Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 20 de 23
Filter
1.
J Vis ; 11(10)2011 Sep 22.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21940763

ABSTRACT

Previous studies have demonstrated an inverse relation between the size of the complete spatial summation area and ganglion cell density. We hypothesized that if this relation is dynamic, the spatial summation area at 6° nasal would expand to compensate for age-related losses of retinal ganglion cells but not in the fovea where age-related loss in ganglion cell density is not significant. This hypothesis was tested by measuring contrast thresholds with a series of Gabor patches varying in size. The spatial summation area was defined by the intersection of the segments of a two-branched, piece-wise linear function fitted to the data with slopes of -0.5 and 0 on a plot of log threshold vs. log area. Results demonstrate a 31% increase in the parafoveal spatial summation area in older observers with no significant age-related change in the fovea. The average foveal data show a significant increase in thresholds with age. Contrary to the foveal data, age comparisons of the parafoveal peak contrast thresholds display no significant difference above [corrected] the summation area. Nevertheless, as expected from the increase in summation area, expressing the parafoveal thresholds as contrast energy reveals a significant difference for stimuli that are smaller than the maximal summation area.


Subject(s)
Aging/physiology , Color Vision/physiology , Retinal Ganglion Cells/physiology , Space Perception/physiology , Adult , Aged , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Photic Stimulation , Sensory Thresholds , Young Adult
2.
Ophthalmic Physiol Opt ; 30(4): 339-50, 2010 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20629957

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: Our lab has previously demonstrated losses in contrast sensitivity to low spatial frequencies under scotopic conditions with older adults. It is not clear, however, whether the temporal frequency of a stimulus alters the relation between age and the spatial contrast sensitivity function (sCSF) under scotopic conditions. METHODS: A maximum-likelihood, two-alternative, temporal forced-choice QUEST procedure was used to measure threshold to spatially and temporally modulated stimuli in both young (mean = 26 years) and old (mean = 75 years) adults. RESULTS: In general, the shapes of the spatial and temporal CSFs were low-pass for both young and old observers; contrast sensitivity decreased at approximately the same rate with increasing spatial frequency and temporal frequency for both age groups, although the overall sensitivity of the old group was lower than that of the young group. The high-frequency resolution limit was lower for the old group compared to the young group. CONCLUSIONS: The differences in contrast sensitivity between the young and old groups suggest a uniform loss in sensitivity of the channels mediating spatial and temporal vision. Because of this loss, the spatial and temporal window of visibility for the older adults is compromised relative to the younger adults.


Subject(s)
Aging/physiology , Contrast Sensitivity/physiology , Night Vision/physiology , Sensory Thresholds/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Adult , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Female , Humans , Male , Photic Stimulation/methods , Young Adult
3.
J Vis ; 9(2): 24.1-16, 2009 Feb 27.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19271934

ABSTRACT

The contributions of optical and neural factors to age-related losses in spatial vision are not fully understood. We used closed-loop adaptive optics to test the visual benefit of correcting monochromatic high-order aberrations (HOAs) on spatial vision for observers ranging in age from 18 to 81 years. Contrast sensitivity was measured monocularly using a two-alternative forced-choice (2AFC) procedure for sinusoidal gratings over 6 mm and 3 mm pupil diameters. Visual acuity was measured using a spatial 4AFC procedure. Over a 6 mm pupil, young observers showed a large benefit of AO at high spatial frequencies, whereas older observers exhibited the greatest benefit at middle spatial frequencies, plus a significantly larger increase in visual acuity. When age-related miosis is controlled, young and old observers exhibited a similar benefit of AO for spatial vision. An increase in HOAs cannot account for the complete senescent decline in spatial vision. These results may indicate a larger role of additional optical factors when the impact of HOAs is removed, but also lend support for the importance of neural factors in age-related changes in spatial vision.


Subject(s)
Aging/psychology , Refraction, Ocular , Refractive Errors/physiopathology , Refractive Errors/psychology , Space Perception , Adolescent , Adult , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Contrast Sensitivity , Female , Humans , Male , Miosis , Visual Acuity , Young Adult
4.
J Vis ; 8(3): 5.1-9, 2008 Mar 10.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18484811

ABSTRACT

Accurately processing orientation information is a fundamental component of visual performance. Single-unit recordings have shown that the orientation tuning of individual neurons in macaque cortical areas V1 and V2 is reduced dramatically with age (M. T. Schmolesky, Y. Wang, M. Pu, & A. G. Leventhal, 2000; S. Yu, Y. Wang, X. Li, Y. Zhou, & A. G. Leventhal, 2006). These researchers suggest that losses in single-unit orientation selectivity result in declines in orientation discrimination and object recognition in older humans. Three experiments were conducted to determine whether human performance is affected by putative age-related changes in tuning of cortical neurons. Ten younger and ten older observers participated in this study. Experiment 1 demonstrated significant differences in the contrast sensitivity of the two age groups. Experiment 2 showed significant differences in orientation discrimination thresholds. However, when thresholds were plotted in terms of multiples of detection threshold, age-related differences were not observed. In Experiment 3, perceptual orientation tuning curves did not significantly differ in shape for younger and older subjects. As in Experiment 2, at any given contrast, there is a large difference in sensitivity between younger and older adults. This implies a model of orientation processing that allows the adult visual system to maintain consistent and reliable orientation information at the network and ultimately the perceptual level.


Subject(s)
Aging/physiology , Contrast Sensitivity/physiology , Orientation/physiology , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Adult , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Female , Humans , Male , Perceptual Masking , Photic Stimulation , Retina/physiology , Sensory Thresholds
6.
J Vis ; 7(6): 8, 2007 Apr 18.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17685791

ABSTRACT

Color appearance remains remarkably stable in the aging visual system despite large changes in the spectral distribution of the retinal stimulus and losses in chromatic sensitivity (P. B. Delahunt, J. L. Hardy, K. Okajima, & J. S. Werner, 2005; J. S. Werner, 1996). This stability could reflect adaptive adjustments in peripheral or central chromatic mechanisms that compensate for sensitivity losses in senescence. We asked whether similar compensatory adjustments play a role in maintaining spatial vision--and whether the adaptation itself shows changes with aging-by examining the effects of adaptation on judgments of image focus. Perceptual aftereffects following adaptation to a uniform field and blurred or sharpened images were compared between younger adults and older observers. Subjects adapted to a sequence of blurred or sharpened images for 120 s, and a two-alternative forced-choice staircase task was used to vary the filter exponent of the test to define the subjective point of best focus. There was a small but significant difference between younger and older observers in the level perceived as best focused in all three adaptation conditions, possibly reflecting differences in the ambient blur level the groups are routinely exposed to. However, the magnitude of the blur aftereffect did not differ between the two age groups. These results suggest that although there may be small differences in the long-term adaptation to blur, younger and older observers do not differ in the strength of adaptation to transient changes in blur. The neural processes mediating adaptation to blur thus appear to remain largely intact with aging.


Subject(s)
Accommodation, Ocular/physiology , Adaptation, Physiological , Aging/physiology , Space Perception/physiology , Adult , Afterimage , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Fixation, Ocular , Humans , Infant , Judgment , Male
7.
Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci ; 47(5): 2080-92, 2006 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16639019

ABSTRACT

PURPOSE: To relate in vivo microscopic retinal changes to visual function in patients who have various forms of retinal dystrophy. METHODS: The UC Davis Adaptive Optics (AO) fundus camera was used to acquire in vivo retinal images at the cellular level. Visual function tests consisting of visual fields, multifocal electroretinography (mfERG), and contrast sensitivity were measured in all subjects by using stimuli that were coincident with areas imaged. Five patients with different forms of retinal dystrophy and three control subjects were recruited. Cone densities were quantified for all retinal images. RESULTS: In all images of diseased retinas, there were extensive areas of dark space between groups of photoreceptors, where no cone photoreceptors were evident. These irregular features were not seen in healthy retinas, but were apparent in patients with retinal dystrophy. There were significant correlations between functional vision losses and the extent to which these irregularities, quantified by cone density, occurred in retinal images. CONCLUSIONS: AO fundus imaging is a reliable technique for assessing and quantifying the changes in the photoreceptor layer as disease progresses. Furthermore, this technique can be useful in cases where visual function tests provide borderline or ambiguous results, as it allows visualization of individual photoreceptors.


Subject(s)
Diagnostic Techniques, Ophthalmological , Photography/methods , Retinal Cone Photoreceptor Cells/pathology , Retinal Degeneration/diagnosis , Vision Disorders/diagnosis , Adolescent , Adult , Cell Count , Contrast Sensitivity/physiology , Electroretinography , Female , Humans , Male , Retinal Degeneration/physiopathology , Vision Disorders/physiopathology , Visual Fields/physiology
8.
J Vis ; 6(5): 625-33, 2006 May 04.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16881793

ABSTRACT

The watercolor effect (WCE) is a phenomenon of long-range color assimilation occurring when a dark chromatic contour delineating a figure is flanked on the inside by a brighter chromatic contour; the brighter color spreads into the entire enclosed area. Here, we determined the optimal chromatic parameters and the cone signals supporting the WCE. To that end, we quantified the effect of color assimilation using hue cancellation as a function of hue, colorimetric purity, and cone modulation of inducing contours. When the inner and outer contours had chromaticities that were in opposite directions in color space, a stronger WCE was obtained as compared with other color directions. Additionally, equal colorimetric purity between the outer and inner contours was necessary to obtain a large effect compared with conditions in which the contours differed in colorimetric purity. However, there was no further increase in the magnitude of the effect when the colorimetric purity increased beyond a value corresponding to an equal vector length between the inner and outer contours. Finally, L-M-cone-modulated WCE was perceptually stronger than S-cone-modulated WCE for our conditions. This last result demonstrates that both L-M-cone and S-cone pathways are important for watercolor spreading. Our data suggest that the WCE depends critically upon the particular spatiochromatic arrangement in the display, with the relative chromatic contrast between the inducing contours being particularly important.


Subject(s)
Color Perception/physiology , Form Perception/physiology , Optical Illusions/physiology , Adult , Color , Humans , Photic Stimulation/methods , Retinal Cone Photoreceptor Cells/physiology
9.
J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform ; 31(3): 420-37, 2005 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15982123

ABSTRACT

Viewing a distorted face induces large aftereffects in the appearance of an undistorted face. The authors examined the processes underlying this adaptation by comparing how selective the aftereffects are for different dimensions of the images including size, spatial frequency content, contrast, and color. Face aftereffects had weaker selectivity for changes in the size, contrast, or color of the images and stronger selectivity for changes in contrast polarity or spatial frequency. This pattern could arise if the adaptation is contingent on the perceived similarity of the stimuli as faces. Consistent with this, changing contrast polarity or spatial frequency had larger effects on the perceived identity of a face, and aftereffects were also selective for different individual faces. These results suggest that part of the sensitivity changes underlying the adaptation may arise at visual levels closely associated with the representation of faces.


Subject(s)
Adaptation, Psychological , Face , Figural Aftereffect , Perceptual Distortion , Analysis of Variance , Color Perception , Humans , Pattern Recognition, Visual , Recognition, Psychology
10.
Vision Res ; 45(11): 1413-24, 2005 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15743611

ABSTRACT

When a dark chromatic contour delineating a figure is flanked on the inside by a brighter chromatic contour, the brighter color will spread into the entire enclosed area. This is known as the watercolor effect (WCE). Here we quantified the effect of color spreading using both color-matching and hue-cancellation tasks. Over a wide range of stimulus chromaticities, there was a reliable shift in color appearance that closely followed the direction of the inducing contour. When the contours were equated in luminance, the WCE was still present, but weak. The magnitude of the color spreading increased with increases in luminance contrast between the two contours. Additionally, as the luminance contrast between the contours increased, the chromaticity of the induced color more closely resembled that of the inside contour. The results support the hypothesis that the WCE is mediated by luminance-dependent mechanisms of long-range color assimilation.


Subject(s)
Color Perception/physiology , Distance Perception/physiology , Optical Illusions , Photic Stimulation , Adult , Female , Humans , Lighting , Male , Psychophysics
11.
Psychon Bull Rev ; 22(5): 1308-19, 2015 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25711183

ABSTRACT

The notion that human learning follows a smooth power law (PL) of diminishing gains is well-established in psychology. This characteristic is observed when multiple curves are averaged, potentially masking more complex dynamics underpinning the curves of individual learners. Here, we analyzed 25,280 individual learning curves, each comprising 500 measurements of cognitive performance taken from four cognitive tasks. A piecewise PL (PPL) model explained the individual learning curves significantly better than a single PL, controlling for model complexity. The PPL model allows for multiple PLs connected at different points in the learning process. We also explored the transition dynamics between PL curve component pieces. Performance in later pieces typically surpassed that in earlier pieces, after a brief drop in performance at the transition point. The transition rate was negatively associated with age, even after controlling for overall performance. Our results suggest at least two processes at work in individual learning curves: locally, a gradual, smooth improvement, with diminishing gains within a specific strategy, which is modeled well as a PL; and globally, a discrete sequence of strategy shifts, in which each strategy is better in the long term than the ones preceding it. The piecewise extension of the classic PL of practice has implications for both individual skill acquisition and theories of learning.


Subject(s)
Cognition , Individuality , Learning Curve , Power, Psychological , Practice, Psychological , Adolescent , Adult , Age Factors , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Choice Behavior , Color Perception , Discrimination, Psychological , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Models, Psychological , Pattern Recognition, Visual , Semantics , Verbal Learning , Young Adult
12.
Front Psychol ; 6: 1652, 2015.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26579035

ABSTRACT

The NeuroCognitive Performance Test (NCPT) is a brief, repeatable, web-based cognitive assessment platform that measures performance across several cognitive domains. The NCPT platform is modular and includes 18 subtests that can be arranged into customized batteries. Here we present normative data from a sample of 130,140 healthy volunteers for an NCPT battery consisting of 8 subtests. Participants took the NCPT remotely and without supervision. Factor structure and effects of age, education, and gender were evaluated with this normative dataset. Test-retest reliability was evaluated in a subset of participants who took the battery again an average of 78.8 days later. The eight NCPT subtests group into 4 putative cognitive domains, have adequate to good test-retest reliability, and are sensitive to expected age- and education-related cognitive effects. Concurrent validity to standard neuropsychological tests was demonstrated in 73 healthy volunteers. In an exploratory analysis the NCPT battery could differentiate those who self-reported Mild Cognitive Impairment or Alzheimer's disease from matched healthy controls. Overall these results demonstrate the reliability and validity of the NCPT battery as a measure of cognitive performance and support the feasibility of web-based, unsupervised testing, with potential utility in clinical and research settings.

13.
Alzheimers Dement (Amst) ; 1(2): 260-7, 2015 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27239508

ABSTRACT

INTRODUCTION: Lumosity's Memory Match (LMM) is an online game requiring visual working memory. Change in LMM scores may be associated with individual differences in age-related changes in working memory. METHODS: Effects of age and time on LMM learning and forgetting rates were estimated using data from 1890 game sessions for users aged 40 to 79 years. RESULTS: There were significant effects of age on baseline LMM scores (ß = -.31, standard error or SE = .02, P < .0001) and lower learning rates (ß = -.0066, SE = .0008, P < .0001). A sample size of 202 subjects/arm was estimated for a 1-year study for subjects in the lower quartile of game performance. DISCUSSION: Online memory games have the potential to identify age-related decline in cognition and to identify subjects at risk for cognitive decline with smaller sample sizes and lower cost than traditional recruitment methods.

14.
PLoS One ; 10(9): e0134467, 2015.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26333022

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: A variety of studies have demonstrated gains in cognitive ability following cognitive training interventions. However, other studies have not shown such gains, and questions remain regarding the efficacy of specific cognitive training interventions. Cognitive training research often involves programs made up of just one or a few exercises, targeting limited and specific cognitive endpoints. In addition, cognitive training studies typically involve small samples that may be insufficient for reliable measurement of change. Other studies have utilized training periods that were too short to generate reliable gains in cognitive performance. METHODS: The present study evaluated an online cognitive training program comprised of 49 exercises targeting a variety of cognitive capacities. The cognitive training program was compared to an active control condition in which participants completed crossword puzzles. All participants were recruited, trained, and tested online (N = 4,715 fully evaluable participants). Participants in both groups were instructed to complete one approximately 15-minute session at least 5 days per week for 10 weeks. RESULTS: Participants randomly assigned to the treatment group improved significantly more on the primary outcome measure, an aggregate measure of neuropsychological performance, than did the active control group (Cohen's d effect size = 0.255; 95% confidence interval = [0.198, 0.312]). Treatment participants showed greater improvements than controls on speed of processing, short-term memory, working memory, problem solving, and fluid reasoning assessments. Participants in the treatment group also showed greater improvements on self-reported measures of cognitive functioning, particularly on those items related to concentration compared to the control group (Cohen's d = 0.249; 95% confidence interval = [0.191, 0.306]). CONCLUSION: Taken together, these results indicate that a varied training program composed of a number of tasks targeted to different cognitive functions can show transfer to a wide range of untrained measures of cognitive performance. TRIAL REGISTRATION: ClinicalTrials.gov NCT-02367898.


Subject(s)
Attention/physiology , Cognition/physiology , Memory, Short-Term/physiology , Practice, Psychological , Problem Solving/physiology , Adolescent , Adult , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Female , Humans , Internet , Male , Middle Aged , Neuropsychological Tests , Young Adult
15.
Vision Res ; 43(10): 1159-75, 2003 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12705956

ABSTRACT

We examined the role of color in the processing of motion of a luminance-varying pattern by alternating the color of a moving pattern and measuring the luminance contrast required for accurate discrimination of the motion direction. We report that the contrast threshold for perceiving the direction of motion of luminance-varying patterns is greatly elevated when the mean chromaticity of the moving luminance pattern alternates between two hues. Thus, color plays a critical role in the discrimination of luminance motion direction. The magnitude of the threshold elevation is directly related to the magnitude of the LM opponent color contrast produced by the color alternation. S-cone contrast produces little or no effect. The interference produced by color alternation was greatly reduced in the retinal periphery. Our results indicate that first-order luminance motion mechanisms are sensitive to the color of moving objects as coded by a differencing of the outputs of L and M cones. Contrary to the widely accepted notion that luminance-defined motion is processed primarily in the spectrally broadband magnocellular (M) pathway, our results suggest that the hue-selective parvocellular (P) mechanisms provide input to first-order motion detectors.


Subject(s)
Color Perception/physiology , Contrast Sensitivity/physiology , Motion Perception/physiology , Humans , Lighting , Psychophysics
16.
Opt Rev ; 11(4): 226-234, 2004 Jul 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19844600

ABSTRACT

The human visual system undergoes continuous anatomical, physiological and functional changes throughout the life span. There is also continuous change in the spectral distribution and intensity of light reaching the retina from infancy through senescence, primarily due to changes in the absorption of short-wave light by the lens. Despite these changes in the retinal stimulus and the signals leaving the retina for perceptual analysis, color appearance is relatively stable during aging as measured by broadband reflective or self-luminous samples, the wavelengths of unique blue and yellow, and the achromatic locus. Measures of ocular media density for younger and older observers show, indeed, that color appearance is independent of ocular media density. This may be explained by a renormalization process that was demonstrated by measuring the chromaticity of the achromatic point before and after cataract surgery. There was a shift following cataract surgery (removal of a brunescent lens) that was initially toward yellow in color space, but over the course of months, drifted back in the direction of the achromatic point before surgery. The spatial characteristics of color mechanisms were quantified for younger and older observers in terms of chromatic perceptive fields and the chromatic contrast sensitivity functions. Younger and older observers differed with small spots or with chromatic spatial gratings near threshold, but there were no significant differences with larger spots or suprathreshold spatial gratings.

17.
Front Hum Neurosci ; 7: 292, 2013.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23801955

ABSTRACT

Making new breakthroughs in understanding the processes underlying human cognition may depend on the availability of very large datasets that have not historically existed in psychology and neuroscience. Lumosity is a web-based cognitive training platform that has grown to include over 600 million cognitive training task results from over 35 million individuals, comprising the largest existing dataset of human cognitive performance. As part of the Human Cognition Project, Lumosity's collaborative research program to understand the human mind, Lumos Labs researchers and external research collaborators have begun to explore this dataset in order uncover novel insights about the correlates of cognitive performance. This paper presents two preliminary demonstrations of some of the kinds of questions that can be examined with the dataset. The first example focuses on replicating known findings relating lifestyle factors to baseline cognitive performance in a demographically diverse, healthy population at a much larger scale than has previously been available. The second example examines a question that would likely be very difficult to study in laboratory-based and existing online experimental research approaches at a large scale: specifically, how learning ability for different types of cognitive tasks changes with age. We hope that these examples will provoke the imagination of researchers who are interested in collaborating to answer fundamental questions about human cognitive performance.

18.
PLoS One ; 5(7): e11537, 2010 Jul 14.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20644719

ABSTRACT

Normal aging is associated with a degradation of perceptual abilities and a decline in higher-level cognitive functions, notably working memory. To remediate age-related deficits, cognitive training programs are increasingly being developed. However, it is not yet definitively established if, and by what mechanisms, training ameliorates effects of cognitive aging. Furthermore, a major factor impeding the success of training programs is a frequent failure of training to transfer benefits to untrained abilities. Here, we offer the first evidence of direct transfer-of-benefits from perceptual discrimination training to working memory performance in older adults. Moreover, using electroencephalography to evaluate participants before and after training, we reveal neural evidence of functional plasticity in older adult brains, such that training-induced modifications in early visual processing during stimulus encoding predict working memory accuracy improvements. These findings demonstrate the strength of the perceptual discrimination training approach by offering clear psychophysical evidence of transfer-of-benefit and a neural mechanism underlying cognitive improvement.


Subject(s)
Aging/physiology , Memory, Short-Term/physiology , Aged , Cognition/physiology , Electroencephalography , Female , Humans , Male , Neuropsychological Tests
19.
Perception ; 35(4): 461-8, 2006.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16700289

ABSTRACT

Color assimilation with bichromatic contours was quantified for spatial extents ranging from von Bezold-type color assimilation to the watercolor effect. The magnitude and direction of assimilative hue change was measured as a function of the width of a rectangular stimulus. Assimilation was quantified by hue cancellation. Large hue shifts were required to null the color of stimuli < or = 9.3 min of arc in width, with an exponential decrease for stimuli increasing up to 7.4 deg. When stimuli were viewed through an achromatizing lens, the magnitude of the assimilation effect was reduced for narrow stimuli, but not for wide ones. These results demonstrate that chromatic aberration may account, in part, for color assimilation over small, but not large, surface areas.


Subject(s)
Color Perception , Contrast Sensitivity , Dark Adaptation , Humans , Light , Models, Psychological , Photic Stimulation/methods , Psychological Tests , Psychophysics , Vision, Binocular , Vision, Monocular
20.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 103(33): 12523-8, 2006 Aug 15.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16888038

ABSTRACT

Normal aging is associated with progressive functional losses in perception, cognition, and memory. Although the root causes of age-related cognitive decline are incompletely understood, psychophysical and neuropsychological evidence suggests that a significant contribution stems from poorer signal-to-noise conditions and down-regulated neuromodulatory system function in older brains. Because the brain retains a lifelong capacity for plasticity and adaptive reorganization, dimensions of negative reorganization should be at least partially reversible through the use of an appropriately designed training program. We report here results from such a training program targeting age-related cognitive decline. Data from a randomized, controlled trial using standardized measures of neuropsychological function as outcomes are presented. Significant improvements in assessments directly related to the training tasks and significant generalization of improvements to nonrelated standardized neuropsychological measures of memory (effect size of 0.25) were documented in the group using the training program. Memory enhancement appeared to be sustained after a 3-month no-contact follow-up period. Matched active control and no-contact control groups showed no significant change in memory function after training or at the 3-month follow-up. This study demonstrates that intensive, plasticity-engaging training can result in an enhancement of cognitive function in normal mature adults.


Subject(s)
Aging/physiology , Cognition/physiology , Education , Memory/physiology , Neuronal Plasticity/physiology , Adult , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Cognition Disorders/prevention & control , Humans , Middle Aged , Neuropsychological Tests , Research Design
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL