Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 62
Filtrar
1.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 121(16): e2401196121, 2024 Apr 16.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38588422

RESUMO

Face pareidolia is a tendency to seeing faces in nonface images that reflects high tuning to a face scheme. Yet, studies of the brain networks underwriting face pareidolia are scarce. Here, we examined the time course and dynamic topography of gamma oscillatory neuromagnetic activity while administering a task with nonface images resembling a face. Images were presented either with canonical orientation or with display inversion that heavily impedes face pareidolia. At early processing stages, the peaks in gamma activity (40 to 45 Hz) to images either triggering or not face pareidolia originate mainly from the right medioventral and lateral occipital cortices, rostral and caudal cuneus gyri, and medial superior occipital gyrus. Yet, the difference occurred at later processing stages in the high-frequency range of 80 to 85 Hz over a set of the areas constituting the social brain. The findings speak rather for a relatively late neural network playing a key role in face pareidolia. Strikingly, a cutting-edge analysis of brain connectivity unfolding over time reveals mutual feedforward and feedback intra- and interhemispheric communication not only within the social brain but also within the extended large-scale network of down- and upstream regions. In particular, the superior temporal sulcus and insula strongly engage in communication with other brain regions either as signal transmitters or recipients throughout the whole processing of face-pareidolia images.


Assuntos
Mapeamento Encefálico , Face , Encéfalo , Lobo Occipital , Lobo Temporal
2.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 119(5)2022 02 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35074880

RESUMO

Despite our fluency in reading human faces, sometimes we mistakenly perceive illusory faces in objects, a phenomenon known as face pareidolia. Although illusory faces share some neural mechanisms with real faces, it is unknown to what degree pareidolia engages higher-level social perception beyond the detection of a face. In a series of large-scale behavioral experiments (ntotal = 3,815 adults), we found that illusory faces in inanimate objects are readily perceived to have a specific emotional expression, age, and gender. Most strikingly, we observed a strong bias to perceive illusory faces as male rather than female. This male bias could not be explained by preexisting semantic or visual gender associations with the objects, or by visual features in the images. Rather, this robust bias in the perception of gender for illusory faces reveals a cognitive bias arising from a broadly tuned face evaluation system in which minimally viable face percepts are more likely to be perceived as male.


Assuntos
Face/fisiologia , Ilusões/fisiologia , Adulto , Reconhecimento Facial/fisiologia , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Estimulação Luminosa/métodos
3.
J Neural Transm (Vienna) ; 131(2): 141-148, 2024 02.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38110521

RESUMO

Visuoperceptual dysfunction is common in Parkinson's disease (PD) and is also reported in its prodromal phase, isolated REM sleep behavior disorder (iRBD). We aimed to investigate color discrimination ability and complex visual illusions known as pareidolias in patients with iRBD and PD compared to healthy controls, and their associating clinical factors. 46 iRBD, 43 PD, and 64 healthy controls performed the Farnsworth-Munsell 100 hue test and noise pareidolia tests. Any relationship between those two visual functions and associations with prodromal motor and non-motor manifestations were evaluated, including MDS-UPDRS part I to III, Cross-Cultural Smell Identification Test, sleep questionnaires, and comprehensive neuropsychological assessment. iRBD and PD patients both performed worse on the Farnsworth-Munsell 100 hue test and had greater number of pareidolias compared to healthy controls. No correlations were found between the extent of impaired color discrimination and pareidolia scores in either group. In iRBD patients, pareidolias were associated with frontal executive dysfunction, while impaired color discrimination was associated with visuospatial dysfunction, hyposmia, and higher MDS-UPDRS-III scores. Pareidolias in PD patients correlated with worse global cognition, whereas color discrimination deficits were associated with frontal executive dysfunction. Color discrimination deficits and pareidolias are frequent but does not correlate with each other from prodromal to clinically established stage of PD. The different pattern of clinical associates with the two visual symptoms suggests that evaluation of both color and pareidolias may aid in revealing the course of neurodegeneration in iRBD and PD patients.


Assuntos
Disfunção Cognitiva , Doença de Parkinson , Transtorno do Comportamento do Sono REM , Humanos , Transtorno do Comportamento do Sono REM/complicações , Transtorno do Comportamento do Sono REM/diagnóstico , Disfunção Cognitiva/complicações , Cognição , Testes Neuropsicológicos
4.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 118(21)2021 05 25.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34001601

RESUMO

Understanding how the young infant brain starts to categorize the flurry of ambiguous sensory inputs coming in from its complex environment is of primary scientific interest. Here, we test the hypothesis that senses other than vision play a key role in initiating complex visual categorizations in 20 4-mo-old infants exposed either to a baseline odor or to their mother's odor while their electroencephalogram (EEG) is recorded. Various natural images of objects are presented at a 6-Hz rate (six images/second), with face-like object configurations of the same object categories (i.e., eliciting face pareidolia in adults) interleaved every sixth stimulus (i.e., 1 Hz). In the baseline odor context, a weak neural categorization response to face-like stimuli appears at 1 Hz in the EEG frequency spectrum over bilateral occipitotemporal regions. Critically, this face-like-selective response is magnified and becomes right lateralized in the presence of maternal body odor. This reveals that nonvisual cues systematically associated with human faces in the infant's experience shape the interpretation of face-like configurations as faces in the right hemisphere, dominant for face categorization. At the individual level, this intersensory influence is particularly effective when there is no trace of face-like categorization in the baseline odor context. These observations provide evidence for the early tuning of face-(like)-selective activity from multisensory inputs in the developing brain, suggesting that perceptual development integrates information across the senses for efficient category acquisition, with early maturing systems such as olfaction driving the acquisition of categories in later-developing systems such as vision.


Assuntos
Encéfalo/fisiologia , Reconhecimento Facial/fisiologia , Odorantes , Visão Ocular/fisiologia , Encéfalo/diagnóstico por imagem , Mapeamento Encefálico , Eletroencefalografia , Feminino , Humanos , Lactente , Masculino , Estimulação Luminosa
5.
Anim Cogn ; 26(3): 885-905, 2023 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36583802

RESUMO

We sometimes perceive meaningful patterns or images in random arrangements of colors and shapes. This phenomenon is called pareidolia and has recently been studied intensively, especially face pareidolia. In contrast, there are few comparative-cognitive studies on face pareidolia with nonhuman primates. This study explored behavioral evidence for face pareidolia in chimpanzees using visual search and matching tasks. Faces are processed in a configural manner, and their perception and recognition are hampered by inversion and misalignment of top and bottom parts. We investigated whether the same effect occurs in a visual search for face-like objects. The results showed an effect of misalignment. On the other hand, consistent results were not obtained with the photographs of fruits. When only the top or bottom half of the face-like object was presented, chimpanzees showed better performance for the top-half condition, suggesting the importance of the eye area in face pareidolia. In the positive-control experiments, chimpanzees received the same experiment using human faces and human participants with face-like objects and fruits. As a result, chimpanzees showed an inefficient search for inverted and misaligned faces and humans for manipulated face-like objects. Finally, to examine the role of face awareness, we tested matching a human face to a face-like object in chimpanzees but obtained no substantial evidence that they saw the face-like object as a "face." Based on these results, we discussed the extents and limits of face pareidolia in chimpanzees.


Assuntos
Face , Pan troglodytes , Humanos , Animais , Pan troglodytes/psicologia , Reconhecimento Psicológico , Estimulação Luminosa
6.
Biol Lett ; 19(9): 20230126, 2023 09.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37700700

RESUMO

As primates, we are hypersensitive to faces and face-like patterns in the visual environment, hence we often perceive illusory faces in otherwise inanimate objects, such as burnt pieces of toast and the surface of the moon. Although this phenomenon, known as face pareidolia, is a common experience, it is unknown whether our susceptibility to face pareidolia is static across our lifespan or what factors would cause it to change. Given the evidence that behaviour towards face stimuli is modulated by the neuropeptide oxytocin (OT), we reasoned that participants in stages of life associated with high levels of endogenous OT might be more susceptible to face pareidolia than participants in other stages of life. We tested this hypothesis by assessing pareidolia susceptibility in two groups of women; pregnant women (low endogenous OT) and postpartum women (high endogenous OT). We found evidence that postpartum women report seeing face pareidolia more easily than women who are currently pregnant. These data, collected online, suggest that our sensitivity to face-like patterns is not fixed and may change throughout adulthood, providing a crucial proof of concept that requires further research.


Assuntos
Longevidade , Ocitocina , Feminino , Humanos , Gravidez , Animais , Período Pós-Parto
7.
Cereb Cortex ; 31(5): 2574-2585, 2021 03 31.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33350440

RESUMO

The latest COVID-19 pandemic reveals that unexpected changes elevate depression bringing people apart, but also calling for social sharing. Yet the impact of depression on social cognition and functioning is not well understood. Assessment of social cognition is crucial not only for a better understanding of major depressive disorder (MDD), but also for screening, intervention, and remediation. Here by applying a novel experimental tool, a Face-n-Food task comprising a set of images bordering on the Giuseppe Arcimboldo style, we assessed the face tuning in patients with MDD and person-by-person matched controls. The key benefit of these images is that single components do not trigger face processing. Contrary to common beliefs, the outcome indicates that individuals with depression express intact face responsiveness. Yet, while in depression face sensitivity is tied with perceptual organization, in typical development, it is knotted with social cognition capabilities. Face tuning in depression, therefore, may rely upon altered behavioral strategies and underwriting brain mechanisms. To exclude a possible camouflaging effect of female social skills, we examined gender impact. Neither in depression nor in typical individuals had females excelled in face tuning. The outcome sheds light on the origins of the face sensitivity and alterations in social functioning in depression and mental well-being at large. Aberrant social functioning in depression is likely to be the result of deeply-rooted maladaptive strategies rather than of poor sensitivity to social signals. This has implications for mental well-being under the current pandemic conditions.


Assuntos
COVID-19/psicologia , Transtorno Depressivo Maior/psicologia , Reconhecimento Facial , Pinturas/psicologia , Estimulação Luminosa/métodos , Cognição Social , Adulto , COVID-19/epidemiologia , Transtorno Depressivo Maior/epidemiologia , Expressão Facial , Reconhecimento Facial/fisiologia , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Adulto Jovem
8.
J Prosthodont ; 31(7): 562-570, 2022 Aug.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34894033

RESUMO

A human face contains a wealth of information about an individual, with which an observer can instinctively make a judgment on the attractiveness of the face. However, despite the profuse literature on facial and smile attractiveness, their origins, determinants, and perceptions remain controversial. The axiom in face processing research is that a face is perceived as an amalgamation of its features, and is referred to as "whole" or "holistic" perception. It is pertinent to the clinician involved in the provision of esthetic restorations to understand this holistic process of face recognition and perception of smile attractiveness. This review paper addresses face recognition and perception of attractiveness by reviewing the holistic perception of faces, including the multidimensional face-space model, and also reviews the smile and facial attractiveness according to the average, multiple motive, and secondary sex characteristics theories.


Assuntos
Reconhecimento Facial , Prosopagnosia , Psicologia Cognitiva , Estética Dentária , Humanos , Percepção , Prostodontia , Sorriso
9.
Proc Biol Sci ; 288(1954): 20210966, 2021 07 14.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34229489

RESUMO

Facial expressions are vital for social communication, yet the underlying mechanisms are still being discovered. Illusory faces perceived in objects (face pareidolia) are errors of face detection that share some neural mechanisms with human face processing. However, it is unknown whether expression in illusory faces engages the same mechanisms as human faces. Here, using a serial dependence paradigm, we investigated whether illusory and human faces share a common expression mechanism. First, we found that images of face pareidolia are reliably rated for expression, within and between observers, despite varying greatly in visual features. Second, they exhibit positive serial dependence for perceived facial expression, meaning an illusory face (happy or angry) is perceived as more similar in expression to the preceding one, just as seen for human faces. This suggests illusory and human faces engage similar mechanisms of temporal continuity. Third, we found robust cross-domain serial dependence of perceived expression between illusory and human faces when they were interleaved, with serial effects larger when illusory faces preceded human faces than the reverse. Together, the results support a shared mechanism for facial expression between human faces and illusory faces and suggest that expression processing is not tightly bound to human facial features.


Assuntos
Reconhecimento Facial , Ilusões , Expressão Facial , Felicidade , Humanos
10.
Int J Geriatr Psychiatry ; 36(9): 1407-1414, 2021 09.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33772864

RESUMO

OBJECTIVES: Previous research has identified that dementia with Lewy bodies (DLB) has abnormal pareidolic responses which are associated with severity of visual hallucinations (VH), and the pareidolia test accurately classifies DLB with VH. We aimed to assess whether these findings would also be evident at the earlier stage of mild cognitive impairment (MCI) with Lewy bodies (MCI-LB) in comparison to MCI due to AD (MCI-AD) and cognitively healthy comparators. METHODS: One-hundred and thirty-seven subjects were assessed prospectively in a longitudinal study with a mean follow-up of 1.2 years (max = 3.7): 63 MCI-LB (22% with VH) and 40 MCI-AD according to current research diagnostic criteria, and 34 healthy comparators. The pareidolia test was administered annually as a repeated measure. RESULTS: Probable MCI-LB had an estimated pareidolia rate 1.2-6.7 times higher than MCI-AD. Pareidolia rates were not associated with concurrent VH, but had a weak association with total score on the North East Visual Hallucinations Inventory. The pareidolia test was not an accurate classifier of either MCI-LB (Area under curve (AUC) = 0.61), or VH (AUC = 0.56). There was poor sensitivity when differentiating MCI-LB from controls (41%) or MCI-AD (27%), though specificity was better (91% and 89%, respectively). CONCLUSIONS: Whilst pareidolic responses are specifically more frequent in MCI-LB than MCI-AD, sensitivity of the pareidolia test is poorer than in DLB, with fewer patients manifesting VH at the earlier MCI stage. However, the high specificity and ease of use may make it useful in specialist clinics where imaging biomarkers are not available.


Assuntos
Doença de Alzheimer , Disfunção Cognitiva , Doença por Corpos de Lewy , Doença de Alzheimer/diagnóstico , Disfunção Cognitiva/diagnóstico , Humanos , Corpos de Lewy , Doença por Corpos de Lewy/diagnóstico , Estudos Longitudinais
11.
Neurol Sci ; 42(12): 5327-5334, 2021 Dec.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33884529

RESUMO

BACKGROUND AND AIM: Some patients with Parkinson's disease (PD) present with pareidolia, an illusion of a meaningless stimulus as a familiar object known to the observer. Since the striatum is associated with processing of visual information, we investigated correlations of pareidolia with motor symptoms and striatal dopaminergic function. METHOD: A noise pareidolia test, assessment of motor symptoms using MDS-UPDRS and 123I-Ioflupane SPECT were performed in 58 drug-naïve PD patients. A number of images in which a participant noticed an illusory face (number of illusory responses) were compared with motor assessment scores and uptake of 123I-ioflupane in the striatum. RESULTS: Of the 58 participants, 22 had at least one illusory response. Mean scores for MDS-UPDRS part III (p<0.05), rigidity (p<0.05), and rigidity on the left side of the body (p<0.01) in patients with pareidolia were significantly higher than those in patients without pareidolia. Uptake of 123I-ioflupane in the right caudate nucleus (p<0.05), anterior putamen (p<0.01), and posterior putamen (p<0.01) in patients with pareidolia was significantly lower than in patients without pareidolia. In the 22 patients with pareidolia, the number of illusory responses was significantly correlated with total scores for MDS-UPDRS part III (r=0.443, p<0.05) and subscores for bradykinesia (r=0.440, p<0.05) and bradykinesia on the left side of the body (r=0.564, p<0.01). The prevalence of pareidolia in left-dominant parkinsonism (16/30 patients) was higher than that in right-dominant parkinsonism (6/28 patients) (p<0.05 by chi-square test). CONCLUSION: Pareidolia in PD patients is associated with dysfunction in the right striatum.


Assuntos
Doença de Parkinson , Preparações Farmacêuticas , Corpo Estriado/diagnóstico por imagem , Humanos , Hipocinesia , Doença de Parkinson/diagnóstico por imagem , Tomografia Computadorizada de Emissão de Fóton Único
12.
Psychopathology ; 54(2): 59-69, 2021.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33657568

RESUMO

OBJECTIVE: Pareidolias are ilusionary misjudgments and are seen as the result of deliberately or unconsciously caused misinterpretations by the human brain, which tends to complete diffuse and apparently incomplete perceptual images. The psychopathological value of pareidolia in the context of neuropsychiatric diseases has, however, been little researched so far. METHODS: In this pilot study, a total of 25 patients (mean age 43.3 years, SD 16.2) with an affective disorder or schizophrenic disease (ICD-10: F3.X or F2.X) and 25 healthy volunteers (mean age 46.1 years, SD 15.4) were compared for sociodemographic factors and psychometric findings, as well as pareidolias and creativity. RESULTS: We found that the patients identified significantly fewer pareidolias than healthy controls (p = 0.002) and that patients with schizophrenia, in particular, had a significantly lower hit rate (p = 0.005). Across the whole group, there were clear positive correlations between pareidolia and high creativity, as well as personality traits such as impulsiveness/spontaneity, extraversion, and conscientiousness. CONCLUSIONS: Unexpectedly, having less nosology-specific features than individual specific properties such as creativity and extraversion, and especially openness and verbal intelligence, in patients with affective disorder or schizophrenia promotes the recognition of pareidolia as a specific form of illusionary misperception.


Assuntos
Encéfalo/patologia , Criatividade , Ilusões/psicologia , Adulto , Estudos de Casos e Controles , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Projetos Piloto
13.
J Neurophysiol ; 123(3): 876-884, 2020 03 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31940235

RESUMO

Migraine is a multifactorial brain disorder characterized by recurrent disabling headache attacks. One of the possible mechanisms in the pathogenesis of migraine may be a decrease in inhibitory cortical stimuli in the primary visual cortex attributable to cortical hyperexcitability. The aim of this study was to investigate the neural correlates underlying face and face pareidolia processing in terms of the event-related potential (ERP) components, N170, vertex positive potential (VPP), and N250, in patients with migraine. In total, 40 patients with migraine without aura, 23 patients with migraine and aura, and 30 healthy controls were enrolled. We recorded ERPs during the presentation of face and face pareidolia images. N170, VPP, and N250 mean amplitudes and latencies were examined. N170 was significantly greater in patients with migraine with aura than in healthy controls. VPP amplitude was significantly greater in patients with migraine without aura than in healthy controls. The face stimuli evoked significantly earlier VPP responses to faces (168.7 ms, SE = 1.46) than pareidolias (173.4 ms, SE = 1.41) in patients with migraine with aura. We did not find a significant difference between N250 amplitude for face and face pareidolia processing. A significant difference was observed between the groups for pareidolia in terms of N170 [F(2,86) = 14,75, P < 0.001] and VPP [F(2,86) = 16.43, P < 0.001] amplitudes. Early ERPs are a valuable tool to study the neural processing of face processing in patients with migraine to demonstrate visual cortical hyperexcitability.NEW & NOTEWORTHY Event-related potentials (ERPs) are important for understanding face and face pareidolia processing in patients with migraine. N170, vertex positive potential (VPP), and N250 ERPs were investigated. N170 was revealed as a potential component of cortical excitability for face and face pareidolia processing in patients with migraine.


Assuntos
Potenciais Evocados/fisiologia , Transtornos de Enxaqueca/fisiopatologia , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos/fisiologia , Adulto , Eletroencefalografia , Reconhecimento Facial/fisiologia , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Adulto Jovem
14.
Psychol Sci ; 31(8): 1001-1012, 2020 08.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32697673

RESUMO

Face pareidolia is the phenomenon of seeing facelike structures in everyday objects. Here, we tested the hypothesis that face pareidolia, rather than being limited to a cognitive or mnemonic association, reflects the activation of visual mechanisms that typically process human faces. We focused on sensory cues to social attention, which engage cell populations in temporal cortex that are susceptible to habituation effects. Repeated exposure to "pareidolia faces" that appear to have a specific direction of attention causes a systematic bias in the perception of where human faces are looking, indicating that overlapping sensory mechanisms are recruited when we view human faces and when we experience face pareidolia. These cross-adaptation effects are significantly reduced when pareidolia is abolished by removing facelike features from the objects. These results indicate that face pareidolia is essentially a perceptual phenomenon, occurring when sensory input is processed by visual mechanisms that have evolved to extract specific social content from human faces.


Assuntos
Atenção , Face , Ilusões/psicologia , Percepção Visual/fisiologia , Adolescente , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Estimulação Luminosa , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Adulto Jovem
15.
Neurol Sci ; 41(6): 1557-1565, 2020 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31980969

RESUMO

BACKGROUND AND PURPOSE: Face pareidolia is described as an interpretation of any unrelated object seen for the first time as a face. It is still unclear how to face pareidolia is processed. In this study, the neural basis of face and face pareidolia processing was investigated through recording event-related potentials (ERPs). METHODS: The ERPs were recorded from 35 right-handed and healthy participants in response to faces and face pareidolia. Amplitudes and latencies of N170, vertex-positive potential (VPP), and N250 components were analyzed, and current source density (CSD) maps relevant to these components were obtained. RESULTS: N170 response was earlier and larger in response to faces compared to face pareidolias. VPP is also evoked earlier in response to faces as in the case of N170; however, the VPP amplitude was larger for face pareidolias than for faces. Statistical analyses did not reveal any differences between faces and face pareidolias in terms of N250 component. CONCLUSION: The results indicated that faces and face pareidolias are processed in the early stages of visual perception. In addition, the N250 component does not reflect the neural processing of faces and face pareidolias.


Assuntos
Córtex Cerebral/fisiologia , Potenciais Evocados/fisiologia , Face , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos/fisiologia , Transtornos da Percepção/fisiopatologia , Adulto , Eletroencefalografia , Reconhecimento Facial/fisiologia , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Fatores de Tempo , Adulto Jovem
16.
Cogn Neuropsychiatry ; 25(2): 113-125, 2020 03.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31810425

RESUMO

Introduction: It has been proposed that hallucinations occur because of problems with reality discrimination (when internal, self-generated cognitions are misattributed to an external, non-self source) and because of elevated levels of top-down processing. In this study, we examined whether visual reality discrimination abilities and elevated top-down processing (assessed via face pareidolia-proneness) were associated with how often non-clinical participants report visual hallucination-like experiences.Methods: Participants (N = 82, mean age = 23.12 years) completed a visual reality discrimination task and a face pareidolia task, as well as self-report measures of schizotypy and of the frequency of visual hallucination-like experiences.Results: Regression analysis demonstrated that the number of false alarms made on the visual reality discrimination task and the number of hits made on the face pareidolia task were independent predictors of the frequency of visual hallucination-like experiences. Correlations between performance on the tasks and levels of schizotypy were not statistically significant.Conclusions: These findings suggest that weaker visual reality discrimination abilities and elevated levels of top-down processing are associated with visual hallucination-proneness and are discussed in terms of the idea that clinical visual hallucinations and non-clinical visual hallucination-like experiences share similar cognitive mechanisms.


Assuntos
Discriminação Psicológica/fisiologia , Alucinações/diagnóstico , Alucinações/psicologia , Estimulação Luminosa/métodos , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Percepção Visual/fisiologia , Adulto , Cognição/fisiologia , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Autorrelato , Adulto Jovem
17.
Pak J Med Sci ; 34(6): 1560-1566, 2018.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30559823

RESUMO

OBJECTIVES: Pareidolia is the interpretation of previously unseen and unrelated objects as familiar due to previous learning. The present study aimed to determine the specific brain areas that exhibited activation during real-face and face-pareidolia processing. METHODS: Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI) scans were performed on 20 healthy subjects under real-face and face-pareidolia conditions in National Magnetic Resonance Research Center (UMRAM), Ankara, Turkey from April 2016 to January 2017. FSL software was used to conduct a FEAT higher level (group) analysis to identify the brain areas activated during real-face and face-pareidolia processing. RESULTS: Under both the real-face and face-pareidolia conditions, activation was observed in the Prefrontal Cortex (PFCX), occipital cortex V1, occipital cortex V2, and inferior temporal regions. Also under both conditions, the same degree of activation was observed in the right Fusiform Face Area (FFA) and the right PFCX. On the other hand, PFCX activation was not evident under the real-face versus face scrambled or face-pareidolia versus pareidolia scrambled conditions. CONCLUSIONS: The present findings suggest that, as in real-face perception, face-pareidolia requires interaction between top-down and bottom-up brain regions including the FFA and frontal and occipitotemporal areas. Additionally, whole-brain analyses revealed that the right PFCX played an important role in processing real faces and in face pareidolia (illusory face perception), as did the FFA.

18.
Clin Anat ; 28(3): 339-54, 2015 Apr.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25511503

RESUMO

This review deals critically with many aspects of the functional genital anatomy of the human female in relation to inducing sexual arousal and its relevance to procreation and recreation. Various controversial problems are discussed including: the roles of clitorally versus coitally induced arousal and orgasm in relation to the health of women, the various sites of induction of orgasm and the difficulty women find in specifically identifying them because of "'ambiguity problems" and "genital site pareidolia," the cervix and sexual arousal, why there are so many sites for arousal, why multiple orgasms occur, genital reflexes and coitus, the sites of arousal and their representation in the brain, and identifying aspects and functions of the genitalia with appropriate new nomenclature.


Assuntos
Genitália Feminina/fisiologia , Recreação/fisiologia , Reprodução/fisiologia , Comportamento Sexual/fisiologia , Nível de Alerta/fisiologia , Feminino , Humanos , Orgasmo/fisiologia , Terminologia como Assunto
19.
J Clin Exp Neuropsychol ; : 1-11, 2024 Jul 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38949538

RESUMO

OBJECTIVE: Prior research on the Noise Pareidolia Test (NPT) has demonstrated its clinical utility in detecting patients with mild cognitive impairment and dementia due to Lewy Body Disease (LBD). However, few studies to date have investigated the neuropsychological factors underlying pareidolia errors on the NPT across the clinical spectrum of LBD. Furthermore, to our knowledge, no research has examined the relationship between cortical thickness using MRI data and NPT subscores. As such, this study sought to explore the neuropsychological and neuroanatomical factors influencing performance on the NPT utilizing the National Alzheimer's Coordinating Center Lewy Body Dementia Module. METHODS: Our sample included participants with normal cognition (NC; n = 56), LBD with mild cognitive impairment (LBD-MCI; n = 97), and LBD with dementia (LBD-Dementia; n = 94). Archival data from NACC were retrospectively analyzed for group differences in neuropsychological test scores and cognitive and psychiatric predictors of NPT scores. Clinicoradiological correlates between NPT subscores and a small subsample of the above LBD participants were also examined. RESULTS: Analyses revealed significant differences in NPT scores among groups. Regression analysis demonstrated that dementia severity, attention, and visuospatial processing contributed approximately 24% of NPT performance in LBD groups. Clinicoradiological analysis suggests a potential contribution of the right fusiform gyrus, but not the inferior occipital gyrus, to NPT pareidolia error scores. CONCLUSIONS: Our findings highlight the interplay of attention and visuoperceptual functions in complex pareidolia in LBD. Further investigation is needed to refine the utility of NPT scores in clinical settings, including identifying patients at risk for visual illusions and hallucinations.

20.
Cureus ; 16(3): e55436, 2024 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38567204

RESUMO

INTRODUCTION: Pareidolias, or visual misperceptions, are a non-motor symptom of Parkinson's disease (PD) with unclear pathophysiology. The noise pareidolia test (NPT) is a tool for screening pareidolias. The usefulness of the NPT in differentiating PD from atypical parkinsonian syndromes (APS) is also unknown. METHODS: We retrospectively investigated 74 patients with PD and 18 patients with APS who took the NPT. Correlations between the number of pareidolic responses, gray matter volume, and cerebral blood flow were also examined in the patients with PD. RESULTS: The median number of pareidolic responses in patients with PD and patients with APS was 0 (interquartile range (IQR): 0-3) and 0 (IQR: 0-1), respectively, and tended to be higher in patients with PD than in those with APS (p = 0.077). It was significantly higher in patients with PD who had hallucinations (2; IQR: 0-9) (p = 0.016). The area under the receiver operating characteristic curve for the number of pareidolic responses in the NPT was 0.62 when used to differentiate PD and APS, and the optimal cutoff number of pareidolic responses was 2/3. Sensitivity and specificity were 25.7% and 100%, respectively. In the PD group, the number of pareidolic responses was correlated with age (r = 0.27; p = 0.021) and the Frontal Assessment Battery (FAB) score (r = -0.34; p = 0.0099). Magnetic resonance imaging showed no significant correlation between the number of pareidolic responses and the volume of focal gray matter. On cerebral hypoperfusion mapping, the left parietal lobe had a significant correlation with the number of pareidolic responses (r = 0.35; p = 0.027). CONCLUSION: The number of pareidolic responses in NPT was suggested to be useful as a red flag to rule out APS in differentiating PD from APS. In PD without dementia, the number of pareidolic responses was associated with reduced blood flow in the left parietal lobe.

SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA