Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 83
Filtrar
Más filtros

Banco de datos
Tipo del documento
Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
Twin Res Hum Genet ; 21(3): 179-190, 2018 06.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29757125

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Considerable evidence from twin and adoption studies indicates that genetic and shared environmental factors play a role in the initiation of smoking behavior. Although twin and adoption designs are powerful to detect genetic and environmental influences, they do not provide information on the processes of assortative mating and parent-offspring transmission and their contribution to the variability explained by genetic and/or environmental factors. METHODS: We examined the role of genetic and environmental factors in individual differences for smoking initiation (SI) using an extended kinship design. This design allows the simultaneous testing of additive and non-additive genetic, shared and individual-specific environmental factors, as well as sex differences in the expression of genes and environment in the presence of assortative mating and combined genetic and cultural transmission, while also estimating the regression of the prevalence of SI on age. A dichotomous lifetime 'ever' smoking measure was obtained from twins and relatives in the 'Virginia 30,000' sample and the 'Australian 25,000'. RESULTS: Results demonstrate that both genetic and environmental factors play a significant role in the liability to SI. Major influences on individual differences appeared to be additive genetic and unique environmental effects, with smaller contributions from assortative mating, shared sibling environment, twin environment, cultural transmission, and resulting genotype-environment covariance. Age regression of the prevalence of SI was significant. The finding of negative cultural transmission without dominance led us to investigate more closely two possible mechanisms for the lower parent-offspring correlations compared to the sibling and DZ twin correlations in subsets of the data: (1) age × gene interaction, and (2) social homogamy. Neither of the mechanism provided a significantly better explanation of the data. CONCLUSIONS: This study showed significant heritability, partly due to assortment, and significant effects of primarily non-parental shared environment on liability to SI.


Asunto(s)
Cultura , Modelos Biológicos , Fumar/genética , Adulto , Factores de Edad , Australia/epidemiología , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Fumar/epidemiología , Estados Unidos/epidemiología
2.
Twin Res Hum Genet ; 21(3): 163-178, 2018 06.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29692273

RESUMEN

Drinking alcohol is a normal behavior in many societies, and prior studies have demonstrated it has both genetic and environmental sources of variation. Using two very large samples of twins and their first-degree relatives (Australia ≈ 20,000 individuals from 8,019 families; Virginia ≈ 23,000 from 6,042 families), we examine whether there are differences: (1) in the genetic and environmental factors that influence four interrelated drinking behaviors (quantity, frequency, age of initiation, and number of drinks in the last week), (2) between the twin-only design and the extended twin design, and (3) the Australian and Virginia samples. We find that while drinking behaviors are interrelated, there are substantial differences in the genetic and environmental architectures across phenotypes. Specifically, drinking quantity, frequency, and number of drinks in the past week have large broad genetic variance components, and smaller but significant environmental variance components, while age of onset is driven exclusively by environmental factors. Further, the twin-only design and the extended twin design come to similar conclusions regarding broad-sense heritability and environmental transmission, but the extended twin models provide a more nuanced perspective. Finally, we find a high level of similarity between the Australian and Virginian samples, especially for the genetic factors. The observed differences, when present, tend to be at the environmental level. Implications for the extended twin model and future directions are discussed.


Asunto(s)
Consumo de Bebidas Alcohólicas , Modelos Biológicos , Gemelos/genética , Adulto , Edad de Inicio , Consumo de Bebidas Alcohólicas/epidemiología , Consumo de Bebidas Alcohólicas/genética , Australia/epidemiología , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Virginia/epidemiología
3.
Nicotine Tob Res ; 19(4): 401-409, 2017 Apr 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27807125

RESUMEN

INTRODUCTION: Previous studies in adolescents were not adequately powered to accurately disentangle genetic and environmental influences on smoking initiation (SI) across adolescence. METHODS: Mega-analysis of pooled genetically informative data on SI was performed, with structural equation modeling, to test equality of prevalence and correlations across cultural backgrounds, and to estimate the significance and effect size of genetic and environmental effects according to the classical twin study, in adolescent male and female twins from same-sex and opposite-sex twin pairs (N = 19 313 pairs) between ages 10 and 19, with 76 358 longitudinal assessments between 1983 and 2007, from 11 population-based twin samples from the United States, Europe, and Australia. RESULTS: Although prevalences differed between samples, twin correlations did not, suggesting similar etiology of SI across developed countries. The estimate of additive genetic contributions to liability of SI increased from approximately 15% to 45% from ages 13 to 19. Correspondingly, shared environmental factors accounted for a substantial proportion of variance in liability to SI at age 13 (70%) and gradually less by age 19 (40%). CONCLUSIONS: Both additive genetic and shared environmental factors significantly contribute to variance in SI throughout adolescence. The present study, the largest genetic epidemiological study on SI to date, found consistent results across 11 studies for the etiology of SI. Environmental factors, especially those shared by siblings in a family, primarily influence SI variance in early adolescence, while an increasing role of genetic factors is seen at later ages, which has important implications for prevention strategies. IMPLICATIONS: This is the first study to find evidence of genetic factors in liability to SI at ages as young as 12. It also shows the strongest evidence to date for decay of effects of the shared environment from early adolescence to young adulthood. We found remarkable consistency of twin correlations across studies reflecting similar etiology of liability to initiate smoking across different cultures and time periods. Thus familial factors strongly contribute to individual differences in who starts to smoke with a gradual increase in the impact of genetic factors and a corresponding decrease in that of the shared environment.


Asunto(s)
Fumar/epidemiología , Fumar/genética , Gemelos/genética , Gemelos/estadística & datos numéricos , Adolescente , Adulto , Australia/epidemiología , Niño , Europa (Continente)/epidemiología , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Estudios en Gemelos como Asunto , Estados Unidos/epidemiología , Adulto Joven
4.
Twin Res Hum Genet ; 20(5): 371-373, 2017 10.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28975877

RESUMEN

The genetic and social causes of individual differences in attitudes to gun control are estimated in a sample of senior male and female twin pairs in the United States. Genetic and environmental parameters were estimated by weighted least squares applied to polychoric correlations for monozygotic (MZ) and dizygotic (DZ) twins of both sexes. The analysis suggests twin similarity for attitudes to gun control in men is entirely genetic while that in women is purely social. Although the volunteer sample is small, the analysis illustrates how the well-tested concepts and methods of genetic epidemiology may be a fertile resource for deepening our scientific understanding of biological and social pathways that affect individual risk to gun violence.


Asunto(s)
Actitud , Armas de Fuego , Caracteres Sexuales , Conducta Social , Gemelos Dicigóticos , Gemelos Monocigóticos , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino
5.
Behav Genet ; 45(4): 382-93, 2015 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25894924

RESUMEN

We tested two models to identify the genetic and environmental processes underlying longitudinal changes in depression among adolescents. The first assumes that observed changes in covariance structure result from the unfolding of inherent, random individual differences in the overall levels and rates of change in depression over time (random growth curves). The second assumes that observed changes are due to time-specific random effects (innovations) accumulating over time (autoregressive effects). We found little evidence of age-specific genetic effects or persistent genetic innovations. Instead, genetic effects are consistent with a gradual unfolding in the liability to depression and rates of change with increasing age. Likewise, the environment also creates significant individual differences in overall levels of depression and rates of change. However, there are also time-specific environmental experiences that persist with fidelity. The implications of these differing genetic and environmental mechanisms in the etiology of depression are considered.


Asunto(s)
Depresión/genética , Ambiente , Predisposición Genética a la Enfermedad , Adolescente , Factores de Edad , Niño , Enfermedades en Gemelos , Femenino , Genotipo , Humanos , Individualidad , Funciones de Verosimilitud , Masculino , Modelos Estadísticos , Análisis Multivariante , Análisis de Regresión , Medio Social , Factores de Tiempo , Gemelos Dicigóticos/genética , Gemelos Monocigóticos/genética
6.
Behav Genet ; 45(4): 461-6, 2015 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25894926

RESUMEN

This study explores power assumptions relating to extended pedigree designs (EPD) and classical twin designs (CTD). We conducted statistical analyses to compare the power of the two designs for examining neuroimaging phenotypes, varying heritability and varying whether shared environmental variance is fixed or free. Results indicated that CTDs have more power to estimate heritability, with the exception of one condition: in EPDs, the power increases relative to CTDs when shared environmental variance contributes to sibling similarity only. We additionally show that assuming a priori that shared environmental effects play no role in a phenotype-as is commonly done in pedigree designs-can lead to substantially biased heritability estimates. General results indicate that both CTDs and EPDs obtain quite precise heritability estimates. Finally, we discuss methodological considerations relating to assumptions about age effects and shared environment.


Asunto(s)
Predisposición Genética a la Enfermedad , Estudios en Gemelos como Asunto , Simulación por Computador , Ambiente , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Modelos Estadísticos , Linaje , Fenotipo , Carácter Cuantitativo Heredable , Proyectos de Investigación , Hermanos , Gemelos Dicigóticos/genética , Gemelos Monocigóticos/genética
7.
Twin Res Hum Genet ; 18(2): 171-8, 2015 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25728588

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVE: Despite an increasing recognition that psychiatric disorders can be diagnosed as early as preschool, little is known how early genetic and environmental risk factors contribute to the development of psychiatric disorders during this very early period of development. METHOD: We assessed infant temperament at age 1, and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), oppositional defiant disorder (ODD), and separation anxiety disorder (SAD) at ages 3 through 5 years in a sample of Hispanic twins. Genetic, shared, and non-shared environmental effects were estimated for each temperamental construct and psychiatric disorder using the statistical program MX. Multivariate genetic models were fitted to determine whether the same or different sets of genes and environments account for the co-occurrence between early temperament and preschool psychiatric disorders. RESULTS: Additive genetic factors accounted for 61% of the variance in ADHD, 21% in ODD, and 28% in SAD. Shared environmental factors accounted for 34% of the variance in ODD and 15% of SAD. The genetic influence on difficult temperament was significantly associated with preschool ADHD, SAD, and ODD. The association between ODD and SAD was due to both genetic and family environmental factors. The temperamental trait of resistance to control was entirely accounted for by the shared family environment. CONCLUSIONS: There are different genetic and family environmental pathways between infant temperament and psychiatric diagnoses in this sample of Puerto Rican preschool age children.


Asunto(s)
Ansiedad de Separación , Trastorno por Déficit de Atención con Hiperactividad , Déficit de la Atención y Trastornos de Conducta Disruptiva , Hispánicos o Latinos/genética , Temperamento , Gemelos/genética , Ansiedad de Separación/genética , Ansiedad de Separación/fisiopatología , Ansiedad de Separación/psicología , Trastorno por Déficit de Atención con Hiperactividad/genética , Trastorno por Déficit de Atención con Hiperactividad/fisiopatología , Trastorno por Déficit de Atención con Hiperactividad/psicología , Déficit de la Atención y Trastornos de Conducta Disruptiva/genética , Déficit de la Atención y Trastornos de Conducta Disruptiva/fisiopatología , Déficit de la Atención y Trastornos de Conducta Disruptiva/psicología , Preescolar , Femenino , Interacción Gen-Ambiente , Humanos , Lactante , Masculino , Carácter Cuantitativo Heredable
8.
Twin Res Hum Genet ; 18(1): 43-51, 2015 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25662421

RESUMEN

Little is known regarding the underlying relationship between smoking initiation and current quantity smoked during adolescence into young adulthood. It is possible that the influences of genetic and environmental factors on this relationship vary across sex and age. To investigate this further, the current study applied a common causal contingency model to data from a Virginia-based twin study to determine: (1) if the same genetic and environmental factors are contributing to smoking initiation and current quantity smoked; (2) whether the magnitude of genetic and environmental factor contributions are the same across adolescence and young adulthood; and (3) if qualitative and quantitative differences in the sources of variance between males and females exist. Study results found no qualitative or quantitative sex differences in the relationship between smoking initiation and current quantity smoked, though relative contributions of genetic and environmental factors changed across adolescence and young adulthood. More specifically, smoking initiation and current quantity smoked remain separate constructs until young adulthood, when liabilities are correlated. Smoking initiation is explained by genetic, shared, and unique environmental factors in early adolescence and by genetic and unique environmental factors in young adulthood; while current quantity smoked is explained by shared environmental and unique environmental factors until young adulthood, when genetic and unique environmental factors play a larger role.


Asunto(s)
Conducta del Adolescente , Desarrollo del Adolescente , Enfermedades en Gemelos/genética , Interacción Gen-Ambiente , Fumar/genética , Trastornos Relacionados con Sustancias/genética , Gemelos Dicigóticos/genética , Gemelos Monocigóticos/genética , Adolescente , Adulto , Niño , Enfermedades en Gemelos/epidemiología , Enfermedades en Gemelos/psicología , Femenino , Estudios de Seguimiento , Humanos , Masculino , Prevalencia , Estudios Prospectivos , Factores Sexuales , Fumar/epidemiología , Fumar/psicología , Medio Social , Trastornos Relacionados con Sustancias/epidemiología , Trastornos Relacionados con Sustancias/psicología , Tabaquismo/epidemiología , Tabaquismo/genética , Tabaquismo/psicología , Gemelos Dicigóticos/psicología , Gemelos Monocigóticos/psicología , Virginia/epidemiología , Adulto Joven
9.
Behav Genet ; 44(4): 337-46, 2014 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24647834

RESUMEN

Twin and family studies implicitly assume that the covariation between family members remains constant across differences in age between the members of the family. However, age-specificity in gene expression for shared environmental factors could generate higher correlations between family members who are more similar in age. Cohort effects (cohort × genotype or cohort × common environment) could have the same effects, and both potentially reduce effect sizes estimated in genome-wide association studies where the subjects are heterogeneous in age. In this paper we describe a model in which the covariance between twins and non-twin siblings is moderated as a function of age difference. We describe the details of the model and simulate data using a variety of different parameter values to demonstrate that model fitting returns unbiased parameter estimates. Power analyses are then conducted to estimate the sample sizes required to detect the effects of moderation in a design of twins and siblings. Finally, the model is applied to data on cigarette smoking. We find that (1) the model effectively recovers the simulated parameters, (2) the power is relatively low and therefore requires large sample sizes before small to moderate effect sizes can be found reliably, and (3) the genetic covariance between siblings for smoking behavior decays very rapidly. Result 3 implies that, e.g., genome-wide studies of smoking behavior that use individuals assessed at different ages, or belonging to different birth-year cohorts may have had substantially reduced power to detect effects of genotype on cigarette use. It also implies that significant special twin environmental effects can be explained by age-moderation in some cases. This effect likely contributes to the missing heritability paradox.


Asunto(s)
Modelos Genéticos , Hermanos , Fumar/genética , Factores de Edad , Familia , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Modelos Estadísticos , Gemelos/genética , Gemelos Dicigóticos/genética , Gemelos Monocigóticos/genética
10.
Behav Genet ; 44(5): 445-55, 2014 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25060210

RESUMEN

Genome wide complex trait analysis (GCTA) is extended to include environmental effects of the maternal genotype on offspring phenotype ("maternal effects", M-GCTA). The model includes parameters for the direct effects of the offspring genotype, maternal effects and the covariance between direct and maternal effects. Analysis of simulated data, conducted in OpenMx, confirmed that model parameters could be recovered by full information maximum likelihood (FIML) and evaluated the biases that arise in conventional GCTA when indirect genetic effects are ignored. Estimates derived from FIML in OpenMx showed very close agreement to those obtained by restricted maximum likelihood using the published algorithm for GCTA. The method was also applied to illustrative perinatal phenotypes from ~4,000 mother-offspring pairs from the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children. The relative merits of extended GCTA in contrast to quantitative genetic approaches based on analyzing the phenotypic covariance structure of kinships are considered.


Asunto(s)
Estudio de Asociación del Genoma Completo , Modelos Genéticos , Sitios de Carácter Cuantitativo , Genotipo , Fenotipo , Carácter Cuantitativo Heredable
11.
Behav Genet ; 44(3): 282-94, 2014 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24569950

RESUMEN

Almost 40 years ago, evidence from large studies of adult twins and their relatives suggested that between 30 and 60% of the variance in social and political attitudes could be explained by genetic influences. However, these findings have not been widely accepted or incorporated into the dominant paradigms that explain the etiology of political ideology. This has been attributed in part to measurement and sample limitations, as well the relative absence of molecular genetic studies. Here we present results from original analyses of a combined sample of over 12,000 twins pairs, ascertained from nine different studies conducted in five democracies, sampled over the course of four decades. We provide evidence that genetic factors play a role in the formation of political ideology, regardless of how ideology is measured, the era, or the population sampled. The only exception is a question that explicitly uses the phrase "Left-Right". We then present results from one of the first genome-wide association studies on political ideology using data from three samples: a 1990 Australian sample involving 6,894 individuals from 3,516 families; a 2008 Australian sample of 1,160 related individuals from 635 families and a 2010 Swedish sample involving 3,334 individuals from 2,607 families. No polymorphisms reached genome-wide significance in the meta-analysis. The combined evidence suggests that political ideology constitutes a fundamental aspect of one's genetically informed psychological disposition, but as Fisher proposed long ago, genetic influences on complex traits will be composed of thousands of markers of very small effects and it will require extremely large samples to have enough power in order to identify specific polymorphisms related to complex social traits.


Asunto(s)
Personalidad/genética , Política , Estudio de Asociación del Genoma Completo , Humanos
12.
Am J Obstet Gynecol ; 210(5): 398-405, 2014 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24096276

RESUMEN

This review describes how improvements in biometric-genetic studies of twin kinships, half-sibships, and cousinships have now demonstrated a sizeable fetal genetic and maternal genetic contribution to the spontaneous onset of labor. This is an important development because previous literature for the most part reports only an influence of the maternal genome. Current estimates of the percent of variation that is attributable to fetal genetic factors range from 11-35%; the range for the maternal genetic contribution is 13-20%. These same studies demonstrate an even larger influence of environmental sources over and above the influence of genetic sources and previously identified environmental risk factors. With these estimates in hand, a major goal for research on pregnancy duration is to identify specific allelic variation and environmental risk to account for this estimated genetic and environmental variation. A review of the current literature can serve as a guide for future research efforts.


Asunto(s)
Edad Gestacional , Inicio del Trabajo de Parto/fisiología , Nacimiento Prematuro/epidemiología , Nacimiento Prematuro/fisiopatología , Ambiente , Femenino , Estudio de Asociación del Genoma Completo , Humanos , Inicio del Trabajo de Parto/genética , Epidemiología Molecular , Embarazo , Resultado del Embarazo/epidemiología , Resultado del Embarazo/genética , Nacimiento Prematuro/genética , Factores de Riesgo
13.
Appetite ; 77: 131-8, 2014 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24631637

RESUMEN

The heritability of variety seeking in the food domain was estimated from a large sample (N = 5,543) of middle age to elderly monozygotic and dizygotic twins from the "Virginia 30,000" twin study. Different dietary variety scores were calculated based on a semi-quantitative food choice questionnaire that assessed consumption frequencies and quantities for a list of 99 common foods. Results indicate that up to 30% of the observed variance in dietary variety was explained through heritable influences. Most of the differences between twins were due to environmental influences that are not shared between twins. Additional non-genetic analyses further revealed a weak relationship between dietary variety and particular demographic variables, including socioeconomic status, age, sex, religious faith, and the number of people living in the same household.


Asunto(s)
Dieta , Ambiente , Conducta Alimentaria , Gemelos Dicigóticos , Gemelos Monocigóticos , Adulto , Anciano , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Virginia
14.
Am J Epidemiol ; 178(4): 543-50, 2013 Aug 15.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23568591

RESUMEN

Although there is increasing evidence that genetic factors influence gestational age, it is unclear to what extent this is due to fetal and/or maternal genes. In this study, we apply a novel analytical model to estimate genetic and environmental contributions to pregnancy history records obtained from 165,952 Swedish families consisting of offspring of twins, full siblings, and half-siblings (1987-2008). Results indicated that fetal genetic factors explained 13.1% (95% confidence interval (CI): 6.8, 19.4) of the variation in gestational age at delivery, while maternal genetic factors accounted for 20.6% (95% CI: 18.1, 23.2). The largest contribution to differences in the timing of birth were environmental factors, of which 10.1% (95% CI: 7.0, 13.2) was due to factors shared by births of the same mother, and 56.2% (95% CI: 53.0, 59.4) was pregnancy specific. Similar models fit to the same data dichotomized at clinically meaningful thresholds (e.g., preterm birth) resulted in less stable parameter estimates, but the collective results supported a model of homogeneous genetic and environmental effects across the range of gestational age. Since environmental factors explained most differences in the timing of birth, genetic studies may benefit from understanding the specific effect of fetal and maternal genes in the context of these yet-unidentified factors.


Asunto(s)
Edad Gestacional , Resultado del Embarazo/genética , Nacimiento Prematuro/genética , Gemelos/genética , Certificado de Nacimiento , Femenino , Feto , Fenómenos Genéticos , Humanos , Recién Nacido , Masculino , Madres , Embarazo , Suecia
15.
Neuroimage ; 59(2): 1123-31, 2012 Jan 16.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21983185

RESUMEN

The hippocampus expresses a large number of androgen receptors; therefore, in men it is potentially vulnerable to the gradual age-related decline of testosterone levels. In the present study we sought to elucidate the nature of the relationship between testosterone and hippocampal volume in a sample of middle-aged male twins (average age 55.8 years). We found no evidence for a correlation between testosterone level and hippocampal volume, as well as no indication of shared genetic influences. However, a significant moderating effect of testosterone on the genetic and environmental determinants of hippocampal volume was observed. Genetic influences on hippocampal volume increased substantially as a function of increasing testosterone level, while environmental influences either decreased or remained stable. These findings provide evidence for an apparent gene-by-hormone interaction on hippocampal volume. To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study to demonstrate that the heritability of a brain structure in adults may be modified by an endogenous biological factor.


Asunto(s)
Hipocampo/anatomía & histología , Hipocampo/fisiología , Testosterona/sangre , Gemelos/genética , Humanos , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética/métodos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Tamaño de los Órganos/fisiología
16.
Behav Genet ; 42(6): 867-74, 2012 Nov.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23011216

RESUMEN

This study sought to determine the relationship between body mass index (BMI) fluctuation and cardiovascular disease phenotypes, diabetes, and depression and the role of genetic and environmental factors in individual differences in BMI fluctuation using the extended twin-family model (ETFM). This study included 14,763 twins and their relatives. Health and Lifestyle Questionnaires were obtained from 28,492 individuals from the Virginia 30,000 dataset including twins, parents, siblings, spouses, and children of twins. Self-report cardiovascular disease, diabetes, and depression data were available. From self-reported height and weight, BMI fluctuation was calculated as the difference between highest and lowest BMI after age 18, for individuals 18-80 years. Logistic regression analyses were used to determine the relationship between BMI fluctuation and disease status. The ETFM was used to estimate the significance and contribution of genetic and environmental factors, cultural transmission, and assortative mating components to BMI fluctuation, while controlling for age. We tested sex differences in additive and dominant genetic effects, parental, non-parental, twin, and unique environmental effects. BMI fluctuation was highly associated with disease status, independent of BMI. Genetic effects accounted for ~34 % of variance in BMI fluctuation in males and ~43 % of variance in females. The majority of the variance was accounted for by environmental factors, about a third of which were shared among twins. Assortative mating, and cultural transmission accounted for only a small proportion of variance in this phenotype. Since there are substantial health risks associated with BMI fluctuation and environmental components of BMI fluctuation account for over 60 % of variance in males and over 50 % of variance in females, environmental risk factors may be appropriate targets to reduce BMI fluctuation.


Asunto(s)
Índice de Masa Corporal , Peso Corporal/genética , Enfermedades Cardiovasculares/genética , Depresión/genética , Diabetes Mellitus/genética , Ambiente , Adolescente , Adulto , Anciano , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Enfermedades Cardiovasculares/etiología , Depresión/etiología , Diabetes Mellitus/etiología , Femenino , Interacción Gen-Ambiente , Humanos , Modelos Logísticos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Modelos Genéticos , Factores de Riesgo , Hermanos , Gemelos Dicigóticos/genética , Gemelos Monocigóticos/genética , Aumento de Peso/genética , Pérdida de Peso/genética
17.
Behav Genet ; 42(1): 107-20, 2012 Jan.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21688193

RESUMEN

Hypertension is a risk factor for cognitive decline, but the mechanisms underlying the effects of hypertension on cognition, particularly in midlife, are unclear. We examined whether hypertension modifies genetic influences on individual differences in cognition. Nine cognitive domains and general cognitive ability were assessed in a sample of 1,237 male twins aged 51-60 who were divided into three blood pressure groups: non-hypertensive; medicated hypertensive; and unmedicated hypertensive. Heritability was significantly lower among unmedicated hypertensives compared to medicated hypertensives and non-hypertensives for visual-spatial ability (p = 0.013) and episodic memory (p = 0.004). There were no heritability differences between non-hypertensives and medicated hypertensives. In addition, there were no significant differences in mean level cognition across the three blood pressure groups. These results suggest that in middle-aged men, untreated hypertension suppresses normal genetic influences on individual differences in certain domains of cognition prior to the emergence of hypertension-related effects on cognitive performance. These results further suggest that antihypertensive medication may protect against or reverse this effect.


Asunto(s)
Trastornos del Conocimiento/complicaciones , Cognición , Hipertensión/genética , Envejecimiento , Antihipertensivos/farmacología , Presión Sanguínea , Trastornos del Conocimiento/genética , Humanos , Masculino , Memoria , Persona de Mediana Edad , Modelos Genéticos , Factores de Riesgo
18.
Behav Genet ; 42(4): 579-91, 2012 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22484556

RESUMEN

The goals of the study were to determine the extent to which the underlying structure of different types of well-being was multidimensional and whether well- and ill-being were influenced by similar or different genetic and environmental factors. Participants were 1226 male twins ages 51-60, from the Vietnam Era Twin Study of Aging. Measures included: psychological well-being, Multidimensional Personality Questionnaire Well-Being scale (MPQWB), life satisfaction, self-esteem, and depressive symptoms. A two-orthogonal-factor common pathway model fit the data well. Psychological well-being and self-esteem loaded most strongly on Factor 1, which was highly heritable (h(2) = .79). Life satisfaction loaded most strongly on Factor 2, which was only moderately heritable (h(2) = .32). Only MPQWB had measure-specific genetic influences. Depressive symptoms loaded on both factors, and only depressive symptoms had measure-specific common environmental influences. All measures had specific unique environmental influences. Results indicate that well-being is genetically and environmentally multidimensional and that ill-being has partial overlap with both latent factors.


Asunto(s)
Depresión/genética , Ambiente , Salud Mental , Satisfacción Personal , Autoimagen , Gemelos Dicigóticos/psicología , Gemelos Monocigóticos/psicología , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Modelos Genéticos , Análisis Multivariante , Personalidad/genética , Inventario de Personalidad , Encuestas y Cuestionarios , Gemelos Dicigóticos/genética , Gemelos Monocigóticos/genética
19.
J Child Psychol Psychiatry ; 53(6): 668-77, 2012 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22141405

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: A critical issue in devising effective interventions for the treatment of children's behavioral and emotional problems identifying genuine family environmental factors that place children at risk. In most twin and family studies, environmental factors are confounded with both direct genetic risk from parents and the indirect effect of genes influencing parents' ability to provide an optimal rearing environment. The present study was undertaken to determine whether parental psychopathology, specifically parental antisocial behavior (ASP), is a genuine environmental risk factor for juvenile conduct disturbance, depression and hyperactivity, or whether the association between parental ASP and children's behavioral and emotional problems can be explained as a secondary consequence of the intergenerational transmission of genetic factors. METHODS: An extended children of twins design comprised of data collected on 2,674 adult female and male twins, their spouses, and 2,454 of their children was used to test whether genetic and/or family environmental factors best accounted for the association between parental antisocial behavior and children's behavioral problems. An age-matched sample of 2,826 juvenile twin pairs from the Virginia Twin Study of Adolescent Behavioral Development was also included to examine developmental differences in gene expression by partitioning child-specific transmissible effects from those effects that persist into adulthood. The fit of alternative models was evaluated using the statistical program Mx. RESULTS: We found distinct patterns of transmission between parental antisocial behavior and juvenile conduct, depression and hyperactivity. Genetic and family environmental factors accounted for the resemblance between parents' ASP and children's conduct disturbance. Family environmental factors alone explained the association between child depression and parental ASP, and the impact of parental ASP on hyperactivity was entirely genetic. CONCLUSIONS: These findings underscore differences in the contribution of genetic and environmental factors on the patterns of association between parental antisocial behavior and juvenile psychopathology, having important clinical implications for the prevention and amelioration of child behavioral and emotional problems.


Asunto(s)
Trastorno por Déficit de Atención con Hiperactividad/genética , Hijo de Padres Discapacitados/psicología , Trastorno de la Conducta/genética , Trastorno Depresivo/genética , Medio Social , Adolescente , Adulto , Trastorno por Déficit de Atención con Hiperactividad/epidemiología , Estudios de Casos y Controles , Niño , Trastorno de la Conducta/epidemiología , Trastorno Depresivo/epidemiología , Femenino , Interacción Gen-Ambiente , Predisposición Genética a la Enfermedad , Humanos , Masculino , Análisis por Apareamiento , Modelos Teóricos , Estados Unidos/epidemiología
20.
Depress Anxiety ; 29(4): 320-7, 2012 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22461084

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Childhood separation anxiety disorder (SAD) is hypothesized to share etiologic roots with panic disorder. The aim of this study was to estimate the genetic and environmental sources of covariance between childhood SAD and adult onset panic attacks (AOPA), with the primary goal to determine whether these two phenotypes share a common genetic diathesis. METHODS: Participants included parents and their monozygotic or dizygotic twins (n = 1,437 twin pairs) participating in the Virginia Twin Study of Adolescent Behavioral Development and those twins who later completed the Young Adult Follow-Up (YAFU). The Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Assessment was completed at three waves during childhood/adolescence followed by the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-III-R at the YAFU. Two separate, bivariate Cholesky models were fit to childhood diagnoses of SAD and overanxious disorder (OAD), respectively, and their relation with AOPA; a trivariate Cholesky model also examined the collective influence of childhood SAD and OAD on AOPA. RESULTS: In the best-fitting bivariate model, the covariation between SAD and AOPA was accounted for by genetic and unique environmental factors only, with the genetic factor associated with childhood SAD explaining significant variance in AOPA. Environmental risk factors were not significantly shared between SAD and AOPA. By contrast, the genetic factor associated with childhood OAD did not contribute significantly to AOPA. Results of the trivariate Cholesky reaffirmed outcomes of bivariate models. CONCLUSIONS: These data indicate that childhood SAD and AOPA share a common genetic diathesis that is not observed for childhood OAD, strongly supporting the hypothesis of a specific genetic etiologic link between the two phenotypes.


Asunto(s)
Ansiedad de Separación/genética , Trastorno de Pánico/genética , Adolescente , Edad de Inicio , Niño , Femenino , Estudios de Seguimiento , Humanos , Masculino , Fenotipo , Escalas de Valoración Psiquiátrica , Factores de Riesgo , Gemelos Dicigóticos , Gemelos Monocigóticos
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA