Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 19 de 19
Filtrar
1.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 113(17): 4818-23, 2016 Apr 26.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27071097

RESUMO

In many animal species, learning and memory have been found to play important roles in regulating intra- and interspecific behavioral interactions in varying environments. In such contexts, aggression is commonly used to obtain desired resources. Previous defeats or victories during aggressive interactions have been shown to influence the outcome of later contests, revealing loser and winner effects. In this study, we asked whether short- and/or long-term behavioral consequences accompany victories and defeats in dyadic pairings between male Drosophila melanogaster and how long those effects remain. The results demonstrated that single fights induced important behavioral changes in both combatants and resulted in the formation of short-term loser and winner effects. These decayed over several hours, with the duration depending on the level of familiarity of the opponents. Repeated defeats induced a long-lasting loser effect that was dependent on de novo protein synthesis, whereas repeated victories had no long-term behavioral consequences. This suggests that separate mechanisms govern the formation of loser and winner effects. These studies aim to lay a foundation for future investigations exploring the molecular mechanisms and circuitry underlying the nervous system changes induced by winning and losing bouts during agonistic encounters.


Assuntos
Comportamento Agonístico , Drosophila melanogaster/fisiologia , Adenilil Ciclases/genética , Adenilil Ciclases/fisiologia , Animais , Sinais (Psicologia) , Proteínas de Drosophila/genética , Proteínas de Drosophila/fisiologia , Drosophila melanogaster/genética , Masculino , Memória de Longo Prazo , Neuropeptídeos/genética , Neuropeptídeos/fisiologia , Fatores de Tempo
2.
Learn Mem ; 22(2): 64-8, 2015 Feb.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25593291

RESUMO

In Drosophila, prior fighting experience influences the outcome of later contests: losing a fight increases the probability of losing second contests, thereby revealing "loser" effects that involve learning and memory. In these experiments, to generate and quantify the behavioral changes observed as consequences of losing fights, we developed a new behavioral arena that eliminates handling. We compared two commonly used fly handling procedures with this new chamber and demonstrated that handling influences aggressive behavior and prevents "loser" effect formation. In addition, we induced and observed novel aspects of learning associated with aggression such as the formation of robust winner effects.


Assuntos
Agressão , Manobra Psicológica , Aprendizagem , Comportamento Social , Animais , Drosophila melanogaster , Masculino , Memória
3.
PLoS One ; 19(3): e0299839, 2024.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38452142

RESUMO

In animals, the access to vital resources often relies on individuals' behavioural personality, strength, motivation, past experiences and dominance status. Dominant individuals would be more territorial, providing them with a better access to food resources and mate. The so-called winner and loser effects induce individuals' behavioural changes after experiencing a victory or a defeat, and lead to an individual persistent state influencing the outcome of subsequent fights. However, whether and how development of winner and loser effects affect individuals' fitness is controversial. The aim of this study is to evaluate how individuals' fitness can be influenced by previous fighting experience in Drosophila melanogaster. In this study, we assess various behavioural performances as indicators for dominant and subordinate fitness. Our results show that subordinates are less territorial than dominants although their locomotor abilities are not affected. We also demonstrate that in a non-competitive context, experiencing a defeat reduces males' motivation to court females but not the reproductive success while in a competitive context, it negatively affects males' reproductive success. However, we found no impact upon either males' ability to distinguish potential mates nor on females' choice of a specific mating partner. Overall, these results indicate that previous defeats reduce reproductive success, a commonly used estimate of individual fitness.


Assuntos
Drosophila melanogaster , Reprodução , Humanos , Masculino , Animais , Feminino , Predomínio Social , Territorialidade , Motivação
4.
Cold Spring Harb Protoc ; 2023(9): 685-692, 2023 09 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37019607

RESUMO

Here, we highlight three different assays that are used to study Drosophila aggression. The advantages and disadvantages of each assay are discussed, as examining different aspects of aggressive behavior presents distinct challenges to researchers. This is because aggression is not a singular behavioral unit. Rather, aggression is the result of interactions between individuals; and, as such, the initiation and frequency of these interactions are impacted by the assay parameters including the method of loading the flies into the observation chamber, the size of the chamber, and the animals' previous social experience. Thus, determining which assay to use depends on the overall question that is the subject of investigation.


Assuntos
Agressão , Drosophila , Animais , Drosophila melanogaster , Comportamento Animal
5.
Cold Spring Harb Protoc ; 2023(9): 618-627, 2023 09 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37019610

RESUMO

Aggression is an innate behavior that likely evolved in the framework of defending or obtaining resources. This complex social behavior is influenced by genetic, environmental, and internal factors. Drosophila melanogaster remains an effective and exciting model organism with which to unravel the mechanistic basis of aggression due to its small but sophisticated brain, an impressive array of neurogenetic tools, and robust stereotypical behavioral patterns. The investigations of many laboratories have led to the identification of external and internal state factors that promote aggression, sex differences in the patterns and outcome of aggression, and neurotransmitters that regulate aggression.


Assuntos
Proteínas de Drosophila , Drosophila , Animais , Feminino , Masculino , Drosophila melanogaster/genética , Comportamento Social , Agressão , Comportamento Animal/fisiologia
6.
Eur J Neurosci ; 35(11): 1684-91, 2012 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22571719

RESUMO

The remodelling of neurons during their development is considered necessary for their normal function. One fundamental mechanism involved in this remodelling process in both vertebrates and invertebrates is axon pruning. A well-documented case of such neuronal remodelling is the developmental axon pruning of mushroom body γ neurons that occurs during metamorphosis in Drosophila. The γ neurons undergo pruning of larval-specific dendrites and axons at metamorphosis, followed by their regrowth as adult-specific dendrites and axons. We recently revealed a molecular cascade required for this pruning. The nuclear receptor ftz-f1 activates the expression of the steroid hormone receptor EcR-B1, a key component for γ remodelling, and represses expression of Hr39, an ftz-f1 homologous gene. If ectopically expressed in the γ neurons, HR39 inhibits normal pruning, probably by competing with endogenous FTZ-F1, which results in decreased EcR-B1 expression. The mushroom bodies are a bilaterally symmetric structure in the larval and adult brain and are involved in the processing of different types of olfactory memory. How memory is affected in pruning-deficient adult flies that possess larval-stage neuronal circuitry will help to explain the functional role of neuron remodelling. Flies overexpressing Hr39 are viable as adults and make it possible to assess the requirement for wild-type mushroom body pruning in memory. While blocking mushroom body neuron remodelling impaired memory after short-term courtship conditioning, long-term memory was normal. These results show that larval pruning is necessary for adult memory and that expression of courtship short-term memory and long-term memory may be parallel and independent.


Assuntos
Diferenciação Celular/fisiologia , Drosophila melanogaster/crescimento & desenvolvimento , Memória de Longo Prazo/fisiologia , Corpos Pedunculados/crescimento & desenvolvimento , Plasticidade Neuronal/fisiologia , Comportamento Sexual Animal/fisiologia , Animais , Drosophila melanogaster/citologia , Drosophila melanogaster/fisiologia , Feminino , Masculino , Corpos Pedunculados/citologia , Corpos Pedunculados/fisiologia , Neurônios/citologia , Neurônios/fisiologia
7.
N Engl J Med ; 360(22): 2289-301, 2009 May 28.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19474426

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: The myelodysplastic syndromes and myeloproliferative disorders are associated with deregulated production of myeloid cells. The mechanisms underlying these disorders are not well defined. METHODS: We conducted a combination of molecular, cytogenetic, comparative-genomic-hybridization, and single-nucleotide-polymorphism analyses to identify a candidate tumor-suppressor gene common to patients with myelodysplastic syndromes, myeloproliferative disorders, and acute myeloid leukemia (AML). The coding sequence of this gene, TET2, was determined in 320 patients. We analyzed the consequences of deletions or mutations in TET2 with the use of in vitro clonal assays and transplantation of human tumor cells into mice. RESULTS: We initially identified deletions or mutations in TET2 in three patients with myelodysplastic syndromes, in three of five patients with myeloproliferative disorders, in two patients with primary AML, and in one patient with secondary AML. We selected the six patients with myelodysplastic syndromes or AML because they carried acquired rearrangements on chromosome 4q24; we selected the five patients with myeloproliferative disorders because they carried a dominant clone in hematopoietic progenitor cells that was positive for the V617F mutation in the Janus kinase 2 (JAK2) gene. TET2 defects were observed in 15 of 81 patients with myelodysplastic syndromes (19%), in 24 of 198 patients with myeloproliferative disorders (12%) (with or without the JAK2 V617F mutation), in 5 of 21 patients with secondary AML (24%), and in 2 of 9 patients with chronic myelomonocytic leukemia (22%). TET2 defects were present in hematopoietic stem cells and preceded the JAK2 V617F mutation in the five samples from patients with myeloproliferative disorders that we analyzed. CONCLUSIONS: Somatic mutations in TET2 occur in about 15% of patients with various myeloid cancers.


Assuntos
Proteínas de Ligação a DNA/genética , Leucemia Mieloide Aguda/genética , Mutação , Síndromes Mielodisplásicas/genética , Transtornos Mieloproliferativos/genética , Proteínas Proto-Oncogênicas/genética , Sequência de Aminoácidos , Animais , Antígenos CD34 , Cromossomos Humanos Par 4/genética , Hibridização Genômica Comparativa , Dioxigenases , Rearranjo Gênico , Células-Tronco Hematopoéticas/imunologia , Humanos , Janus Quinase 2/genética , Camundongos , Camundongos Endogâmicos NOD , Camundongos SCID , Dados de Sequência Molecular , Polimorfismo de Nucleotídeo Único , Deleção de Sequência
8.
Front Behav Neurosci ; 14: 599676, 2020.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33519392

RESUMO

In competition for food, mates and territory, most animal species display aggressive behavior through visual threats and/or physical attacks. Such naturally-complex social behaviors have been shaped by evolution. Environmental pressure, such as the one imposed by dietary regimes, forces animals to adapt to specific conditions and ultimately to develop alternative behavioral strategies. The quality of the food resource during contests influence animals' aggression levels. However, little is known regarding the effects of a long-term dietary restriction-based environmental pressure on the development of alternative fighting strategies. To address this, we employed two lines of the wild-type Drosophila melanogaster Canton-S (CS) which originated from the same population but raised under two distinct diets for years. One diet contained both proteins and sugar, while the second one was sugar-free. We set up male-male aggression assays using both CS lines and found differences in aggression levels and the fighting strategies employed to establish dominance relationships. CS males raised on a sugar-containing diet started fights with a physical attack and employed a high number of lunges for establishing dominance but displayed few wing threats throughout the fight. In contrast, the sugar-free-raised males favored wing threats as an initial aggressive demonstration and used fewer lunges to establish dominance, but displayed a higher number of wing threats. This study demonstrates that fruit flies that have been raised under different dietary conditions have adapted their patterns of aggressive behavior and developed distinct fighting strategies: one favoring physical attacks, while the other one favoring visual threats.

9.
Sci Rep ; 9(1): 4084, 2019 03 11.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30858499

RESUMO

Aggression and courtship behavior were examined of wild Drosophila melanogaster flies isolated from two contrasting microclimates found at Evolution Canyon in Mt. Carmel, Israel: an African-like dry tropical Slope (AS) and a European-like humid temperate Slope (ES), separated by 250 meters. Studies were carried out to ask whether behavioral differences existed between the two populations obtained from opposite slopes with divergent microclimates in Israel. First, we measured and compared intraslope aggression between same sex fly pairings collected from the same slope. Both male and female flies displayed similar fighting abilities from both slopes. ES males, however, from the humid biome, showed a tendency to lunge more per aggressive encounter, compared with AS males from the dry biome. Next, we tested interslope aggression by pairing flies from opposite slopes. ES males displayed higher numbers of lunges, and won more fights against their AS opponents. We also observed enhanced courtship performances in ES compared to AS males. The fighting and courtship superiority seen in ES males could reinforce fitness and pre-mating reproductive isolation mechanisms that underlie incipient sympatric speciation. This may support an evolutionary advantage of adaptively divergent fruit fly aggression phenotypes from different environments.


Assuntos
Agressão/fisiologia , Drosophila melanogaster/fisiologia , Especiação Genética , Simpatria/genética , Animais , Evolução Biológica , Corte , Drosophila melanogaster/genética , Microclima , Fenótipo
10.
Fly (Austin) ; 11(2): 129-138, 2017 04 03.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27834611

RESUMO

In competition for food, territory and mates, male fruit flies (Drosophila melanogaster) engage in agonistic encounters with conspecifics. The fighting strategies used to obtain these resources are influenced by previous and present experience, environmental cues, and the internal state of the animal including hormonal and genetic influences. Animals that experience prior defeats show submissive behavior and are more likely to lose 2nd contests, while animals that win 1st fights are more aggressive and have a higher probability of winning 2nd contests. In a recent report, we examined these loser and winner effects in greater detail and demonstrated that both winners and losers show short-term memory of the results of previous bouts while only losers demonstrate a longer-term memory that requires protein synthesis. The recent findings also suggested that an individual recognition mechanism likely exists that can serve important roles in evaluating the fighting ability of opponents and influencing future fighting strategy. In this article, we follow up on these results by asking how previous defeated and victorious flies change their fighting strategies in the presence of 2nd losing and winning flies, by searching for evidence of territory marking, and discussing the existing literature in light of our findings.


Assuntos
Comportamento Animal , Drosophila melanogaster/fisiologia , Comportamento Social , Agressão , Animais , Feminino , Masculino
11.
Nat Commun ; 8: 15510, 2017 06 05.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28580949

RESUMO

Efficient energy use has constrained the evolution of nervous systems. However, it is unresolved whether energy metabolism may resultantly regulate major brain functions. Our observation that Drosophila flies double their sucrose intake at an early stage of long-term memory formation initiated the investigation of how energy metabolism intervenes in this process. Cellular-resolution imaging of energy metabolism reveals a concurrent elevation of energy consumption in neurons of the mushroom body, the fly's major memory centre. Strikingly, upregulation of mushroom body energy flux is both necessary and sufficient to drive long-term memory formation. This effect is triggered by a specific pair of dopaminergic neurons afferent to the mushroom bodies, via the D5-like DAMB dopamine receptor. Hence, dopamine signalling mediates an energy switch in the mushroom body that controls long-term memory encoding. Our data thus point to an instructional role for energy flux in the execution of demanding higher brain functions.


Assuntos
Drosophila melanogaster/metabolismo , Memória de Longo Prazo/fisiologia , Memória/fisiologia , Corpos Pedunculados/metabolismo , Animais , Animais Geneticamente Modificados , Comportamento Animal/fisiologia , Encéfalo/fisiologia , Condicionamento Clássico/fisiologia , Dopamina/metabolismo , Neurônios Dopaminérgicos/metabolismo , Proteínas de Drosophila/metabolismo , Metabolismo Energético , Feminino , Genótipo , Masculino , Neurônios/metabolismo , Receptores Dopaminérgicos/metabolismo , Olfato/fisiologia , Sacarose/química , Ativação Transcricional , Regulação para Cima
12.
J Nat Sci ; 1(3): e56, 2015.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25789340

RESUMO

Aggressive behavior in Drosophila melanogaster serves to acquire or defense vital resources such as food, territory or access to mates. Flies learn from previous fighting experience and modify and adapt their behavior to new situations, suggesting that learning and memory play a major role in agonistic encounters. Prior fighting experience influences the outcome of later contests: losing a fight increases the probability of losing second contests, revealing the formation of a "loser" effect. In a recent publication, we developed a new behavioral arena that eliminates handling of flies prior to, during and after fights to study the learning and memory associated with aggression. We compared two handling procedures commonly used in laboratories to study aggression with the new chambers and demonstrated that handling negatively influences aggression and prevents "loser" effect formation. In addition, we observed new aspects of behavior such as the formation of robust winner effects.

13.
J Vis Exp ; (106): e53395, 2015 12 30.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26780386

RESUMO

Aggressive behavior in Drosophila melanogaster is composed of the sequential expression of stereotypical behavioral patterns (for analysis see (1)). This complex behavior is influenced by genetic, hormonal and environmental factors. As in many organisms, previous fighting experience influences the fighting strategy of flies and the outcome of later contests: losing a fight increases the probability of losing later contests, revealing "loser" effects that likely involve learning and memory (2-4). The learning and memory that accompanies expression of complex social behaviors like aggression, is sensitive to pre-test handling of animals (5,6). Many experimental procedures are used in different laboratories to study aggression (7-9), however, no routinely used protocol that excludes handling of flies is currently available. Here, we report a new behavioral apparatus that eliminates handling of flies, using instead their innate negative geotactic responses to move animals into or out of fighting chambers. In this protocol, small circular fight arenas containing a food cup are divided into two equal halves by a removable plastic slider prior to introduction of flies. Flies enter chambers from their home isolation vials via sliding chamber doors and geotaxis. Upon removal of plastic sliders, flies are free to interact. After specified time periods, flies are separated again by sliders for subsequent experimentation. All of this is done easily without handling of individual flies. This apparatus offers a novel approach to study aggression and the associated learning and memory, including the formation of "loser" effects in fly fights. In addition, this new general-purpose behavioral apparatus can be employed to study other social behaviors of flies and should, in general, be of interest for investigating experience-related changes in fundamental behavioral processes.


Assuntos
Agressão/fisiologia , Comportamento Animal/fisiologia , Drosophila melanogaster/fisiologia , Entomologia/métodos , Animais , Aprendizagem por Associação , Feminino , Masculino , Memória
14.
Cell Rep ; 11(8): 1280-92, 2015 May 26.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25981036

RESUMO

Understanding how the various memory components are encoded and how they interact to guide behavior requires knowledge of the underlying neural circuits. Currently, aversive olfactory memory in Drosophila is behaviorally subdivided into four discrete phases. Among these, short- and long-term memories rely, respectively, on the γ and α/ß Kenyon cells (KCs), two distinct subsets of the ∼2,000 neurons in the mushroom body (MB). Whereas V2 efferent neurons retrieve memory from α/ß KCs, the neurons that retrieve short-term memory are unknown. We identified a specific pair of MB efferent neurons, named M6, that retrieve memory from γ KCs. Moreover, our network analysis revealed that six discrete memory phases actually exist, three of which have been conflated in the past. At each time point, two distinct memory components separately recruit either V2 or M6 output pathways. Memory retrieval thus features a dramatic convergence from KCs to MB efferent neurons.


Assuntos
Proteínas de Drosophila/metabolismo , Drosophila/metabolismo , Memória/fisiologia , Corpos Pedunculados/metabolismo , Neurônios/metabolismo , Animais , Animais Geneticamente Modificados , Feminino
15.
Cell Rep ; 5(3): 769-80, 2013 Nov 14.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24209748

RESUMO

One of the challenges facing memory research is to combine network- and cellular-level descriptions of memory encoding. In this context, Drosophila offers the opportunity to decipher, down to single-cell resolution, memory-relevant circuits in connection with the mushroom bodies (MBs), prominent structures for olfactory learning and memory. Although the MB-afferent circuits involved in appetitive learning were recently described, the circuits underlying appetitive memory retrieval remain unknown. We identified two pairs of cholinergic neurons efferent from the MB α vertical lobes, named MB-V3, that are necessary for the retrieval of appetitive long-term memory (LTM). Furthermore, LTM retrieval was correlated to an enhanced response to the rewarded odor in these neurons. Strikingly, though, silencing the MB-V3 neurons did not affect short-term memory (STM) retrieval. This finding supports a scheme of parallel appetitive STM and LTM processing.


Assuntos
Comportamento Apetitivo/fisiologia , Drosophila/fisiologia , Memória de Longo Prazo/fisiologia , Corpos Pedunculados/fisiologia , Neurônios Eferentes/fisiologia , Animais , Animais Geneticamente Modificados , Feminino
16.
Nat Neurosci ; 15(4): 592-9, 2012 Feb 26.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22366756

RESUMO

A fundamental duty of any efficient memory system is to prevent long-lasting storage of poorly relevant information. However, little is known about dedicated mechanisms that appropriately trigger production of long-term memory (LTM). We examined the role of Drosophila dopaminergic neurons in the control of LTM formation and found that they act as a switch between two exclusive consolidation pathways leading to LTM or anesthesia-resistant memory (ARM). Blockade, after aversive olfactory conditioning, of three pairs of dopaminergic neurons projecting on mushroom bodies, the olfactory memory center, enhanced ARM, whereas their overactivation conversely impaired ARM. Notably, blockade of these neurons during the intertrial intervals of a spaced training precluded LTM formation. Two pairs of these dopaminergic neurons displayed sustained calcium oscillations in naive flies. Oscillations were weakened by ARM-inducing massed training and were enhanced during LTM formation. Our results indicate that oscillations of two pairs of dopaminergic neurons control ARM levels and gate LTM.


Assuntos
Sinalização do Cálcio/fisiologia , Neurônios Dopaminérgicos/fisiologia , Memória de Longo Prazo/fisiologia , Corpos Pedunculados/fisiologia , Animais , Animais Geneticamente Modificados , Drosophila melanogaster , Feminino , Fatores de Tempo
17.
Curr Biol ; 21(19): 1647-53, 2011 Oct 11.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21962716

RESUMO

It is broadly accepted that long-term memory (LTM) is formed sequentially after learning and short-term memory (STM) formation, but the nature of the relationship between early and late memory traces remains heavily debated [1-5]. To shed light on this issue, we used an olfactory appetitive conditioning in Drosophila, wherein starved flies learned to associate an odor with the presence of sugar [6]. We took advantage of the fact that both STM and LTM are generated after a unique conditioning cycle [7, 8] to demonstrate that appetitive LTM is able to form independently of STM. More specifically, we show that (1) STM retrieval involves output from γ neurons of the mushroom body (MB), i.e., the olfactory memory center [9, 10], whereas LTM retrieval involves output from αß MB neurons; (2) STM information is not transferred from γ neurons to αß neurons for LTM formation; and (3) the adenylyl cyclase RUT, which is thought to operate as a coincidence detector between the olfactory stimulus and the sugar stimulus [11-14], is required independently in γ neurons to form appetitive STM and in αß neurons to form LTM. Taken together, these results demonstrate that appetitive short- and long-term memories are formed and processed in parallel.


Assuntos
Drosophila melanogaster/fisiologia , Animais , Condicionamento Operante , Drosophila melanogaster/genética , Memória de Longo Prazo , Memória de Curto Prazo , Corpos Pedunculados/fisiologia , Neurônios/fisiologia , Odorantes , Olfato , Sacarose/farmacologia
18.
Nat Neurosci ; 14(7): 903-10, 2011 Jun 19.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21685917

RESUMO

Aversive olfactory memory is formed in the mushroom bodies in Drosophila melanogaster. Memory retrieval requires mushroom body output, but the manner in which a memory trace in the mushroom body drives conditioned avoidance of a learned odor remains unknown. To identify neurons that are involved in olfactory memory retrieval, we performed an anatomical and functional screen of defined sets of mushroom body output neurons. We found that MB-V2 neurons were essential for retrieval of both short- and long-lasting memory, but not for memory formation or memory consolidation. MB-V2 neurons are cholinergic efferent neurons that project from the mushroom body vertical lobes to the middle superiormedial protocerebrum and the lateral horn. Notably, the odor response of MB-V2 neurons was modified after conditioning. As the lateral horn has been implicated in innate responses to repellent odorants, we propose that MB-V2 neurons recruit the olfactory pathway involved in innate odor avoidance during memory retrieval.


Assuntos
Rememoração Mental/fisiologia , Neurônios Motores/fisiologia , Corpos Pedunculados/citologia , Olfato/fisiologia , Vias Aferentes/fisiologia , Análise de Variância , Animais , Animais Geneticamente Modificados , Aprendizagem da Esquiva/fisiologia , Comportamento Animal , Antígenos CD8/metabolismo , Colina O-Acetiltransferase/metabolismo , Condicionamento Psicológico , Proteínas de Drosophila/genética , Proteínas de Drosophila/metabolismo , Drosophila melanogaster , Regulação da Expressão Gênica/fisiologia , Glutamato Descarboxilase/metabolismo , Proteínas de Fluorescência Verde/genética , Memória de Curto Prazo/fisiologia , Modelos Biológicos , Neurônios Motores/classificação , Odorantes , Condutos Olfatórios , Sinapsinas/metabolismo , Temperatura , Fatores de Tempo , Fatores de Transcrição/metabolismo , Tirosina 3-Mono-Oxigenase/metabolismo , Proteínas Vesiculares de Transporte de Glutamato/metabolismo
19.
Mech Ageing Dev ; 130(8): 547-52, 2009 Aug.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19486910

RESUMO

The steroid hormone ecdysone influences Drosophila lifespan. Longevity is extended in mutants deficient for ecdysone synthesis or mutants of the ecdysone receptor (EcR). However, the underlying mechanisms remain unclear. Here we conditionally inactivated EcR by RNA interference or expression of dominant negative forms, using the RU486 inducible system. A mild ubiquitous inactivation of EcR during adulthood was sufficient to slow the aging of male flies, whereas a stronger EcR inactivation decreased longevity. Surprisingly, ubiquitous inactivation of EcR strongly decreased female lifespan. This deleterious effect was suppressed in sterile ovo(D1) mutant females, suggesting that EcR represses a negative signal for lifespan produced in ovaries. These results reveal a complex adult and sex-specific control of lifespan by steroid signalling in Drosophila.


Assuntos
Drosophila/fisiologia , Regulação da Expressão Gênica , Longevidade , Receptores de Esteroides/fisiologia , Animais , Feminino , Genes Dominantes , Masculino , Mifepristona/farmacologia , Modelos Biológicos , Interferência de RNA , Receptores de Esteroides/metabolismo , Fatores Sexuais , Transdução de Sinais , Esteroides/metabolismo
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA