Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 43
Filtrar
Más filtros

Banco de datos
País/Región como asunto
Tipo del documento
Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
Psychol Sci ; 34(9): 1024-1032, 2023 09.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37594058

RESUMEN

Grandiose narcissism is defined as increased motivation for status and viewing oneself as entitled and superior to others. We hypothesized that these tendencies might be associated with basal levels of testosterone because testosterone is considered the most social hormone-driving dominance and the motivation to achieve social status. We distinguished between two facets of grandiose narcissism: agentic (i.e., the tendency to self-promotion in order to win others' admiration and social influence) and antagonistic (i.e., a reactive strategy used to restore threatened status). In 283 adult men, we examined the association between these facets of narcissism and blood-tested and self-reported testosterone levels. Agentic narcissism-the default narcissistic strategy-was positively associated with both testosterone indicators. Moreover, self-reported and objectively measured testosterone were positively correlated. These findings extend previous work by showing that the facets of narcissism have distinct hormonal underpinnings.


Asunto(s)
Narcisismo , Testosterona , Adulto , Masculino , Humanos , Autoinforme , Motivación , Ego
2.
Neuropsychol Rehabil ; 33(10): 1650-1671, 2023 Dec.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37988367

RESUMEN

Emotional disorders are pervasive in the acquired brain injury (ABI) population, adversely affecting quality of life and rehabilitation. This study aimed to explore the unique associative effects of alexithymia as measured by the Perth Alexithymia Questionnaire (PAQ; i.e., difficulty identifying positive/negative feelings, difficulty describing positive/negative feelings, and externally orientated thinking), on emotional outcomes as measured by the Depression Anxiety Stress Scale-21 (DASS-21) and Mayo-Portland Adaptability Inventory (MPAI-4) Adjustment index, in 83 adults with ABI. The addition of alexithymia to hierarchical multiple regression models (controlling for demographic, injury-related, and functional outcome variables) yielded statistically significant changes in R2 for all emotional outcome measures (i.e., Depression, Anxiety, Stress, and Adjustment). Difficulty identifying negative feelings was found to be a significant unique predictor of Depression (ß = .43 p = <.001), Anxiety (ß = .40, p <.001), Stress (ß = .49, p <.001), and Adjustment (ß = .26, p = .001). Externally oriented thinking was found to be a significant unique predictor of Adjustment (ß = -.15, p = .033). These findings strengthen the argument that alexithymia, especially difficulties identifying negative feelings, may be an important risk factor for psychological distress in ABI and should be considered during early rehabilitation.


Asunto(s)
Síntomas Afectivos , Lesiones Encefálicas , Adulto , Humanos , Síntomas Afectivos/etiología , Calidad de Vida , Emociones , Factores de Riesgo , Lesiones Encefálicas/complicaciones , Lesiones Encefálicas/psicología
3.
Neuropsychol Rev ; 32(1): 51-69, 2022 03.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33759039

RESUMEN

Apathy and depression are common sequelae of acquired brain injury (ABI). Apathy is a syndrome characterized by diminished motivation and purposeful behaviours. Depression is a mood disorder featuring sadness, worthlessness, anhedonia and suicidal ideation. Both are associated negatively with activities of daily living (ADL), the skills required to fulfil basic and complex physical needs. However, the current literature's results are inconsistent and based on relatively small sample sizes. Furthermore, the unique and combined effects of apathy and depression as predictors of ADL have not yet been estimated. This is important, as both may have implications for planning rehabilitation after an ABI. Consequently, we aimed to estimate the association between apathy, depression and ADL in the stroke and traumatic brain injured population via meta-analysis and meta-analytic path-analysis. Based on the meta-analyses (N = 1,166 to N = 1,389), we estimated the following statistically significant bivariate effects: depression and apathy (r = .53, 95% CI: .42/.63), depression and ADL (r = -.27, 95% CI: -.43/-.11), apathy and ADL (r = -.41, 95% CI: -.51/-.31). A meta-analytic mediation model found that depression had a significant indirect effect onto ADL (ß = -.17, 95% CI: -.26/-.09), while apathy had a significant direct effect (ß = -.34, 95% CI: -.48/-.19) onto ADL (model R2 = .16). We interpreted the results to suggest that apathy and depression may impact adversely on engagement with ADL in people with ABI, although the potential influence of depression on ADL may occur primarily through its influence on apathy. Thus, greater focus on apathy by practitioners may be merited in cases with ABI.


Asunto(s)
Apatía , Lesiones Traumáticas del Encéfalo , Accidente Cerebrovascular , Actividades Cotidianas , Adulto , Lesiones Traumáticas del Encéfalo/complicaciones , Depresión/etiología , Humanos , Accidente Cerebrovascular/complicaciones
4.
J Int Neuropsychol Soc ; 28(9): 902-915, 2022 10.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34549700

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVE: Exercise has been found to be important in maintaining neurocognitive health. However, the effect of exercise intensity level remains relatively underexplored. Thus, to test the hypothesis that self-paced high-intensity exercise and cardiorespiratory fitness (peak aerobic capacity; VO2peak) increase grey matter (GM) volume, we examined the effect of a 6-month exercise intervention on frontal lobe GM regions that support the executive functions in older adults. METHODS: Ninety-eight cognitively normal participants (age = 69.06 ± 5.2 years; n = 54 female) were randomised into either a self-paced high- or moderate-intensity cycle-based exercise intervention group, or a no-intervention control group. Participants underwent magnetic resonance imaging and fitness assessment pre-intervention, immediately post-intervention, and 12-months post-intervention. RESULTS: The intervention was found to increase fitness in the exercise groups, as compared with the control group (F = 9.88, p = <0.001). Changes in pre-to-post-intervention fitness were associated with increased volume in the right frontal lobe (ß = 0.29, p = 0.036, r = 0.27), right supplementary motor area (ß = 0.30, p = 0.031, r = 0.29), and both right (ß = 0.32, p = 0.034, r = 0.30) and left gyrus rectus (ß = 0.30, p = 0.037, r = 0.29) for intervention, but not control participants. No differences in volume were observed across groups. CONCLUSIONS: At an aggregate level, six months of self-paced high- or moderate-intensity exercise did not increase frontal GM volume. However, experimentally-induced changes in individual cardiorespiratory fitness was positively associated with frontal GM volume in our sample of older adults. These results provide evidence of individual variability in exercise-induced fitness on brain structure.


Asunto(s)
Capacidad Cardiovascular , Sustancia Gris , Anciano , Encéfalo/patología , Corteza Cerebral/patología , Femenino , Sustancia Gris/diagnóstico por imagen , Humanos , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Persona de Mediana Edad
5.
Neuropsychol Rev ; 31(4): 722-738, 2021 12.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33624197

RESUMEN

Alexithymia is the inability to identify and describe one's own emotions. Some research suggests that organic alexithymia may occur after acquired brain injury (ABI). However, the results in the literature are inconsistent, when comparisons are made against healthy controls. Furthermore, a precise estimate of alexithymia prevalence in the ABI population has not yet been reported. Consequently, this meta-analysis aimed to estimate the prevalence and characteristics of alexithymia in ABI, as measured by the Toronto Alexithymia Scale-20 (TAS-20). Based on 22 unique ABI samples, a series of random-effects meta-analyses estimated moderate to large positive effect sizes (i.e., greater alexithymia in ABI samples) for the TAS-20 total scale (Hedges' g = 1.00, 95% CI [0.75, 1.35]), as well as the subscales: difficulty identifying feelings (Hedges' g = 0.92, 95% CI [0.66, 1.17]), difficulty describing feelings (Hedges' g = 0.69, 95% CI [0.50, 0.87]) and externally oriented thinking (Hedges' g = 0.75, 95% CI [0.64, 0.85]). Furthermore, a meta-regression identified a larger effect size (TAS-20 total scale score) for traumatic brain injury (TBI) samples, in comparison to non-TBI samples. Finally, the prevalence of clinically significant levels of alexithymia (TAS-20 total scale ≥ 68.4; i.e., two SDs above the general population mean) in ABI patients was estimated at 15.2%. We interpreted the results to suggest that ABI may have a substantial negative impact on affective processing abilities and, thus, comprehensive assessment of emotional functioning deficits following ABI should be considered by practitioners.


Asunto(s)
Síntomas Afectivos , Lesiones Encefálicas , Adulto , Síntomas Afectivos/epidemiología , Síntomas Afectivos/etiología , Lesiones Encefálicas/complicaciones , Lesiones Encefálicas/epidemiología , Emociones , Humanos , Prevalencia
6.
Am J Geriatr Psychiatry ; 29(2): 129-140, 2021 02.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32732104

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: There is a paucity of interventional research that systematically assesses the role of exercise intensity and cardiorespiratory fitness, and their relationship with executive function in older adults. To address this limitation, we have examined the effect of a systematically manipulated exercise intervention on executive function. METHODS: Ninety-nine cognitively normal participants (age = 69.10 ± 5.2 years; n = 54 female) were randomized into either a high-intensity cycle-based exercise, moderate-intensity cycle-based exercise, or no-intervention control group. All participants underwent neuropsychological testing and fitness assessment at baseline (preintervention), 6-month follow-up (postintervention), and 12-month postintervention. Executive function was measured comprehensively, including measures of each subdomain: Shifting, Updating/ Working Memory, Inhibition, Verbal Generativity, and Nonverbal Reasoning. Cardiorespiratory fitness was measured by analysis of peak aerobic capacity; VO2peak. RESULTS: First, the exercise intervention was found to increase cardiorespiratory fitness (VO2peak) in the intervention groups, in comparison to the control group (F =10.40, p≤0.01). However, the authors failed to find mean differences in executive function scores between the high-intensity, moderate intensity, or inactive control group. On the basis of change scores, cardiorespiratory fitness was found to associate positively with the executive function (EF) subdomains of Updating/Working Memory (ß = 0.37, p = 0.01, r = 0.34) and Verbal Generativity (ß = 0.30, p = 0.03, r = 0.28) for intervention, but not control participants. CONCLUSION: At the aggregate level, the authors failed to find evidence that 6-months of high-intensity aerobic exercise improves EF in older adults. However, it remains possible that individual differences in experimentally induced changes in cardiorespiratory fitness may be associated with changes in Updating/ Working Memory and Verbal Generativity.


Asunto(s)
Cognición , Función Ejecutiva/fisiología , Ejercicio Físico/fisiología , Anciano , Capacidad Cardiovascular/fisiología , Capacidad Cardiovascular/psicología , Ejercicio Físico/psicología , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Memoria a Corto Plazo , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas
7.
Neuropsychol Rev ; 30(2): 194-223, 2020 06.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32198606

RESUMEN

A number of cognitive abilities have been reported to predict outcome following a non-traumatic acquired brain injury (ABI) in adults. However, the results are inconsistent. Furthermore, the unique and combined capacity of these cognitive abilities to predict ABI outcome has not been evaluated. Consequently, we employed meta-analysis and multiple regression to evaluate the capacity of various neuropsychological domains to predict two separate outcome variables in adults: (1) activities of daily living; and (2) quality of life. Based on the activities of daily living meta-analysis (N = 2384), we estimated the following significant bivariate effects: memory (r = .31, 95% CI: .20/.41]), language (r = .33, 95% CI:.26/.40), attention (r = .38, 95% CI: .30/.46]), executive functions (r = .29, 95% CI: .19/.39]), and visuospatial abilities (r = .41, 95% CI: .34/ .48). Based on the quality of life meta-analysis (N = 1037), we estimated the following significant bivariate effects: memory (r = .12, 95% CI: .03/.20]), language (r = .19, 95% CI: .06/ .32), attention (r = .30, 95% CI: .16/.44]), executive functions (r = .24, 95% CI: .12/.37) and visuospatial/constructional abilities (r = .30, 95% CI: .14/.46). Meta-analytic structural equation modelling (metaSEM) identified two significant, unique predictors of activities of daily living, attention and visuospatial abilities, and the model accounted for 21% of the variance (multiple R2 = .21, 95%CI: .16/.26). For the corresponding quality of life metaSEM, no statistically significant unique predictors were identified, however, a significant multiple correlation was observed, multiple R2 = .11 (95%CI: 04/.18). We conclude that practitioners may be able to predict, with some degree of accuracy, functional outcome following a stroke and other non-traumatic ABI in adults. We also provide some critical commentary on the nature and quality of the measures used in this area of research to represent the cognitive dimensions of interest.


Asunto(s)
Lesiones Encefálicas/rehabilitación , Rehabilitación de Accidente Cerebrovascular , Accidente Cerebrovascular/psicología , Actividades Cotidianas , Adulto , Atención , Función Ejecutiva , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Memoria , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Calidad de Vida , Recuperación de la Función
8.
Mem Cognit ; 48(1): 171, 2020 Jan.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31372845

RESUMEN

Confidence intervals and regression lines were omitted from Fig. 1 in this article as originally published. This error was introduced during production. The original article has been corrected.

9.
Mem Cognit ; 47(8): 1445-1456, 2019 11.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31228014

RESUMEN

Internet blogs have become an important platform for the discussion of many scientific issues, including climate change. Blogs, and in particular the comment sections of blogs, also play a major role in the dissemination of contrarian positions that question mainstream climate science. The effect of this content on people's attitudes is not fully understood. In particular, it is unknown how the interaction between the content of blog posts and blog comments affects readers' attitudes. We report an experiment that orthogonally varied those two variables using blog posts and comments that either did, or did not, support the scientific consensus on climate change. We find that beliefs are partially shaped by readers' perception of how widely an opinion expressed in a blog post appears to be shared by other readers. The perceived social consensus among readers, in turn, is determined by whether blog comments endorse or reject the contents of a post. When comments reject the content, perceived reader consensus is lower than when comments endorse the content. The results underscore the importance of perceived social consensus on opinion formation.


Asunto(s)
Actitud , Blogging , Cambio Climático , Consenso , Percepción Social , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino
10.
J Aging Phys Act ; 27(5): 703-710, 2019 09 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30747562

RESUMEN

Objectives: To examine the associations between physical activity duration and intensity, cardiorespiratory fitness, and executive function in older adults. Methods: Data from 99 cognitively normal adults (age = 69.10 ± 5.1 years; n = 54 females) were used in the current study. Physical activity (intensity and duration) was measured with the International Physical Activity Questionnaire, and fitness was measured by analysis of maximal aerobic capacity, VO2peak. Executive function was measured comprehensively, including measures of Shifting, Updating, Inhibition, Generativity, and Nonverbal Reasoning. Results: Higher levels of cardiorespiratory fitness were associated with better performance on Generativity (B = .55; 95% confidence interval [.15, .97]). No significant associations were found between self-reported physical activity intensity/duration and executive functions. Discussion: To our knowledge, this study is the first to identify an association between fitness and Generativity. Associations between physical activity duration and intensity and executive function requires further study, using objective physical activity measures and longitudinal observations.


Asunto(s)
Capacidad Cardiovascular/fisiología , Función Ejecutiva , Anciano , Capacidad Cardiovascular/psicología , Función Ejecutiva/fisiología , Ejercicio Físico/fisiología , Ejercicio Físico/psicología , Femenino , Humanos , Vida Independiente/psicología , Masculino , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Encuestas y Cuestionarios
11.
Neuropsychol Rev ; 27(3): 187-201, 2017 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28681109

RESUMEN

Several neuropsychological dimensions are correlated with functional outcome (e.g., ability to return to family and community roles) following traumatic brain injury (TBI). Commonly investigated neuropsychological dimensions include verbal memory, visuo-spatial construction, set-shifting, generativity, and processing speed. Unfortunately, small sample sizes across relevant studies have contributed to inconsistent results. Furthermore, no studies have concurrently measured all of the candidate neuropsychological predictors, most of which are known to be inter-correlated. Thus, the unique predictive effects associated with the candidate predictors in TBI recovery have never been investigated. Consequently, this study used both meta-analysis and multiple regression to statistically evaluate neuropsychological candidate predictors across two outcome variables (1) the Glasgow Outcome Scale-Extended (GOS-E) and (2) the Disability Rating Scale (DRS). Seven studies met inclusion criteria. Based on the meta-analyses, the following neuropsychological dimensions were found to be correlated with the GOS-E: immediate verbal memory (r = .43, 95% CI [.27, .58]), delayed verbal memory (r = .43, 95% CI [.21, .61]), visuo-spatial construction (r = .29, 95% CI [.15, .53]), set-shifting (r = -.31, 95% CI [-.45, -.15], and generativity (r = .44, 95% CI [.32, .54]). By contrast, only one neuropsychological dimension was found to be significantly related to the DRS (generativity: r = -.21, 95% CI [-.39, -.01]). Multiple regression on the GOS-E relevant meta-analytically derived correlation matrix determined that all neuropsychological dimensions were significant predictors of the GOS-E (multiple R 2 = .31) with the exception of immediate verbal memory or learning. However, due to analytic characteristics, these findings must be interpreted with caution. Results were consistent with the need to consider multiple neuropsychological abilities in recovery and rehabilitation following TBI.


Asunto(s)
Lesiones Traumáticas del Encéfalo , Adulto , Lesiones Traumáticas del Encéfalo/diagnóstico , Lesiones Traumáticas del Encéfalo/psicología , Lesiones Traumáticas del Encéfalo/rehabilitación , Escala de Consecuencias de Glasgow , Humanos , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Recuperación de la Función , Índice de Severidad de la Enfermedad
13.
Int J Clin Exp Hypn ; 72(3): 229-253, 2024.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38861252

RESUMEN

We investigated whether adding hypnosis to CBT (CBTH) improved treatment outcomes for MDD with a two-armed, parallel-treated, randomized-controlled trial using anonymous self-report and clinician-blinded assessments. Expectancy, credibility, and attitude to hypnosis were also examined. Participants (n = 66) were randomly allocated to 10-weekly sessions of group-based CBT or CBTH. LMM analyses of ITT and Completer data at post-treatment, six-month and 12-month follow-up showed that both treatments were probably efficacious but we did not find significant differences between them. Analyses of remission and response to treatment data revealed that the CBTH Completer group significantly outperformed CBT at 12-month follow-up (p = .011). CBTH also displayed significantly higher associations between credibility, expectancy and mood outcomes up to 12-month follow-up (all p < .05 or better), while attitude to hypnosis showed one significant association (r = -0.57, p < .05). These results suggest that hypnosis shows promise as an adjunct in the treatment of MDD but a larger sample size is required to fully test its merits.


Asunto(s)
Terapia Cognitivo-Conductual , Trastorno Depresivo Mayor , Hipnosis , Humanos , Hipnosis/métodos , Femenino , Masculino , Trastorno Depresivo Mayor/terapia , Trastorno Depresivo Mayor/psicología , Terapia Cognitivo-Conductual/métodos , Adulto , Persona de Mediana Edad , Resultado del Tratamiento
14.
Cognition ; 250: 105816, 2024 Jun 21.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38908305

RESUMEN

Research on individual differences in face recognition has provided important foundational insights: their broad range, cognitive specificity, strong heritability, and resilience to change. Elusive, however, has been the key issue of practical relevance: do these individual differences correlate with aspects of life that go beyond the recognition of faces, per se? Though often assumed, especially in social realms, such correlates remain largely theoretical, without empirical support. Here, we investigate an array of potential social correlates of face recognition. We establish social relationship quality as a reproducible correlate. This link generalises across face recognition tasks and across independent samples. In contrast, we detect no robust association with the sheer quantity of social connections, whether measured directly via number of social contacts or indirectly via extraversion-related personality indices. These findings document the existence of a key social correlate of face recognition and provide some of the first evidence to support its practical relevance. At the same time, they challenge the naive assumption that face recognition relates equally to all social outcomes. In contrast, they suggest a focused link of face recognition to the quality, not quantity, of one's social connections.

15.
Psychol Sci ; 24(5): 622-33, 2013 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23531484

RESUMEN

Although nearly all domain experts agree that carbon dioxide emissions are altering the world's climate, segments of the public remain unconvinced by the scientific evidence. Internet blogs have become a platform for denial of climate change, and bloggers have taken a prominent role in questioning climate science. We report a survey of climate-blog visitors to identify the variables underlying acceptance and rejection of climate science. Our findings parallel those of previous work and show that endorsement of free-market economics predicted rejection of climate science. Endorsement of free markets also predicted the rejection of other established scientific findings, such as the facts that HIV causes AIDS and that smoking causes lung cancer. We additionally show that, above and beyond endorsement of free markets, endorsement of a cluster of conspiracy theories (e.g., that the Federal Bureau of Investigation killed Martin Luther King, Jr.) predicted rejection of climate science as well as other scientific findings. Our results provide empirical support for previous suggestions that conspiratorial thinking contributes to the rejection of science. Acceptance of science, by contrast, was strongly associated with the perception of a consensus among scientists.


Asunto(s)
Procesos Climáticos , Negación en Psicología , Fraude/psicología , Motivación/fisiología , Vuelo Espacial , United States National Aeronautics and Space Administration , Blogging , Cambio Climático , Análisis Factorial , Humanos , Luna , Ciencia , Encuestas y Cuestionarios , Estados Unidos
16.
J Pers Assess ; 95(6): 645-56, 2013.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23862685

RESUMEN

Paulhus (1984) proposed a 2-factor model of socially desirable responding (SDR) and created the Balanced Inventory of Desirable Responding (BIDR) to capture the 2 dimensions: self-deceptive enhancement and impression management. However, the 2-factor model has yet to be supported via confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) of the BIDR. Paulhus and Reid (1991) proposed a revised model of SDR, which bifurcated the self-deceptive enhancement dimension into 2 factors: self-deceptive enhancement (SDE; positively keyed items) and self-deceptive denial (SDD; negatively keyed items). Thus, it was considered useful to test the revised SDR model on the BIDR, as this might have implications for the manner in which the BIDR should be scored. Additionally, as the BIDR subscales might be positively correlated, it was considered useful from a both a theoretical and practical perspective to test the possibility that the BIDR might measure a general SDR process. Based on a sample of 466 adults, Paulhus and Reid's revised model of SDR was largely supported when tested via CFA on a bifactor model, which included a first-order general SDR factor and 2 nested factors. Thus, applied researchers might consider using total BIDR composite scores, a self-deceptive enhancement composite score (positively keyed items), and impression management composite scores. However, ideally, researchers would use a bifactor model to test substantive hypotheses, as the bifactor model partitions true score variance into unique sources, which facilitates less ambiguous interpretations of effects.


Asunto(s)
Negación en Psicología , Control Interno-Externo , Modelos Psicológicos , Autoimagen , Deseabilidad Social , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Motivación , Inventario de Personalidad
17.
PLoS One ; 18(4): e0283951, 2023.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37018324

RESUMEN

Misinformation can continue to influence reasoning after correction; this is known as the continued influence effect (CIE). Theoretical accounts of the CIE suggest failure of two cognitive processes to be causal, namely memory updating and suppression of misinformation reliance. Both processes can also be conceptualised as subcomponents of contemporary executive function (EF) models; specifically, working-memory updating and prepotent-response inhibition. EF may thus predict susceptibility to the CIE. The current study investigated whether individual differences in EF could predict individual differences in CIE susceptibility. Participants completed several measures of EF subcomponents, including those of updating and inhibition, as well as set shifting, and a standard CIE task. The relationship between EF and CIE was then assessed using a correlation analysis of the EF and CIE measures, as well as structural equation modelling of the EF-subcomponent latent variable and CIE latent variable. Results showed that EF can predict susceptibility to the CIE, especially the factor of working-memory updating. These results further our understanding of the CIE's cognitive antecedents and provide potential directions for real-world CIE intervention.


Asunto(s)
Función Ejecutiva , Memoria a Corto Plazo , Humanos , Función Ejecutiva/fisiología , Memoria a Corto Plazo/fisiología , Individualidad , Solución de Problemas , Inhibición Psicológica
18.
Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) ; : 17470218231203679, 2023 Oct 30.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37710359

RESUMEN

Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and the broader autistic phenotype (BAP) have been suggested to be associated with perceptual-cognitive difficulties processing human faces. However, the empirical results are mixed, arguably, in part due to inadequate samples and analyses. Consequently, we administered the Cambridge Face Perception Test (CFPT), the Reading the Mind in the Eyes Test (RMET), a vocabulary test, and the Autism Quotient (AQ) to a sample of 318 adults in the general community. Based on a disattenuated path analytic modelling strategy, we found that both face perception ability (ß = -.21) and facial emotional expression recognition ability (ß = -.27) predicted uniquely and significantly the Communication dimension of AQ. Vocabulary failed to yield a significant, direct effect onto the Communication dimension of the AQ. We conclude that difficulties perceiving information from the faces of others may contribute to difficulties in nonverbal communication, as conceptualised and measured within the context of BAP.

19.
Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) ; 75(7): 1259-1271, 2022 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34541938

RESUMEN

Research has consistently shown that misinformation can continue to affect inferential reasoning after a correction. This phenomenon is known as the continued influence effect (CIE). Recent studies have demonstrated that CIE susceptibility can be predicted by individual differences in stable cognitive abilities. Based on this, it was reasoned that CIE susceptibility ought to have some degree of stability itself; however, this has never been tested. The current study aimed to investigate the temporal stability of retraction sensitivity, arguably a major determinant of CIE susceptibility. Participants were given parallel forms of a standard CIE task 4 weeks apart, and the association between testing points was assessed with an intra-class correlation coefficient and confirmatory factor analysis. Results suggested that retraction sensitivity is relatively stable and can be predicted as an individual-differences variable. These results encourage continued individual-differences research on the CIE and have implications for real-world CIE intervention.


Asunto(s)
Comunicación , Solución de Problemas , Cognición , Humanos , Individualidad
20.
Dev Psychol ; 58(7): 1254-1263, 2022 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35389686

RESUMEN

Working memory is an often studied and important psychological construct. The growth of working memory capacity (WMC) in childhood is described as linear. Average adult WMC is estimated as either four or five "chunks." Using latent curve models of data from a measure of digit span backward that was administered longitudinally to a large sample representative of the native-English-speaking U.S. kindergarten population in 2011, we found that WMC growth in childhood is curvilinear. It shows an increasing yet decelerating pattern. Scoring rules (e.g., requiring 50% or 75% of trials correct) influence age-based estimates, but WMCs have likely been underestimated in children, and the average adult WMC of five is more plausible than four, as measured by digit span backward. Developmental WMC estimates, such as those reported in this research, may help others develop prescriptive learning interventions for children and understand its growth and decline across the life span. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).


Asunto(s)
Memoria a Corto Plazo , Adulto , Niño , Humanos
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA