Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 218
Filtrar
1.
Nature ; 632(8023): 131-138, 2024 Aug.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39020167

RESUMO

A single dose of psilocybin, a psychedelic that acutely causes distortions of space-time perception and ego dissolution, produces rapid and persistent therapeutic effects in human clinical trials1-4. In animal models, psilocybin induces neuroplasticity in cortex and hippocampus5-8. It remains unclear how human brain network changes relate to subjective and lasting effects of psychedelics. Here we tracked individual-specific brain changes with longitudinal precision functional mapping (roughly 18 magnetic resonance imaging visits per participant). Healthy adults were tracked before, during and for 3 weeks after high-dose psilocybin (25 mg) and methylphenidate (40 mg), and brought back for an additional psilocybin dose 6-12 months later. Psilocybin massively disrupted functional connectivity (FC) in cortex and subcortex, acutely causing more than threefold greater change than methylphenidate. These FC changes were driven by brain desynchronization across spatial scales (areal, global), which dissolved network distinctions by reducing correlations within and anticorrelations between networks. Psilocybin-driven FC changes were strongest in the default mode network, which is connected to the anterior hippocampus and is thought to create our sense of space, time and self. Individual differences in FC changes were strongly linked to the subjective psychedelic experience. Performing a perceptual task reduced psilocybin-driven FC changes. Psilocybin caused persistent decrease in FC between the anterior hippocampus and default mode network, lasting for weeks. Persistent reduction of hippocampal-default mode network connectivity may represent a neuroanatomical and mechanistic correlate of the proplasticity and therapeutic effects of psychedelics.


Assuntos
Encéfalo , Alucinógenos , Rede Nervosa , Psilocibina , Adolescente , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Adulto Jovem , Encéfalo/citologia , Encéfalo/diagnóstico por imagem , Encéfalo/efeitos dos fármacos , Encéfalo/fisiologia , Mapeamento Encefálico , Rede de Modo Padrão/citologia , Rede de Modo Padrão/diagnóstico por imagem , Rede de Modo Padrão/efeitos dos fármacos , Rede de Modo Padrão/fisiologia , Alucinógenos/farmacologia , Alucinógenos/administração & dosagem , Voluntários Saudáveis , Hipocampo/citologia , Hipocampo/diagnóstico por imagem , Hipocampo/efeitos dos fármacos , Hipocampo/fisiologia , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética , Metilfenidato/farmacologia , Metilfenidato/administração & dosagem , Rede Nervosa/citologia , Rede Nervosa/diagnóstico por imagem , Rede Nervosa/efeitos dos fármacos , Rede Nervosa/fisiologia , Psilocibina/farmacologia , Psilocibina/administração & dosagem , Percepção Espacial/efeitos dos fármacos , Percepção do Tempo/efeitos dos fármacos , Ego
2.
J Neurosci ; 44(34)2024 Aug 21.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39048314

RESUMO

Recent studies suggest that time estimation relies on bodily rhythms and interoceptive signals. We provide the first direct electrophysiological evidence suggesting an association between the brain's processing of heartbeat and duration judgment. We examined heartbeat-evoked potential (HEP) and contingent negative variation (CNV) during an auditory duration-reproduction task and a control reaction-time task spanning 4, 8, and 12 s intervals, in both male and female participants. Interoceptive awareness was assessed with the Self-Awareness Questionnaire (SAQ) and interoceptive accuracy through the heartbeat-counting task (HCT). Results revealed that SAQ scores, but not the HCT, correlated with mean reproduced durations with higher SAQ scores associating with longer and more accurate duration reproductions. Notably, the HEP amplitude changes during the encoding phase of the timing task, particularly within 130-270 ms (HEP1) and 470-520 ms (HEP2) after the R-peak, demonstrated interval-specific modulations that did not emerge in the control task. A significant ramp-like increase in HEP2 amplitudes occurred during the duration-encoding phase of the timing but not during the control task. This increase within the reproduction phase of the timing task correlated significantly with the reproduced durations for the 8 s and the 4 s intervals. The larger the increase in HEP2, the greater the under-reproduction of the estimated duration. CNV components during the encoding phase of the timing task were more negative than those in the reaction-time task, suggesting greater executive resources orientation toward time. We conclude that interoceptive awareness (SAQ) and cortical responses to heartbeats (HEP) predict duration reproductions, emphasizing the embodied nature of time.


Assuntos
Encéfalo , Eletroencefalografia , Frequência Cardíaca , Interocepção , Percepção do Tempo , Humanos , Masculino , Feminino , Percepção do Tempo/fisiologia , Frequência Cardíaca/fisiologia , Adulto Jovem , Adulto , Interocepção/fisiologia , Encéfalo/fisiologia , Conscientização/fisiologia , Potenciais Evocados/fisiologia , Tempo de Reação/fisiologia , Variação Contingente Negativa/fisiologia
3.
J Cogn Neurosci ; 36(4): 567-571, 2024 04 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38261401

RESUMO

For decades, the intriguing connection between the human alpha rhythm (an 8- to 13-Hz oscillation maximal over posterior cortex) and temporal processes in perception has furnished a rich landscape of proposals. The past decade, however, has seen a surge in interest in the topic, bringing new theoretical, analytic, and methodological developments alongside fresh controversies. This Special Focus on alpha-band dynamics and temporal processing provides an up-to-date snapshot of the playing field, with contributions from leading researchers in the field spanning original perspectives, new evidence, comprehensive reviews and meta-analyses, as well as discussion of ongoing controversies and paths forward. We hope that the perspectives captured here will help catalyze future research and shape the pathways toward a theoretically grounded and mechanistic account of the link between alpha dynamics and temporal properties of perception.


Assuntos
Eletroencefalografia , Percepção do Tempo , Humanos , Ritmo alfa , Córtex Cerebral , Estimulação Luminosa
4.
J Cogn Neurosci ; 36(7): 1265-1281, 2024 Jun 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38652104

RESUMO

Human faces and bodies represent various socially important signals. Although adults encounter numerous new people in daily life, they can recognize hundreds to thousands of different individuals. However, the neural mechanisms that differentiate one person from another person are unclear. This study aimed to clarify the temporal dynamics of the cognitive processes of face and body personal identification using face-sensitive ERP components (P1, N170, and N250). The present study performed three blocks (face-face, face-body, and body-body) of different ERP adaptation paradigms. Furthermore, in the above three blocks, ERP components were used to compare brain biomarkers under three conditions (same person, different person of the same sex, and different person of the opposite sex). The results showed that the P1 amplitude for the face-face block was significantly greater than that for the body-body block, that the N170 amplitude for a different person of the same sex condition was greater than that for the same person condition in the right hemisphere only, and that the N250 amplitude gradually increased as the degree of face and body sex-social categorization grew closer (i.e., same person condition > different person of the same sex condition > different person of the opposite sex condition). These results suggest that early processing of the face and body processes the face and body separately and that structural encoding and personal identification of the face and body process the face and body collaboratively.


Assuntos
Eletroencefalografia , Potenciais Evocados , Reconhecimento Facial , Humanos , Feminino , Masculino , Adulto Jovem , Adulto , Reconhecimento Facial/fisiologia , Potenciais Evocados/fisiologia , Percepção do Tempo/fisiologia , Percepção Social , Encéfalo/fisiologia
5.
Neuroimage ; 297: 120706, 2024 Aug 15.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38936649

RESUMO

Time and space form an integral part of every human experience, and for the neuronal representation of these perceptual dimensions, previous studies point to the involvement of the right-hemispheric intraparietal sulcus and structures in the medial temporal lobe. Here we used multi-voxel pattern analysis (MVPA) to investigate long-term memory traces for temporal and spatial stimulus features in those areas. Participants were trained on four images associated with short versus long durations and with left versus right locations. Our results demonstrate stable representations of both temporal and spatial information in the right posterior intraparietal sulcus. Building upon previous findings of stable neuronal codes for directly perceived durations and locations, these results show that the reactivation of long-term memory traces for temporal and spatial features can be decoded from neuronal activation patterns in the right parietal cortex.


Assuntos
Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética , Lobo Parietal , Percepção Espacial , Humanos , Lobo Parietal/fisiologia , Lobo Parietal/diagnóstico por imagem , Masculino , Feminino , Adulto , Adulto Jovem , Percepção Espacial/fisiologia , Percepção do Tempo/fisiologia , Mapeamento Encefálico/métodos , Memória de Longo Prazo/fisiologia
6.
J Neurophysiol ; 132(1): 61-67, 2024 Jul 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38810256

RESUMO

Temporal intervals appear compressed at the time of saccades. Here, I asked if saccadic compression of time is related to motor planning or to saccade execution. To dissociate saccade motor planning from its execution, I used the double-step paradigm, in which subjects have to perform two horizontal saccades successively. At various times around the saccade sequence, I presented two large horizontal bars, which marked an interval lasting 100 ms. After 700 ms, a second temporal interval was presented, varying in duration across trials. Subjects were required to judge which interval appeared shorter. I found that during the first saccades in the double-step paradigm, temporal intervals were compressed. Maximum temporal compression coincided with saccade onset. Around the time of the second saccade, I found temporal compression as well, however, the time of maximum compression preceded saccade onset by about 70 ms. I compared the magnitude and time of temporal compression between double-step saccades and amplitude-matched single saccades, which I measured separately. Although I found no difference in time compression magnitude, the time when maximum compression occurred differed significantly. I conclude that the temporal shift of time compression in double-step saccades demonstrates the influence of saccade motor planning on time perception.NEW & NOTEWORTHY Visually defined temporal intervals appear compressed at the time of saccades. Here, I tested time perception during double-step saccades dissociating saccade planning from execution. Although around the time of the first saccade, peak compression was found at saccade onset, compression around the time of the second saccade peaked 70 ms before saccade onset. The results suggest that saccade motor planning influences time perception.


Assuntos
Movimentos Sacádicos , Percepção do Tempo , Movimentos Sacádicos/fisiologia , Humanos , Masculino , Adulto , Feminino , Percepção do Tempo/fisiologia , Adulto Jovem , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia
7.
Cerebellum ; 23(4): 1386-1398, 2024 Aug.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38147293

RESUMO

Temporal prediction (TP) influences our perception and cognition. The cerebellum could mediate this multi-level ability in a context-dependent manner. We tested whether a modulation of the cerebellar neural activity, induced by transcranial Direct Current Stimulation (tDCS), changed the TP ability according to the temporal features of the context and the duration of target interval. Fifteen healthy participants received anodal, cathodal, and sham tDCS (15 min × 2 mA intensity) over the right cerebellar hemisphere during a TP task. We recorded reaction times (RTs) to a target during the task in two contextual conditions of temporal anticipation: rhythmic (i.e., interstimulus intervals (ISIs) were constant) and single-interval condition (i.e., the estimation of the timing of the target was based on the prior exposure of the train of stimuli). Two ISIs durations were explored: 600 ms (short trials) and 900 ms (long trials). Cathodal tDCS improved the performance during the TP task (shorter RTs) specifically in the rhythmic condition only for the short trials and in the single-interval condition only for the long trials. Our results suggest that the inhibition of cerebellar activity induced a different improvement in the TP ability according to the temporal features of the context. In the rhythmic context, the cerebellum could integrate the temporal estimation with the anticipatory motor responses critically for the short target interval. In the single-interval context, for the long trials, the cerebellum could play a main role in integrating representation of time interval in memory with the elapsed time providing an accurate temporal prediction.


Assuntos
Cerebelo , Tempo de Reação , Percepção do Tempo , Estimulação Transcraniana por Corrente Contínua , Humanos , Cerebelo/fisiologia , Estimulação Transcraniana por Corrente Contínua/métodos , Masculino , Feminino , Adulto Jovem , Percepção do Tempo/fisiologia , Tempo de Reação/fisiologia , Adulto , Antecipação Psicológica/fisiologia
8.
Cogn Psychol ; 151: 101662, 2024 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38772251

RESUMO

Performing an action to initiate a consequence in the environment triggers the perceptual illusion of temporal binding. This phenomenon entails that actions and following effects are perceived to occur closer in time than they do outside the action-effect relationship. Here we ask whether temporal binding can be explained in terms of multisensory integration, by assuming either multisensory fusion or partial integration of the two events. We gathered two datasets featuring a wide range of action-effect delays as a key factor influencing integration. We then tested the fit of a computational model for multisensory integration, the statistically optimal cue integration (SOCI) model. Indeed, qualitative aspects of the data on a group-level followed the principles of a multisensory account. By contrast, quantitative evidence from a comprehensive model evaluation indicated that temporal binding cannot be reduced to multisensory integration. Rather, multisensory integration should be seen as one of several component processes underlying temporal binding on an individual level.


Assuntos
Percepção Visual , Humanos , Adulto , Masculino , Feminino , Percepção Visual/fisiologia , Adulto Jovem , Sinais (Psicologia) , Percepção Auditiva/fisiologia , Ilusões , Percepção do Tempo , Modelos Psicológicos
9.
Psychophysiology ; 61(8): e14585, 2024 Aug.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38594873

RESUMO

Accurate time perception is a crucial element in a wide range of cognitive tasks, including decision-making, memory, and motor control. One commonly observed phenomenon is that when given a range of time intervals to consider, people's estimates often cluster around the midpoint of those intervals. Previous studies have suggested that the range of these intervals can also influence our judgments, but the neural mechanisms behind this "range effect" are not yet understood. We used both behavioral tests and electroencephalographic (EEG) measures to understand how the range of sample time intervals affects the accuracy of people's subsequent time estimates. Study participants were exposed to two different setups: In the "blocked-range" (BR) session, short and long intervals were presented in separate blocks, whereas in the "interleaved-range" (IR) session, intervals of various lengths were presented randomly. Our findings indicated that the BR context led to more accurate time estimates compared to the IR context. In terms of EEG data, the BR context resulted in quicker buildup of contingent negative variation (CNV), which also reached higher amplitude levels and dissolved more rapidly during the encoding stage. We also observed an enhanced amplitude in the offset P2 component of the EEG signal. Overall, our results suggest that the variability in time intervals, as defined by their range, influences the neural processes that underlie time estimation.


Assuntos
Eletroencefalografia , Percepção do Tempo , Humanos , Masculino , Feminino , Adulto Jovem , Adulto , Percepção do Tempo/fisiologia , Variação Contingente Negativa/fisiologia , Potenciais Evocados/fisiologia
10.
Psychophysiology ; 61(8): e14589, 2024 Aug.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38615339

RESUMO

The neural circuits of reward processing and interval timing (including the perception and production of temporal intervals) are functionally intertwined, suggesting that it might be possible for momentary reward processing to influence subsequent timing behavior. Previous animal and human studies have mainly focused on the effect of reward on interval perception, whereas its impact on interval production is less clear. In this study, we examined whether feedback, as an example of performance-contingent reward, biases interval production. We recorded EEG from 20 participants while they engaged in a continuous drumming task with different realistic tempos (1728 trials per participant). Participants received color-coded feedback after each beat about whether they were correct (on time) or incorrect (early or late). Regression-based EEG analysis was used to unmix the rapid occurrence of a feedback response called the reward positivity (RewP), which is traditionally observed in more slow-paced tasks. Using linear mixed modeling, we found that RewP amplitude predicted timing behavior for the upcoming beat. This performance-biasing effect of the RewP was interpreted as reflecting the impact of fluctuations in reward-related anterior cingulate cortex activity on timing, and the necessity of continuous paradigms to make such observations was highlighted.


Assuntos
Eletroencefalografia , Desempenho Psicomotor , Recompensa , Percepção do Tempo , Humanos , Masculino , Feminino , Percepção do Tempo/fisiologia , Adulto , Adulto Jovem , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Giro do Cíngulo/fisiologia , Potenciais Evocados/fisiologia , Retroalimentação Psicológica/fisiologia
11.
Exp Brain Res ; 242(5): 1161-1174, 2024 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38489024

RESUMO

Mental Time Travel (MTT) allows us to remember past events and imagine future ones. According to previous literature, the Temporal Distance of events affects MTT: our ability to order events worsens for close, compared to far, events. However, those studies established distances a-priori, albeit the way we perceive events' temporal distance may subjectively differ from their objective distance. Thus, in the current study, we aimed to investigate the effects of Perceived Temporal Distance (PTD) on the MTT ability and the brain areas mediating this process. Thirty-three healthy volunteers took part in an fMRI MTT task. Participants were asked to project themselves into the past, present, or future, and to judge a series of events as relative-past or relative-future, in relation to the adopted time location. Outside the scanner, participants provided PTD estimates for each stimulus of the MTT task. Participants' performance and functional activity were analyzed as a function of these estimations. At the behavioural level, PTD predicts the modulation of the performance for relative-past and relative-future. Bilateral angular gyrus, retrosplenial cortex, temporo-parietal region and medial, middle and superior frontal gyri mediate the PTD effect. In addition to these areas, the closer the relative-future events are perceived, the higher the involvement of left parahippocampal and lingual gyri and right cerebellum. Thus, perceived proximity of events activates frontal and posterior parietal areas, which therefore might mediate the processing of PTD in the cognitive spatial representation of time. Future proximity also activates cerebellum and medial temporal areas, known to be involved in imaginative and constructive cognitive functions.


Assuntos
Mapeamento Encefálico , Encéfalo , Imaginação , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética , Percepção do Tempo , Humanos , Masculino , Feminino , Adulto , Percepção do Tempo/fisiologia , Adulto Jovem , Encéfalo/fisiologia , Encéfalo/diagnóstico por imagem , Imaginação/fisiologia
12.
Exp Brain Res ; 242(5): 1025-1036, 2024 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38451320

RESUMO

This study examined the relation between movement amplitude and tempo during self-paced rhythmic finger tapping to test a preferred velocity account of the preferred tempo construct. Preferred tempo refers to the concept that individuals have preferences for the pace of actions or events in their environment (e.g., the desired pace of walking or tempo of music). The preferred velocity hypothesis proposes that assessments of preferred tempo do not represent a pure time preference independent of spatial movement characteristics, but rather reflects a preference for an average movement velocity, predicting that preferred tempo will depend on movement amplitude. We tested this by having participants first perform a novel spontaneous motor amplitude (SMA) task in which they repetitively tapped their finger at their preferred amplitude without instructions about tapping tempo. Next, participants completed the spontaneous motor tempo (SMT) task in which they tapped their finger at their preferred tempo without instructions about tapping amplitude. Finally, participants completed a target amplitude version of the SMT task where they tapped at their preferred tempo at three target amplitudes (low, medium, and high). Participants (1) produced similar amplitudes and tempi regardless of instructions to produce either their preferred amplitude or preferred tempo, maintaining the same average movement velocity across SMA and SMT tasks and (2) altered their preferred tempo for different target amplitudes in the direction predicted by their estimated preferred velocity from the SMA and SMT tasks. Overall, results show the interdependence of movement amplitude and tempo in tapping assessments of preferred tempo.


Assuntos
Dedos , Movimento , Desempenho Psicomotor , Humanos , Masculino , Feminino , Dedos/fisiologia , Adulto Jovem , Movimento/fisiologia , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Adulto , Percepção do Tempo/fisiologia , Periodicidade , Adolescente
13.
Conscious Cogn ; 118: 103635, 2024 02.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38219402

RESUMO

William James's use of "time in passing" and "stream of thoughts" may be two sides of the same coin that emerge from the brain segmenting the continuous flow of information into discrete events. Herein, we investigated how the density of events affects two temporal experiences: the felt duration and speed of time. Using a temporal bisection task, participants classified seconds-long videos of naturalistic scenes as short or long (duration), or slow or fast (passage of time). Videos contained a varying number and type of events. We found that a large number of events lengthened subjective duration and accelerated the felt passage of time. Surprisingly, participants were also faster at estimating their felt passage of time compared to duration. The perception of duration scaled with duration and event density, whereas the felt passage of time scaled with the rate of change. Altogether, our results suggest that distinct mechanisms underlie these two experiential times.


Assuntos
Percepção do Tempo , Humanos , Tempo , Encéfalo , Emoções
14.
Conscious Cogn ; 119: 103665, 2024 03.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38354485

RESUMO

Intentional actions produce a temporal compression between the action and its outcome, known as intentional binding. However, Suzuki et al. (2019) recently showed that temporal compression can be observed without intentional actions. However, their results show a clear regression to the mean, which might have confounded the estimates of temporal intervals. To control these effects, we presented temporal intervals block-wise. Indeed, we found systematically greater compression for active than passive trials, in contrast to Suzuki et al. (2019). In our second experiment, our goal was to conceptually replicate the previous study. However, we were unable to reproduce their results and instead found more pronounced temporal compression in active trials compared to passive ones. In a subsequent attempt at a direct replication, we did not observe the same findings as the original study. Our findings reinforce the theory that intentions rather than causality cause temporal binding. During the preparation of this work, the authors used ChatGPT in order to improve the readability of the paper. After using this tool/service, the authors reviewed and edited the content as needed and take full responsibility for the content of the publication.


Assuntos
Percepção do Tempo , Humanos , Intenção , Desempenho Psicomotor
15.
Conscious Cogn ; 120: 103672, 2024 04.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38452630

RESUMO

The sense of agency is the ability to recognize that we are the actors of our actions and their consequences. We explored whether and how spatial cues may modulate the agency experience by manipulating the ecological validity of the experimental setup (real-space or computer-based setup) and the distance of the action-outcome (near or far). We tested 58 healthy adults collecting explicit agency judgments and the perceived time interval between movements and outcomes (to quantify the intentional binding phenomenon, an implicit index of agency). Participants show greater implicit agency for voluntary actions when there is a temporal and spatial action-outcome contingency. Conversely, participants reported similar explicit agency for outcomes appearing in the near and far space. Notably, these effects were independent of the ecological validity of the setting. These results suggest that spatial proximity, realistic or illusory, is essential for feeling implicitly responsible for the consequences of our actions.


Assuntos
Desempenho Psicomotor , Percepção do Tempo , Adulto , Humanos , Emoções , Julgamento , Sinais (Psicologia)
16.
Conscious Cogn ; 123: 103727, 2024 Aug.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38972289

RESUMO

The intentional binding effect refers to the phenomenon where the perceived temporal interval between a voluntary action and its sensory consequence is subjectively compressed. Prior research revealed the importance of tactile feedback from the keyboard on this effect. Here we examined the necessity of such tactile feedback by utilizing a touch-free key-press device without haptic feedback, and explored how initial/outcome sensory modalities (visual/auditory/tactile) and their consistency influence the intentional binding effect. Participants estimated three delay lengths (250, 550, or 850 ms) between the initial and outcome stimuli. Results showed that regardless of the combinations of sensory modalities between the initial and the outcome stimuli (i.e., modal consistency), the intentional binding effect was only observed in the 250 ms delay condition. This findings indicate a stable intentional binding effect both within and across sensory modalities, supporting the existence of a shared mechanism underlying the binding effect in touch-free voluntary actions.


Assuntos
Retroalimentação Sensorial , Intenção , Desempenho Psicomotor , Percepção do Tato , Humanos , Masculino , Feminino , Adulto Jovem , Adulto , Percepção do Tato/fisiologia , Retroalimentação Sensorial/fisiologia , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Percepção Visual/fisiologia , Percepção do Tempo/fisiologia , Percepção Auditiva/fisiologia , Volição/fisiologia
17.
Child Dev ; 95(4): 1299-1314, 2024.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38334228

RESUMO

This research investigated children's and adults' understanding of the mind by assessing beliefs about the temporal features of mental states. English-speaking North American participants, varying in socioeconomic status (Study 1: N = 50 adults; Study 2: N = 112, 8- to 10-year-olds and adults; and Study 3: N = 116, 5- to 7-year-olds and adults; tested 2017-2022), estimated the duration (seconds to a lifetime) of emotions, desires (wanting), preferences (liking), and control trials (e.g., napping and having eyes). Participants were 56% female and 44% male; 32% Asian, 1% Black, 13% Hispanic/Latino, 38% White (non-Hispanic/Latino), and 16% multiracial or another race/ethnicity. Children and adults judged that preferences last longer than emotions and desires, with age differences in distinguishing specific emotions by duration ( η p 2 s > .03 ). By 5 to 7 years, ideas about the mind include consideration of time.


Assuntos
Emoções , Humanos , Feminino , Masculino , Criança , Adulto , Emoções/fisiologia , Pré-Escolar , Adulto Jovem , Percepção do Tempo/fisiologia , Fatores Etários , Fatores de Tempo , Desenvolvimento Infantil/fisiologia
18.
Addict Biol ; 29(2): e13367, 2024 02.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38380757

RESUMO

Alcohol use disorder (AUD) has been associated with attentional deficits and impairments of working memory. Meanwhile, attention and working memory are critical for time perception. However, it remains unclear how time perception alters in AUD patients and how attention and working memory affect their time perception. The current study aims to clarify the time perception characteristics of AUD patients and the cognitive mechanisms underlying their time perception dysfunction. Thirty-one patients (three of them were excluded) with AUD and thirty-one matched controls completed the Time Bisection Task, Attention Network Test and Digital Span Backward Test to assess their abilities in time perception, attention network and working memory, respectively. The results showed that, after controlling for anxiety, depression, and impulsivity, AUD patients had a lower proportion of 'long' responses at intervals of 600, 750, 900, 1050 and 1200 ms. Furthermore, they displayed higher subjective equivalence points and higher Weber ratios compared to controls. Moreover, AUD patients showed impaired alerting and executive control networks as well as reduced working memory resources. Only working memory resources mediated the impact of AUD on time perception. In conclusion, our findings suggested that the duration underestimation in AUD patients is predominantly caused by working memory deficits.


Assuntos
Alcoolismo , Percepção do Tempo , Humanos , Memória de Curto Prazo/fisiologia , Função Executiva/fisiologia , Consumo de Bebidas Alcoólicas
19.
Perception ; 53(7): 405-414, 2024 Jul.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38465583

RESUMO

A novel motion stimulus is perceived to last longer than the subsequent motion stimulus moving in the opposite direction. A previous study suggested that the discrepancy in the processing latency for different onset types, as measured by reaction time, may play a role in this duration expansion. The present study examined whether the speed of motion stimuli influences this duration expansion. Experiment 1 demonstrated that the duration expansion ceased to occur when the stimulus speed increased. Experiment 2 showed that the increase in the speed reduced the reaction time for various onset types. However, the size of the changes in the reaction time did not match the reduction in the magnitude of the duration expansion observed in Experiment 1. These results suggest that the increase in speed eliminates the duration expansion of the novel motion stimulus, but the difference in the processing latency alone may not be the sole mechanism.


Assuntos
Percepção de Movimento , Tempo de Reação , Humanos , Percepção de Movimento/fisiologia , Tempo de Reação/fisiologia , Adulto Jovem , Adulto , Masculino , Feminino , Percepção do Tempo/fisiologia
20.
Psychol Res ; 88(3): 815-825, 2024 Apr.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38206333

RESUMO

Sense of agency (SoA) is the sense of having control over one's own actions and through them events in the outside world. Sometimes temporal cues, that is temporal contiguity between action and effect, or temporal expectation regarding the occurrence of the effect are used to infer whether one has agency over an effect. This has mainly been investigated in Western cultures. However, Western and Eastern cultures differ in their time concepts and thus their usage of temporal cues may also differ. We investigated whether Western and Eastern cultures (Austrian vs. Mongolian students) use temporal cues differently. Participants performed adaption blocks in which actions were followed by immediate (immediate effect group) or by delayed (delayed effect group) effects. In subsequent test blocks the action-effect delay was varied and participants' SoA over the effect was assessed. In Austrian students, the immediate effect group experienced more SoA for short action-effect delays, whereas the reverse was true for the delayed effect group. Thus, temporal expectation rather than temporal contiguity is used as predominant agency cue. In Mongolian students, SoA did not significantly differ between different action-effect delays in both groups, indicating that Mongolian students hardly rely on temporal cues. In conclusion, due to linear time concepts in Western cultures, the timing of an effect may be an important agency cue in Austrian students. However, due to cyclical time concepts in some Eastern cultures, it may be a less important agency cue in Mongolian students. Thus, the use of temporal agency cues is culture-dependent.


Assuntos
Desempenho Psicomotor , Percepção do Tempo , Humanos , Sinais (Psicologia)
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA