dev ; 83(2): 423-33, 2012.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22235892

RESUMO

Generic statements (e.g., "Lions have manes") make claims about kinds (e.g., lions as a category) and, for adults, are distinct from quantificational statements (e.g., "Most lions have manes"), which make claims about how many individuals have a given property. This article examined whether young children also understand that generics do not depend purely on quantitative information. Five-year-olds (n = 36) evaluated pairs of questions expressing properties that were matched in prevalence but varied in whether adults accept them as generically true (e.g., "Do lions have manes?" [true] vs. "Are lions boys?" [false]). Results demonstrated that children evaluate generics based on more than just quantitative information. Data suggest that even young children recognize that generics make claims about kinds.


Assuntos
Aprendizagem por Associação , Generalização Psicológica , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos , Semântica , Fatores Etários , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Feminino , Humanos , Julgamento , Desenvolvimento da Linguagem , Masculino , Revelação da Verdade , Adulto Jovem
14.
Sci Am ; 317(3): 60-65, 2017 Aug 15.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28813383
15.
Cogn Sci ; 46(12): e13223, 2022 12.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36537717

RESUMO

Generic language (e.g., "tigers have stripes") leads children to assume that the referenced category (e.g., tigers) is inductively informative and provides a causal explanation for the behavior of individual members. In two preregistered studies with 4- to 7-year-old children (N = 497), we considered the mechanisms underlying these effects by testing how correcting generics might affect the development of these beliefs about novel social and animal kinds (Study 1) and about gender (Study 2). Correcting generics by narrowing their scope to a single individual limited beliefs that the referenced categories could explain what their members would be like while broadening the scope to a superordinate category (Study 2) uniquely limited endorsement of gender norms. Across both studies, correcting generics did not alter beliefs about feature heritability and had mixed effects on inductive inferences, suggesting that additional mechanisms (e.g., causal reasoning about shared features) contribute to the development of full-blown essentialist beliefs. These results help illuminate the mechanisms by which generics lead children to view categories as having rich inductive and causal potential; in particular, they suggest that children interpret generics as signals that speakers in their community view the referenced categories as meaningful kinds that support generalization. The findings also point the way to concrete suggestions for how adults can effectively correct problematic generics (e.g., gender stereotypes) that children may hear in daily life.


Assuntos
Idioma , Resolução de Problemas , Animais , Criança , Humanos , Desenvolvimento Infantil , Generalização Psicológica
16.
Dev Psychol ; 58(8): 1455-1471, 2022 Aug.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35446067

RESUMO

Language that uses noun labels and generic descriptions to discuss people who do science (e.g., "Let's be scientists! Scientists discover new things") signals to children that "scientists" is a distinctive category. This identity-focused language promotes essentialist beliefs and leads to disengagement from science among young children in experimental contexts. The extent to which these cues shape the development of children's beliefs and behaviors in daily life, however, depends on (a) the availability of identity-focused language in children's environments and (b) the power of these cues to shape beliefs over time, even in the noisier, more variable contexts in which children are exposed to them. Documenting the availability of this language, linguistic coding of children's media (Study 1) and prekindergarten teachers' language from one science lesson (Study 2; n = 103; 98 female, one male, four unknown; 66% White, 8% African American, 6% Asian/Asian American, 3% mixed/biracial; 21% of the sample, of any race, identified as Hispanic/Latinx) confirmed that identity-focused language was the most common form of science language in these two samples. Further, children (Study 3; n = 83; Mage = 4.36 years; 43 female, 40 male; 64% White, 12% Asian/Asian American, 24% mixed/biracial; 36% of the sample, of any race, identified as Hispanic/Latinx) who were exposed to lower proportions of identity-focused language from their teachers developed increasingly inclusive science beliefs and greater science engagement over time. These findings suggest that linguistic input is an important mechanism through which exclusive beliefs about science are conveyed to children in daily life. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).


Assuntos
Idioma , Linguística , Negro ou Afro-Americano , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Sinais (Psicologia) , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino
17.
J Health Care Poor Underserved ; 31(3): 1379-1398, 2020.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33416701

RESUMO

Our goal was to develop a patient-centered text-message intervention for adolescent females in an urban safety-net health system. We conducted interviews with adolescent females to explore sexual health knowledge and inform the development of a text-messaging intervention. Focused group discussions (FGDs) verified or challenged interview themes and elicited preferences for intervention design. Forty-two females participated, including 15 interviewees and 27 FGD participants. Over half (67%) were Hispanic/Latina, 19% Black, 10% White and 5% Asian. The average age was 16 (±1.5) and 55% reported ever having sex. Participants felt susceptible to and were more concerned with preventing unintended pregnancies than sexually transmitted infections, and described more barriers to condom use than other contraceptive methods. Their input informed the development of a text-messaging intervention, which is described. This study supports the acceptability of a patient-centered texting intervention for promoting and normalizing healthy sexual behaviors among adolescent females in an urban safety-net setting.


Assuntos
Saúde Sexual , Infecções Sexualmente Transmissíveis , Envio de Mensagens de Texto , Adolescente , Feminino , Humanos , Motivação , Gravidez , Comportamento Sexual , Infecções Sexualmente Transmissíveis/prevenção & controle
18.
Acad Pediatr ; 20(4): 475-484, 2020.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31560971

RESUMO

OBJECTIVE: To evaluate the feasibility, acceptability, and initial efficacy of a pilot texting intervention ("t4she") in primary care designed to increase sexual health knowledge and promote dual protection strategies to reduce unintended pregnancies and sexually transmitted infections among adolescent females. METHODS: Participants were recruited from 2 federally qualified health centers. Eligibility included: being 13 to 18 years of age; assigned female at birth; English-speaking; not currently pregnant and/or intending to become pregnant; and having texting capabilities. A randomized controlled trial assessed between-group differences at 3 and 6 months on knowledge, Health Belief Model constructs, and sexual behaviors. Input on intervention acceptability was obtained at 3 months. RESULTS: Among 244 participants enrolled and randomized, the average age was 16 (±1.6), 80% were Hispanic/Latina, 53% had ever had vaginal sex, and 50% had used prescription birth control with 24% currently using a long-acting reversible method. Among those sexually active, 29% reported consistent condom use and 24% reported engaging in dual protection behaviors at last sex. Among participants with all follow-up data (N = 136), intervention participants had significant increases in sexual health knowledge and reported more prescription birth control use at follow-up than control participants. No significant outcome differences were found for condom use or dual protection behaviors. Intervention participants reported receiving messages, being introduced to new information, and reading and sharing the messages. CONCLUSIONS: The pilot t4she sexual health intervention significantly improved knowledge and use of short-acting prescription birth control among young females in primary care and was acceptable by youth and feasible to implement.


Assuntos
Saúde Sexual , Infecções Sexualmente Transmissíveis , Envio de Mensagens de Texto , Adolescente , Anticoncepção , Feminino , Humanos , Recém-Nascido , Gravidez , Comportamento Sexual , Infecções Sexualmente Transmissíveis/prevenção & controle
19.
Am Psychol ; 73(9): 1139-1153, 2018 12.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30525794

RESUMO

Despite the numerous intellectual contributions made by women, we find evidence of bias against them in contexts that emphasize intellectual ability. In the first experiment, 347 participants were asked to refer individuals for a job. Approximately half of the participants were led to believe that the job required high-level intellectual ability; the other half were not. A Bayesian mixed-effects logistic regression revealed that the odds of referring a woman were 38.3% lower when the job description mentioned intellectual ability, consistent with the possibility of gender bias. We also found evidence of gender bias in Experiment 2, which was a preregistered direct replication of Experiment 1 with a larger and more diverse sample (811 participants; 44.6% people of color). Experiment 3 provided a developmental investigation of this bias by testing whether young children favor boys over girls in the context of intellectually challenging activities. Five- to 7-year-olds (N = 192) were taught how to play a team game. Half of the children were told that the game was for "really, really smart" children; the other half were not. Children then selected 3 teammates from among 6 unfamiliar children. Children's initial selections were driven by ingroup bias (i.e., girls chose girls and boys chose boys), but children subsequently showed bias against girls, choosing girls as teammates for the "smart" game only 37.6% of the time (vs. 53.4% for the other game). Bias against women and girls in contexts where brilliance is prized emerges early and is a likely obstacle to their success. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2018 APA, all rights reserved).


Assuntos
Inteligência , Sexismo/psicologia , Estereotipagem , Adulto , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Psicologia da Criança
20.
Dev Psychol ; 53(6): 1178-1187, 2017 06.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28358541

RESUMO

The present study investigates the processes by which essentialist beliefs about religious categories develop. Children (ages 5 and 10) and adults (n = 350) from 2 religious groups (Jewish and Christian), with a range of levels of religiosity, completed switched-at-birth tasks in which they were told that a baby had been born to parents of 1 religion but raised by parents of another religion. Results indicated that younger children saw religion-based categories as possible essential kinds, regardless of the child's own religious background, but that culture-specific patterns emerged across development. This work shows that cultural context plays a powerful role in guiding the development of essentialist beliefs about religious categories. (PsycINFO Database Record


Assuntos
Cultura , Psicologia da Criança , Religião e Psicologia , Comportamento Social , Adulto , Fatores Etários , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Comparação Transcultural , Feminino , Humanos , Estudos Longitudinais , Masculino , Relações Mãe-Filho
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA