Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 5 de 5
Filtrar
1.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 110(38): 15225-30, 2013 Sep 17.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24003112

RESUMEN

After hearing a tone, the human auditory system becomes more sensitive to similar tones than to other tones. Current auditory models explain this phenomenon by a simple bandpass attention filter. Here, we demonstrate that auditory attention involves multiple pass-bands around octave-related frequencies above and below the cued tone. Intriguingly, this "octave effect" not only occurs for physically presented tones, but even persists for the missing fundamental in complex tones, and for imagined tones. Our results suggest neural interactions combining octave-related frequencies, likely located in nonprimary cortical regions. We speculate that this connectivity scheme evolved from exposure to natural vibrations containing octave-related spectral peaks, e.g., as produced by vocal cords.


Asunto(s)
Atención/fisiología , Percepción Auditiva/fisiología , Audición/fisiología , Modelos Biológicos , Psicoacústica , Estimulación Acústica , Humanos
2.
J Biol Rhythms ; 33(4): 420-431, 2018 08.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29984614

RESUMEN

Studies with monochromatic light stimuli have shown that the action spectrum for melatonin suppression exhibits its highest sensitivity at short wavelengths, around 460 to 480 nm. Other studies have demonstrated that filtering out the short wavelengths from white light reduces melatonin suppression. However, this filtering of short wavelengths was generally confounded with reduced light intensity and/or changes in color temperature. Moreover, it changed the appearance from white light to yellow/orange, rendering it unusable for many practical applications. Here, we show that selectively tuning a polychromatic white light spectrum, compensating for the reduction in spectral power between 450 and 500 nm by enhancing power at even shorter wavelengths, can produce greatly different effects on melatonin production, without changes in illuminance or color temperature. On different evenings, 15 participants were exposed to 3 h of white light with either low or high power between 450 and 500 nm, and the effects on salivary melatonin levels and alertness were compared with those during a dim light baseline. Exposure to the spectrum with low power between 450 and 500 nm, but high power at even shorter wavelengths, did not suppress melatonin compared with dim light, despite a large difference in illuminance (175 vs. <5 lux). In contrast, exposure to the spectrum with high power between 450 and 500 nm (also 175 lux) resulted in almost 50% melatonin suppression. For alertness, no significant differences between the 3 conditions were observed. These results open up new opportunities for lighting applications that allow for the use of electrical lighting without disturbance of melatonin production.


Asunto(s)
Color , Iluminación/métodos , Melatonina/biosíntesis , Melatonina/efectos de la radiación , Temperatura , Adulto , Ritmo Circadiano/efectos de la radiación , Femenino , Humanos , Luz/efectos adversos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Estimulación Luminosa , Saliva/química , Vigilia , Adulto Joven
4.
Vision Res ; 47(26): 3307-14, 2007 Dec.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18028979

RESUMEN

When determining an object's orientation an implicit object axis is formed, based on local contour information. Due to the oblique effect (i.e., the more precise perception of horizontal/vertical orientations than oblique orientations), an object's orientation will be perceived more precise if the axis is either horizontal or vertical than when the axis is oblique. In this study we investigated which object axis is used to determine orientation for objects containing multiple axes. We tested human subjects in a series of experiments using the method of adjustment. We found that observers always use object axes allowing for the highest object orientation discrimination, namely the axes lying closest to the horizontal/vertical. This implies that the weight the visual system attaches to axial object information is in accordance with the precision with which this information is perceived.


Asunto(s)
Señales (Psicología) , Discriminación en Psicología , Percepción de Forma/fisiología , Adulto , Anisotropía , Humanos , Orientación , Psicofísica , Movimientos Sacádicos
5.
Perception ; 39(7): 909-17, 2010.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20842968

RESUMEN

The oblique effect is a decreased sensitivity for oblique orientations compared with horizontal/vertical orientations. We investigated whether orientation-matching acuity for binocularly presented stimuli is dependent on orientations of retinal projections or on the perceived orientation of the stimulus. Using a stereoscope, we constructed a stimulus, such that retinal orientations were oblique, while the perceived orientation was vertical. We then used this stimulus to test observers in an orientation-matching task. If orientation matching depends on the orientations of retinal projections, low matching performance would be expected, since retinal projections are oblique. However, if orientation matching depends on perceived orientation (vertical), high matching performance would be expected. We found that observers' performance can only be predicted from the perceived orientation, not the orientations of retinal projections, implying that in binocular viewing the oblique effect occurs after binocular fusion.


Asunto(s)
Ilusiones Ópticas/fisiología , Orientación/fisiología , Visión Binocular/fisiología , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Adulto , Humanos
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA