Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 155
Filtrar
Más filtros

Tipo del documento
Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
Int J Lang Commun Disord ; 58(6): 1927-1938, 2023.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37260193

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Weak theory of mind (ToM) ability is a core deficit in children with autism. A growing body of work has found that there is a close relation between complement syntax and ToM in autistic children. However, researchers have not yet investigated whether other linguistic components may explain the difficulties in ToM reasoning in autistic children. AIMS: To determine whether verb factuality (i.e., mental and action verbs) is related to ToM ability, as measured by a false belief understanding (FBU) task after controlling the effect of complement syntax for Mandarin-speaking autistic children. METHODS & PROCEDURES: Participants were verbal autistic children, aged 4-7 years. Their IQ performance (verbal, performance and total IQ) and the comprehension of complement syntax were evaluated. A total of 38 children scoring over 9 points in complement syntax test and 90 points in the verbal IQ test were selected to complete verb factuality and FBU task. The χ2 tests and correlation analyses were carried out on two relations: (1) ToM ability and understanding of verb factuality; and (2) ToM ability and comprehension of complement syntax. OUTCOMES & RESULTS: A total of 11 autistic children completed the action verb factuality task, while 14 completed the mental verb factuality task and 13 completed both tasks. Participants performed well on the verb factuality task, and their ToM performance appeared to be related to their linguistic ability, regardless of the type of verb (i.e., action or mental verb). However, complement syntax scores did not significantly predict the success of the FBU task for the autistic children. CONCLUSIONS & IMPLICATIONS: The results of this study link weaker ToM ability with the understanding of verb factuality among autistic children aged 4-7 and provide new evidence for the view that the development of language facilitates improvement of ToM skills. The findings shed new light on how language affects or determines social interactions. WHAT THIS PAPER ADDS: What is already known on the subject ToM ability is crucial for social interaction. It has been claimed that mental verb factuality (e.g., think, had thought) may play a role in the development of ToM reasoning in typically developing children, but whether there is a link between the mastery of verb factuality and ToM skills in autistic children is still unclear. What this study adds to existing knowledge Correlational analysis revealed links between verbal ToM measured by unexpected location task and factuality test of mental verbs as well as action verbs. More importantly, this study confirmed the extra role of verb factuality in explaining ToM ability after excluding the influence of the complement syntax. What are the potential or actual clinical implications of this work? This study suggests that the factuality of verbs could serve as a tool for autistic children to infer the mental states of others. Thus, training on this linguistic structure could be taken into consideration when improving the social skills of autistic children.


Asunto(s)
Trastorno Autístico , Teoría de la Mente , Humanos , Niño , Lenguaje , Pruebas del Lenguaje
2.
Psychiatr Psychol Law ; 30(3): 229-248, 2023.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37346057

RESUMEN

The study aimed to establish a normative data set for the Paulhus Deception Scales (PDS) and Structured Inventory of Malingered Symptomology (SIMS) in a community adult sample of high functioning autistic (HFA) people. Assessments were administered anonymously online. Seventy surveys were completed, with respondents contributing from 16 countries. The majority of subscales and total scores for the PDS and SIMS fell above cut-off for self-report response bias, suggesting that completion of these measures by HFA individuals may lead to conclusions of intentional response distortion, even when this is not the case. Significant relationships were found between high scores and education level, as well as psychological distress. The findings of the study raise concerns about the use of these measures with HFA people, particularly in 'high stakes' situations.

3.
Cogn Process ; 23(1): 129-153, 2022 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34487273

RESUMEN

Few works have addressed the processing of indirect requests in High-Functioning Autism (HFA), and results are conflicting. Some studies report HFA individuals' difficulties in indirect requests comprehension; others suggest that it might be preserved in HFA. Furthermore, the role of Theory of Mind in understanding indirect requests is an open issue. The goal of this work is twofold: first, assessing whether comprehension of indirect requests for information is preserved in HFA; second, exploring whether mind-reading skills predict this ability. We tested a group of (n = 14; 9-12 years) HFA children and two groups of younger (n = 19; 5-6 years) and older (n = 28; 9-12 years) typically developing (TD) children in a semi-structured task involving direct, indirect and highly indirect requests for information. Results suggested that HFA can understand indirect and highly indirect requests, as well as TD children. Yet, while Theory of Mind skills seem to enhance older TD children understanding, this is not the case for HFA children. Therefore, interestingly, they could rely on different interpretative strategies.


Asunto(s)
Trastorno del Espectro Autista , Trastorno Autístico , Niño , Humanos
4.
Eat Weight Disord ; 27(4): 1555-1561, 2022 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34264484

RESUMEN

PURPOSE: The relationship between autism spectrum disorders (ASDs) and eating disorders (EDs) has been widely studied in the last decades. We aimed to directly compare patients with EDs, individuals with high-functioning ASDs (HF-ASDs) and healthy controls (HC) at measures detecting: (1) symptoms of eating disorders, (2) eating disturbances known to be characteristic of autism. METHODS: Thirty-four patients with EDs, 34 individuals with HF-ASDs and 35 HC, all females, completed the eating attitude test (EAT-26) and the Swedish eating assessment for autism spectrum disorders (SWEAA), two self-report questionnaires assessing, respectively, symptoms and concerns characteristic of eating disorders and ASD-related eating disturbances. RESULTS: At the EAT-26, patients with EDs scored significantly higher than individuals with HF-ASDs, and both of them scored higher than HC (p < 0.05, ηp2 = 0.283). Conversely, at the SWEAA, no differences between individuals with HF-ASDs and patients with EDs emerged (p = 901), but they both scored higher than HC (p < 0.05, ηp2 = 0.247). CONCLUSION: Individuals with HF-ASDs did not seem to reach the same level of EDs symptomatology as patients with EDs. Patients with EDs did not seem to present a different amount of autistic-eating behaviours than subjects with HF-ASDs. Patients with EDs and individuals with HF-ASDs scored higher than HC at both scales. Our results give further preliminary evidence of the overlap between autistic traits and EDs symptomatology, and should be taken into account in the definition of a shared model between EDs and ASDs. LEVEL OF EVIDENCE: Level II; Evidence obtained from controlled trial without randomization.


Asunto(s)
Trastorno del Espectro Autista , Trastorno Autístico , Trastornos de Alimentación y de la Ingestión de Alimentos , Trastorno del Espectro Autista/complicaciones , Trastornos de Alimentación y de la Ingestión de Alimentos/complicaciones , Femenino , Humanos , Autoinforme , Encuestas y Cuestionarios
5.
Occup Ther Health Care ; : 1-18, 2022 Sep 16.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36112015

RESUMEN

This study explored the effect of music on the driving performance of experienced young adult drivers with and without autism spectrum disorder (ASD) using an interactive driving simulator with two types of scenarios. A 2 (Group: autism/neurotypical) × 2 (Music: music/no music) × 2 (Scenario: hazards/wayfinding) factorial design was used with the order of scenarios and music conditions counterbalanced. Participants were 34 neurotypical drivers and 5 drivers with ASD, all with at least 3 years of driving experience. Paired sample t-tests demonstrated no effect of music for any condition. Overall, the drivers with ASD had higher performance means than the neurotypical group with significant differences in the wayfinding scenario and the category for following regulations. Neurotypical drivers had better performance on the wayfinding scenario than the hazard scenario in maneuvers and being attentive to the environment. Although this study had a low number of drivers with ASD, it suggests drivers with ASD can perform better than neurotypical drivers, possibly because they follow road rule guidelines more consistently than neurotypical peers. In addition, this is the first study to examine wayfinding skills in teens/young adults with and without ASD.

6.
Cogn Process ; 22(4): 659-673, 2021 Nov.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34152544

RESUMEN

Research on how children with neurodevelopmental disorders perceive, process, and interpret visual illusions (VIs) has been extensively focused on children with autism spectrum disorder providing controversial findings. In this study, we investigated the patterns of vulnerability to a wide set of VIs comprising 23 standard text book VIs and their variations in a clinical sample of children with neurodevelopmental disorders compared to typically developing children (TD). A total of 176 children, aged between 4.6 and 13.8 years old, were distributed into four groups: high-functioning autism (HFA; N = 23), attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD; N = 42), specific learning disorder (SLD; N = 70), and TD (N = 41). Regression models, adjusted for sex, age, and non-verbal IQ, showed that HFA was associated with greater responses accuracy than TD children to the full battery of VIs, to the cognitive illusions, to the distortions, and to both geometrical illusions of size/shape (cognitive distortions) and lightness contrast effects (physical distortions). The susceptibility of ADHD children was found attenuated for illusory contours and greater for paradoxical illusions in comparison with TD children. No significant differences were shown between the SLD group and the TD children. Our findings, which were adjusted for the same duration of visual working memory across groups, showed that there is a potential specific tendency of HFA children to failure of processing visual information in context. Contrarily, children with ADHD showed in general normal global processing such as children diagnosed with SLD.


Asunto(s)
Trastorno por Déficit de Atención con Hiperactividad , Trastorno del Espectro Autista , Ilusiones , Trastornos del Neurodesarrollo , Adolescente , Trastorno del Espectro Autista/complicaciones , Niño , Preescolar , Humanos , Memoria a Corto Plazo
7.
J Neural Transm (Vienna) ; 127(1): 81-94, 2020 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31838600

RESUMEN

Copy-number variants (CNVs), in particular rare, small and large ones (< 1% frequency) and those encompassing brain-related genes, have been shown to be associated with neurodevelopmental disorders like autism spectrum disorders (ASDs), attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), and intellectual disability (ID). However, the vast majority of CNV findings lack specificity with respect to autistic or developmental-delay phenotypes. Therefore, the aim of the study was to investigate the size and frequency of CNVs in high-functioning ASD (HFA) without ID compared with a random population sample and with published findings in ASD and ID. To investigate the role of CNVs for the "core symptoms" of high-functioning autism, we included in the present exploratory study only patients with HFA without ID. The aim was to test whether HFA have similar large rare (> 1 Mb) CNVs as reported in ASD and ID. We performed high-resolution chromosomal microarray analysis in 108 children and adolescents with HFA without ID. There was no significant difference in the overall number of rare CNVs compared to 124 random population samples. However, patients with HFA carried significantly more frequently CNVs containing brain-related genes. Surprisingly, six HFA patients carried very large CNVs known to be typically present in ID. Our findings provide new evidence that not only small, but also large CNVs affecting several key genes contribute to the genetic etiology/risk of HFA without affecting their intellectual ability.


Asunto(s)
Trastorno del Espectro Autista/genética , Variaciones en el Número de Copia de ADN/genética , Discapacidad Intelectual/genética , Adolescente , Niño , Preescolar , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Análisis por Micromatrices
8.
BMC Psychiatry ; 20(1): 152, 2020 04 06.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32252695

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Fairness has received much attention in our society. At present, the findings regarding fair decision-making in high-functioning autism spectrum disorder (HF-ASD) are inconsistent. Previous studies have shown that the fair decision-making of typically developing children is influenced by theory of mind (ToM) and executive functioning (EF). As those with HF-ASD have defects in both domains, this study aims to explore the differences in fair decision-making between children and adolescents with HF-ASD and those with typical development (TD). METHODS: We used a simple ultimatum game (UG) to explore 31 children and adolescents with HF-ASD and 38 children and adolescents with TD. T tests and chi-square tests were used to compare group differences, and Pearson correlation analysis and stepwise regression analysis were used to analyse the mechanisms influencing the two groups' unfair acceptance rates. RESULTS: The results show that children with HF-ASD are more likely to accept unfair offers, but for adolescents, the difference is not significant. Regression analysis showed that the interaction between the behavior regulation index (BRI) and age could negatively predict the unfair acceptance rate of children and adolescents with HF-ASD. Working memory and ToM can negatively predict the unfair acceptance rate of those with TD. CONCLUSION: This study concluded that the development of fair decision-making by children and adolescents with HF-ASD falls far behind that of those with TD. Intuition processes play a dominant role in the fair decision-making processes of children and adolescents with HF-ASD, and we believe that comorbidity, age, experience and emotional management are important factors influencing the fair decision-making of individuals with HF-ASD.


Asunto(s)
Trastorno del Espectro Autista/psicología , Toma de Decisiones/fisiología , Función Ejecutiva/fisiología , Teoría de la Mente/fisiología , Adolescente , Trastorno Autístico , Niño , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Memoria a Corto Plazo/fisiología , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas
9.
Cogn Neuropsychiatry ; 25(3): 163-178, 2020 05.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31931670

RESUMEN

Introduction: Cognitive impairments are common in both Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASD) and schizophrenia, but it is unclear whether the pattern of difficulties is similar or different in the two disorders. This cross-sectional and longitudinal study compared the neuropsychological functioning in adolescents with ASD with adolescents with Early Onset Schizophrenia (EOS).Methods: At baseline and at two-year follow-up, participants were assessed with a brief neuropsychological test battery measuring executive functions, visual and verbal learning, delayed recall and recognition and psychomotor speed.Results: We found similar levels of neuropsychological impairment across groups and over time in the adolescents with ASD or EOS. Adolescents in both groups did not improve significantly on verbal learning, verbal delayed recall, visual learning, visual delayed recall or visual delayed recognition, and both groups performed poorer on verbal recognition. Both groups improved on measures of psychomotor processing and executive functions.Conclusion: The findings suggest that it may be difficult to differentiate adolescents with EOS and ASD based on neuropsychological task performance. An implication of the results is that adolescents with either disorder may benefit from a similar approach to the treatment of cognitive impairment in the disorders.


Asunto(s)
Trastorno del Espectro Autista/diagnóstico , Trastorno del Espectro Autista/psicología , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Esquizofrenia/diagnóstico , Psicología del Esquizofrénico , Adolescente , Edad de Inicio , Niño , Estudios Transversales , Función Ejecutiva/fisiología , Femenino , Estudios de Seguimiento , Humanos , Estudios Longitudinales , Masculino , Reconocimiento en Psicología/fisiología , Aprendizaje Verbal/fisiología
10.
Zhonghua Yi Xue Za Zhi ; 100(31): 2446-2451, 2020 Aug 18.
Artículo en Zh | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32819061

RESUMEN

Objective: To explore the characteristics of executive function in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder comorbid with high functioning autism. Methods: A total of 165 children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD group), 65 children with attention-deficit/Hyperactivity disorder comorbid with high functioning autism (ADHD-HFA group), and 84 healthy controls (control group) (based on the criteria of DSM-5) were recruited from the Outpatient Clinic of Child Healthcare Department of Shen Zhen Children's Hospital. The Rey complex figure test (RCFT), trail making test (TMT), Stroop color-word test were used to assess working memory, shifting and inhibition. Results: ADHD group (2.1±1.9, 7±5, 2.1±2.0 and 7±5) and ADHD-HFA group (2.0±2.0, 7±6, 2.0±2.1 and 6±5) performed worse than control group (3.4±2.0, 10±5, 3.4±2.0 and 10±6) in Rey complex figure test (all P<0.05). ADHD group ((171±8) s, (27.40±0.82) s and (52.29±1.62) s) and ADHD-HFA group ((197±11) s, (29.7±1.1) s and (58.6±2.1) s) group took longer time on the TMT-2, Stroop2 and Stroop4 test than control group ((135±18) s, (22.4±1.9) s and (38.7±3.8) s) (all P<0.05). In children with low intelligence quotient (IQ), ADHD group ((30±8) s) and ADHD-HFA group ((34±9) s) performed worse on Stroop3 test than control group ((20±4) s) (all P<0.05). In children with average IQ, ADHD group ((19±5) s and (24±8) s) took longer time on the Stroop1 and Stroop3 test than control group ((16±3) s and (19±4) s) (all P<0.05). In children with high IQ, ADHD-HFA group ((20±8) s) spent more time on Stroop1 than control group ((15±4) s) (P<0.05). Inattention symptoms were associated with the time on TMT-2 of ADHD-HFA group (r=0.275 and 0.329, all P<0.05). The score of item 1 in autism spectrum screening questionnaire (ASSQ) was negatively correlated with immediate recall structure and detail scores as well as delay structure scores of Rey complex figure test (r=-0.358, -0.326 and -0.306, all P<0.05). The score of item 4 was positively correlated with errors of Stroop4 (r=0.296, P<0.05). The score of item 22 was positively correlated with time of color interference (r=0.279, P<0.05). Conclusions: Children with ADHD-HFA are likely to demonstrate the spatial working memory, shifting and inhibition deficits associated with ADHD alone. Some domains of executive function impairment in ADHD-HFA group are related with symptoms of inattention/hyperactivity and autism.


Asunto(s)
Trastorno por Déficit de Atención con Hiperactividad , Trastorno Autístico , Niño , Comorbilidad , Función Ejecutiva , Humanos , Memoria a Corto Plazo , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas
11.
Neurocase ; 25(6): 217-224, 2019 12.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31522609

RESUMEN

In high-functioning autism, deficits in emotional processing and theory of mind are relevant to understanding the particularities of social functioning. Here we used a multiple baseline and ABA single-case design to assess the efficacy of an individualized social cognitive training program using both pen-and-paper and computerized materials for an 18-year old patient. After the treatment phase, we found significant improvement in both emotional processes and theory of mind. These results provide further significant data showing that therapeutic tools based on digital relational simulation are a promising way for helping people with autism to compensate for their impaired social functioning.


Asunto(s)
Trastorno del Espectro Autista/psicología , Trastorno del Espectro Autista/terapia , Terapia Cognitivo-Conductual , Conducta Social , Adolescente , Terapia Cognitivo-Conductual/métodos , Humanos , Masculino , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Proyectos Piloto , Ensayos Clínicos Controlados Aleatorios como Asunto , Resultado del Tratamiento
12.
J Intellect Disabil Res ; 63(10): 1207-1220, 2019 10.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31267604

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) experience set-shifting deficit as a part of executive function, which can lead to cognitive and behavioural flexibility deficits and/or restricted behaviours. Despite the increasing body of research on this cognitive deficit, set-shifting training has not been exclusively studied in ASD. AIMS: In this study, a training condition [set-shifting improvement tasks (SSIT)] was developed to improve set-shifting ability; afterwards, the possible effects of these tasks were investigated. METHODS AND PROCEDURES: With the aim of improving set-shifting ability in children with autism, a training program (SSIT), involving a computer game (Tatka, a puzzle game produced by our research team) with some home-based tasks (for generalisation purposes), was developed. Then, in a quasi-experimental design, the effects of SSIT tasks were studied on children (n = 13, 5-7 years old) with high-functioning autism. Outcome measures (pre-training, post-training and a 6-week follow-up) were assessed using Modified Wisconsin Card Sorting Test, Bender-Gestalt Test and Behavioural Flexibility Rating Scale. RESULTS AND OUTCOMES: A significant change was observed in both cognitive (Bender Gestalt, ηp2=0.84 ; WCST; =p20.87 ) and behavioural flexibilities ( ηp2=0.79 ) and also in repetitive behaviours ( ηp2=0.45 ). Furthermore, the result remained stable to some extent for about 1 month after the training condition. CONCLUSIONS AND IMPLICATIONS: Developing the SSIT is just an initial step in the major target of creating cognitive rehabilitation tools to be used by clinicians and parents for children diagnosed with ASD and should be understood as a supplement, rather than an alternative, to the main treatments such as applied behaviour analysis. Future research with larger samples are needed to confirm whether this intervention is effective for children with ASD.


Asunto(s)
Trastorno del Espectro Autista , Disfunción Cognitiva , Remediación Cognitiva/métodos , Función Ejecutiva/fisiología , Trastorno del Espectro Autista/complicaciones , Trastorno del Espectro Autista/fisiopatología , Trastorno del Espectro Autista/rehabilitación , Niño , Preescolar , Disfunción Cognitiva/etiología , Disfunción Cognitiva/fisiopatología , Disfunción Cognitiva/rehabilitación , Remediación Cognitiva/instrumentación , Femenino , Juegos Experimentales , Humanos , Masculino , Evaluación de Resultado en la Atención de Salud
13.
Phys Occup Ther Pediatr ; 39(4): 373-387, 2019.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30188233

RESUMEN

The Parentship protocol is a short-term intervention program in occupational therapy for parents of adolescents with High-Functioning Autism Spectrum Disorder (HFASD). Its purpose is to promote parental resilience and enhance adolescents' participation in daily life. Aims: To explore and analyze the perceptions of occupational therapists and parents of individuals with HFASD, regarding the content of the protocol and its theoretical framework. Methods: Implementation of a phenomenological qualitative approach using two focus groups (six occupational therapists and five parents). A transcript-based analysis was used for analyzing the data. Results: The degree of agreement regarding the potential purposes and contents of the protocol was high. In addition, nine themes were raised and led to changes and additions in the protocol. Conclusions: The study provided support for content validity and acceptability of the Parentship protocol. Future research should test the feasibility of this new intervention program.


Asunto(s)
Trastorno del Espectro Autista/terapia , Educación , Terapeutas Ocupacionales , Padres/educación , Actividades Cotidianas , Adolescente , Femenino , Grupos Focales , Humanos , Relaciones Padres-Hijo , Relaciones Profesional-Familia , Investigación Cualitativa , Encuestas y Cuestionarios
14.
Psychol Med ; 48(8): 1264-1273, 2018 06.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28920569

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Evidence suggests that autism and schizophrenia share similarities in genetic, neuropsychological and behavioural aspects. Although both disorders are associated with theory of mind (ToM) impairments, a few studies have directly compared ToM between autism patients and schizophrenia patients. This study aimed to investigate to what extent high-functioning autism patients and schizophrenia patients share and differ in ToM performance. METHODS: Thirty high-functioning autism patients, 30 schizophrenia patients and 30 healthy individuals were recruited. Participants were matched in age, gender and estimated intelligence quotient. The verbal-based Faux Pas Task and the visual-based Yoni Task were utilised to examine first- and higher-order, affective and cognitive ToM. The task/item difficulty of two paradigms was examined using mixed model analyses of variance (ANOVAs). Multiple ANOVAs and mixed model ANOVAs were used to examine group differences in ToM. RESULTS: The Faux Pas Task was more difficult than the Yoni Task. High-functioning autism patients showed more severely impaired verbal-based ToM in the Faux Pas Task, but shared similar visual-based ToM impairments in the Yoni Task with schizophrenia patients. CONCLUSIONS: The findings that individuals with high-functioning autism shared similar but more severe impairments in verbal ToM than individuals with schizophrenia support the autism-schizophrenia continuum. The finding that verbal-based but not visual-based ToM was more impaired in high-functioning autism patients than schizophrenia patients could be attributable to the varied task/item difficulty between the two paradigms.


Asunto(s)
Trastorno Autístico/psicología , Psicología del Esquizofrénico , Teoría de la Mente , Adulto , Estudios de Casos y Controles , Femenino , Hong Kong , Hospitales Psiquiátricos , Humanos , Pruebas de Inteligencia , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Análisis Multivariante , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Análisis y Desempeño de Tareas
15.
J Exp Child Psychol ; 171: 1-13, 2018 07.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29494824

RESUMEN

This study examined judgment about punishment and whether punishment promoted cooperation in the prisoner's dilemma game (PDG) in children with high-functioning autism (HFA) and typically developing (TD) children. In total, 66 6- to 12-year-olds participated in this study. Children were first asked about judgments regarding rewards and punishment in stories, and then they were asked to play the PDG with a partner in conditions with and without punishment. Results showed that children with HFA believed that hitting others should deserve punishment to a greater extent than TD children did. It indicated that children with HFA understood that bad acts should be punished, suggesting that these children have already acquired the general concept of "punishment." Children displayed higher levels of cooperation in the condition with punishment than in the condition without punishment in the PDG, suggesting that punishment promoted cooperation in the PDG in both children with HFA and TD children.


Asunto(s)
Trastorno Autístico/psicología , Conducta Cooperativa , Castigo , Estudios de Casos y Controles , Niño , Femenino , Juegos Recreacionales , Humanos , Juicio , Masculino , Recompensa
16.
Clin Linguist Phon ; 31(3): 234-249, 2017.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27739870

RESUMEN

Some overlap has been suggested among the subtypes of autism spectrum disorder (ASD) in children. The Japanese version of the Children's Communication Checklist-2 (CCC-2) is a useful measure for identifying profiles in relation to communication impairments in children with ASD. The aim of this study was to investigate whether the CCC-2 could identify subtypes in relation to communication impairments in Japanese children with ASD. The study participants were 113 children with ASD but without intellectual disabilities aged 3-12 years. Parents were given the Japanese version of the CCC-2 and asked to rate their children, who were then classified into two groups based on statistical analysis. Significant differences were found between clusters in mean CCC-2 subscales. These results suggest that one subtype was associated with low language competence and strong characteristics of autism, while the other was associated with relatively high language competence and milder characteristics of autism.


Asunto(s)
Pueblo Asiatico , Trastorno del Espectro Autista/clasificación , Lista de Verificación/estadística & datos numéricos , Comunicación , Lista de Verificación/normas , Niño , Preescolar , Análisis por Conglomerados , Femenino , Humanos , Japón , Masculino , Padres , Encuestas y Cuestionarios
17.
J Neural Transm (Vienna) ; 123(3): 353-63, 2016 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26559825

RESUMEN

The Contactin Associated Protein-like 2 (CNTNAP2) gene has been discussed to be associated with different symptoms of autism spectrum disorders (ASDs) and other neurodevelopmental disorders. We aimed to elucidate the genetic association of CNTNAP2 within high functioning ASD (HFA), focusing on autism specific symptoms and reducing intelligence related factors. Furthermore, we compared our findings conducting a meta-analysis in patients with ASD and HFA only. A case-control association study was performed for HFA (HFA, n = 105; controls, n = 133). Moreover, we performed a family-based association study (DFAM) analysis (HFA, n = 44; siblings, n = 57). Individuals were genotyped for the two most frequently reported single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) in the CNTNAP2 gene (rs2710102, rs7794745). Furthermore, a meta-analysis using the MIX2 software integrated our results with previously published data. A significant association for the carriers of the T-allele of the rs7794745 with HFA was found in the case-control sample [OR = 1.547; (95 % CI 1.056-2.266); p = 0.025]. No association could be found by DFAM with any of the CNTNAP2 SNPs with HFA. The meta-analysis of both SNPs did not show a significant association with either ASD or with HFA. Overall, including case-control, sibs, and meta-analysis, we could not detect any significant association with the CNTNAP2 gene and HFA. Our results point in the direction that CNTNAP2 may not play a major role in HFA, but rather seems to have a significance in neurodevelopmental disorders or in individuals displaying intellectual delays.


Asunto(s)
Trastorno Autístico/genética , Proteínas de la Membrana/genética , Proteínas del Tejido Nervioso/genética , Adolescente , Estudios de Casos y Controles , Niño , Preescolar , Femenino , Predisposición Genética a la Enfermedad/genética , Genotipo , Humanos , Masculino , Reacción en Cadena de la Polimerasa , Polimorfismo de Nucleótido Simple , Hermanos
18.
Int J Lang Commun Disord ; 51(2): 137-47, 2016 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26215207

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: There is variability in the language of children with autism, even those who are high functioning. However, little is known about how they process language structures in real time, including how they handle potential ambiguity, and whether they follow referential constraints. Previous research with older autism spectrum disorder (ASD) participants has shown that these individuals can use context to access rapidly the meaning of ambiguous words. The severity of autism has also been shown to influence the speed in which children with ASD access lexical information. AIMS: To understand more about how children with ASD process language in real time (i.e., as it unfolds). The focus was the integration of information and use of referential constraints to identify a referent named in a sentence. METHODS & PROCEDURES: We used an eye-tracking task to compare performance between young, high-functioning children with autism (HFA) and children with typical development (TD). A large sample of 5-9-year-old children (mean age = 6;8 years), 48 with HFA and 56 with TD participated; all were attending mainstream schools. For each item participants were shown a display of four images that differed in two dimensions. Each sentence contained an adjective and noun that restricted the choice from four to two (the target and competitor), followed by a prepositional phrase (e.g., the blue square with dots); this added modifying information to provide a unique description of the target. We calculated looking time at the target, the competitor and the two distractors for each 200 ms time interval as children processed the sentence and looked at the display. Generalized estimating equations were used to carry out repeated-measures analyses on the proportion of looking time to target and competitor and time to fixate to target. OUTCOMES & RESULTS: Children in both groups (HFA and TD) looked at the target and competitor more than at the distractors following the adjective and noun and following the modifying information in the prepositional phrase more at the target. However, the HFA group was significantly slower in both phases and looked proportionally less at the target. Across the sample, IQ and language did not affect the results; however, age and attention had an impact. The older children showed an advantage in processing the information as did the children with higher attention scores. CONCLUSIONS & IMPLICATIONS: The HFA group took longer than the TD group to integrate the disambiguating information provided in the course of processing a sentence and integrate it with the visual information, indicating that for the ASD group incremental processing was not as advanced as for children with ASD, and they were less sensitive to referential conventions. Training for young children with ASD on the use of referential conventions and available contextual clues may be of benefit to them in understanding the language they hear.


Asunto(s)
Trastorno del Espectro Autista/diagnóstico , Comprensión , Lingüística , Lectura , Semántica , Aprendizaje Verbal , Trastorno del Espectro Autista/psicología , Niño , Preescolar , Formación de Concepto , Femenino , Humanos , Inteligencia , Masculino , Tiempo de Reacción , Valores de Referencia , Percepción del Habla
19.
Phys Occup Ther Pediatr ; 36(1): 46-58, 2016.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26422262

RESUMEN

AIMS: Children with high-functioning Autism Spectrum Disorder (HFASD) have major difficulties in social communication skills, which may impact their performance and participation in everyday life. The goal of this study was to examine whether the StoryTable, an intervention paradigm based on a collaborative narrative, multitouch tabletop interface, enhanced social interaction for children with HFASD, and to determine whether the acquired abilities were transferred to behaviors during other tasks. METHODS: Fourteen boys with HFASD, aged 7-12 years, participated in a 3-week, 11-session intervention. Social interactions during two nonintervention tasks were videotaped at three points in time, one prior to the intervention (pre), a second immediately following the intervention (post) and a third three weeks after the intervention (follow-up). The video-recorded files were coded using the Friendship Observation Scale to ascertain the frequencies of positive and negative social interactions and collaborative play. Differences in these behaviors were tested for significance using nonparametric statistical tests. RESULTS: There were significantly higher rates of positive social interactions and collaborative play, and lower rates of negative social interactions following the intervention suggesting generalization of the social skills learned during the intervention. Improvement was maintained when tested three weeks later. CONCLUSION: These findings provide support for the use of collaborative technology-based interventions within educational settings to enhance social interaction of children with HFASD.


Asunto(s)
Trastorno del Espectro Autista/diagnóstico , Trastorno del Espectro Autista/rehabilitación , Terapia Cognitivo-Conductual/métodos , Relaciones Interpersonales , Niño , Manual Diagnóstico y Estadístico de los Trastornos Mentales , Juegos Experimentales , Humanos , Masculino , Medición de Riesgo , Muestreo , Índice de Severidad de la Enfermedad , Conducta Social , Resultado del Tratamiento
20.
Encephale ; 41(1): 47-55, 2015 Feb.
Artículo en Francés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25439857

RESUMEN

INTRODUCTION: The category of pervasive developmental disorders (PDD) without intellectual disability (including Asperger syndrome and high-functioning autism) has increased steadily among individuals since the 1980s. Although some symptoms may decrease with age, functional disability persists and is largely related to abnormalities in social interaction. Within the framework of PDD without intellectual disability, improving social skills appears to be a primary target for intervention programs. Despite a recent increase in the number of studies on this topic, few validated programs are yet available for clinical settings. BACKGROUND: We have developed an intervention targeting the improvement of social skills from the analysis of video sequences. The goal of this intervention is to promote communication within the group through sharing their interests and emotions, and to enhance the understanding of social situations. In order to assess the efficiency of this intervention, we have conducted a prospective, open, and uncontrolled study. First, it aimed at assessing the immediate effect of our intervention on a single social skill (communication) in an experimental situation (in the group) and in an ecological situations (family and school). Second, this study aimed at assessing the effects of this intervention on the subjects' social adjustment. METHOD: This study included 16 individuals with high-functioning autism/Asperger syndrome. Participants were evaluated before and after a 6-month video-based training using measures of socio-communicative and adaptive skills. RESULTS: Results revealed a statistically significant increase in the communication skills not only in the group (15.5%), but also at home (13.7%) and at school (8.7%). The evaluation of socio-adaptive behavior indicates a statistically significant increase in communication (12%), family (7%) and social autonomy (8%), and leisure activities (8%). DISCUSSION: The communication and social adjustment scores obtained upon inclusion were low, despite low autistic intensity scores. However, the improvement at six months was significant for most studied variables. These results are consistent with our clinical findings and seem partly explained by the use of video supports as the mediator of exchanges within the group. However, because of some methodological limitations, the conclusions on the effects of the intervention should be nuanced. CONCLUSIONS: This type of intervention seems to be an interesting therapeutic indication for individuals with high-functioning autism/Asperger syndrome. The first results are encouraging, and all participants enjoyed attending the meetings. These conclusion elements encourage us to continue this intervention and to pursue further research by studying the impact on the individuals' quality of life.


Asunto(s)
Síndrome de Asperger/terapia , Trastorno del Espectro Autista/terapia , Terapia Conductista/métodos , Comunicación , Psicoterapia de Grupo/métodos , Trastorno de la Conducta Social/terapia , Habilidades Sociales , Grabación de Cinta de Video , Adolescente , Síndrome de Asperger/diagnóstico , Síndrome de Asperger/psicología , Trastorno del Espectro Autista/diagnóstico , Trastorno del Espectro Autista/psicología , Niño , Estudios de Seguimiento , Humanos , Ajuste Social , Trastorno de la Conducta Social/diagnóstico , Trastorno de la Conducta Social/psicología , Medio Social
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA