Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 48
Filtrar
Más filtros

Banco de datos
País/Región como asunto
Tipo del documento
Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 121(28): e2320750121, 2024 Jul 09.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38950367

RESUMEN

Meta-analyses have concluded that positive emotions do not reduce appetitive risk behaviors (risky behaviors that fulfill appetitive or craving states, such as smoking and excessive alcohol use). We propose that this conclusion is premature. Drawing on the Appraisal Tendency Framework and related theories of emotion and decision-making, we hypothesized that gratitude (a positive emotion) can decrease cigarette smoking, a leading cause of premature death globally. A series of multimethod studies provided evidence supporting our hypothesis (collective N = 34,222). Using nationally representative US samples and an international sample drawn from 87 countries, Studies 1 and 2 revealed that gratitude was inversely associated with likelihood of smoking, even after accounting for numerous covariates. Other positive emotions (e.g., compassion) lacked such consistent associations, as expected. Study 3, and its replication, provided further support for emotion specificity: Experimental induction of gratitude, unlike compassion or sadness, reduced cigarette craving compared to a neutral state. Study 4, and its replication, showed that inducing gratitude causally increased smoking cessation behavior, as evidenced by enrollment in a web-based cessation intervention. Self-reported gratitude mediated the effects in both experimental studies. Finally, Study 5 found that current antismoking messaging campaigns by the US Centers for Disease Control and Prevention primarily evoked sadness and compassion, but seldom gratitude. Together, our studies advance understanding of positive emotion effects on appetitive risk behaviors; they also offer practical implications for the design of public health campaigns.


Asunto(s)
Emociones , Conductas Relacionadas con la Salud , Salud Pública , Humanos , Emociones/fisiología , Masculino , Femenino , Adulto , Promoción de la Salud/métodos , Cese del Hábito de Fumar/psicología , Persona de Mediana Edad , Fumar/psicología , Estados Unidos
2.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 117(2): 943-949, 2020 01 14.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31888990

RESUMEN

Do negative feelings in general trigger addictive behavior, or do specific emotions play a stronger role? Testing these alternative accounts of emotion and decision making, we drew on the Appraisal Tendency Framework to predict that sadness, specifically, rather than negative mood, generally, would 1) increase craving, impatience, and actual addictive substance use and 2) do so through mechanisms selectively heightened by sadness. Using a nationally representative, longitudinal survey, study 1 (n = 10,685) revealed that sadness, but not other negative emotions (i.e., fear, anger, shame), reliably predicted current smoking as well as relapsing 20 years later. Study 2 (n = 425) used an experimental design, and found further support for emotion specificity: Sadness, but not disgust, increased self-reported craving relative to a neutral state. Studies 3 and 4 (n = 918) introduced choice behavior as outcome variables, revealing that sadness causally increased impatience for cigarette puffs. Moreover, study 4 revealed that the effect of sadness on impatience was more fully explained by concomitant appraisals of self-focus, which are specific to sadness, than by concomitant appraisals of negative valence, which are general to all negative emotions. Importantly, study 4 also examined the topography of actual smoking behavior, finding that experimentally induced sadness (as compared to neutral emotion) causally increased the volume and duration of cigarette puffs inhaled. Together, the present studies provide support for a more nuanced model regarding the effects of emotion on tobacco use, in particular, as well as on addictive behavior, in general.


Asunto(s)
Conducta Adictiva , Emociones/efectos de los fármacos , Tristeza/efectos de los fármacos , Trastornos Relacionados con Sustancias , Adolescente , Adulto , Afecto , Conducta de Elección , Fumar Cigarrillos/efectos adversos , Toma de Decisiones , Femenino , Humanos , Estudios Longitudinales , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Tristeza/psicología , Adulto Joven
3.
Psychol Sport Exerc ; 65: 102348, 2023 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36465329

RESUMEN

Developmentally appropriate sport contexts have the potential to positively influence young people's physiological, psychological, and social outcomes. However, little is known about how families returned to sport in the wake of COVID-19-related restrictions or how socioeconomic and demographic factors influenced parents' perceptions of barriers to returning. A nationally representative sample (N = 6183) of American youth sport parents completed a questionnaire in which they provided demographic information and answered questions related to the barriers they perceived in returning to sport, such as the risk of their child getting sick. Structural equation modeling was used to examine the relationships among a range of socioeconomic and demographic factors and these barriers to returning. Results suggest that parents from racially minoritized and urban neighborhoods held higher levels of concern over health-related and practical barriers to returning to sport. Findings highlight the importance of designing available, equitable, and appropriate youth sport contexts.

4.
J Clin Psychol Med Settings ; 28(2): 405-417, 2021 06.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32519037

RESUMEN

Chronic pain has an estimated annual prevalence rate between 10 and 35%. In the US, first-line treatment for chronic pain is often opioids. OBJECTIVE: To our knowledge, this is the first study exploring psychological flexibility and its association with pain severity, pain interference and risk of opioid misuse in chronic pain patients. METHODS: Data were collected at two outpatient pain clinics in the northeastern United States. Adults (N = 99) completed a cross-sectional survey with validated measures. Pain severity and pain interference were hypothesized to uniquely predict the risk of opioid misuse. Pain severity was hypothesized to predict pain interference. Finally, psychological flexibility was hypothesized as an indirect effect in these relationships. RESULTS: Main findings suggest that pain severity predicts risk of opioid misuse, mediated by psychological flexibly. Pain interference also predicts risk of opioid misuse, mediated by psychological flexibility. Finally, results suggest pain severity predicts pain interference, mediated by psychological flexibility. DISCUSSION: Implications of findings are discussed in terms of future psychological and medical assessments and interventions for chronic pain patients seeking prescription opioids.


Asunto(s)
Dolor Crónico , Trastornos Relacionados con Opioides , Mal Uso de Medicamentos de Venta con Receta , Adulto , Analgésicos Opioides/efectos adversos , Dolor Crónico/tratamiento farmacológico , Dolor Crónico/epidemiología , Estudios Transversales , Humanos , Trastornos Relacionados con Opioides/complicaciones , Trastornos Relacionados con Opioides/epidemiología
5.
Annu Rev Psychol ; 66: 799-823, 2015 Jan 03.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25251484

RESUMEN

A revolution in the science of emotion has emerged in recent decades, with the potential to create a paradigm shift in decision theories. The research reveals that emotions constitute potent, pervasive, predictable, sometimes harmful and sometimes beneficial drivers of decision making. Across different domains, important regularities appear in the mechanisms through which emotions influence judgments and choices. We organize and analyze what has been learned from the past 35 years of work on emotion and decision making. In so doing, we propose the emotion-imbued choice model, which accounts for inputs from traditional rational choice theory and from newer emotion research, synthesizing scientific models.


Asunto(s)
Toma de Decisiones/fisiología , Emociones/fisiología , Modelos Psicológicos , Humanos
6.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 109(44): 17903-7, 2012 Oct 30.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23012416

RESUMEN

As leaders ascend to more powerful positions in their groups, they face ever-increasing demands. As a result, there is a common perception that leaders have higher stress levels than nonleaders. However, if leaders also experience a heightened sense of control--a psychological factor known to have powerful stress-buffering effects--leadership should be associated with reduced stress levels. Using unique samples of real leaders, including military officers and government officials, we found that, compared with nonleaders, leaders had lower levels of the stress hormone cortisol and lower reports of anxiety (study 1). In study 2, leaders holding more powerful positions exhibited lower cortisol levels and less anxiety than leaders holding less powerful positions, a relationship explained significantly by their greater sense of control. Altogether, these findings reveal a clear relationship between leadership and stress, with leadership level being inversely related to stress.


Asunto(s)
Liderazgo , Estrés Psicológico , Ansiedad , Humanos , Hidrocortisona/sangre
7.
J Child Sex Abus ; 24(3): 217-37, 2015.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25942283

RESUMEN

A quasi-experimental design was used to compare the effectiveness of group (game-based cognitive behavioral) therapy to group-plus-individual therapy for child sexual abuse. The sample consisted predominantly of children from economically disadvantaged, African-American or Latino backgrounds. Pretreatment scores were examined in order to determine which factors influence treatment referral decisions. Results suggest that children who were referred for individual therapy in addition to group therapy report higher pretreatment levels of sexualized behavior. Posttreatment differences were also compared across therapy conditions. Results suggest that individual therapy is needed to address the sexual concerns of survivors but that it may not be needed to augment the effects of group therapy for other symptoms. Implications for treatment are discussed.


Asunto(s)
Abuso Sexual Infantil/rehabilitación , Conducta Infantil/psicología , Terapia Cognitivo-Conductual/métodos , Ludoterapia/métodos , Pobreza/psicología , Psicoterapia de Grupo/métodos , Derivación y Consulta , Resultado del Tratamiento , Negro o Afroamericano , Niño , Preescolar , Femenino , Hispánicos o Latinos , Humanos , Masculino , Poblaciones Vulnerables
8.
Psychol Sci ; 25(6): 1262-7, 2014 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24760144

RESUMEN

The human mind tends to excessively discount the value of delayed rewards relative to immediate ones, and it is thought that "hot" affective processes drive desires for short-term gratification. Supporting this view, recent findings demonstrate that sadness exacerbates financial impatience even when the sadness is unrelated to the economic decision at hand. Such findings might reinforce the view that emotions must always be suppressed to combat impatience. But if emotions serve adaptive functions, then certain emotions might be capable of reducing excessive impatience for delayed rewards. We found evidence supporting this alternative view. Specifically, we found that (a) the emotion gratitude reduces impatience even when real money is at stake, and (b) the effects of gratitude are differentiable from those of the more general positive state of happiness. These findings challenge the view that individuals must tamp down affective responses through effortful self-regulation to reach more patient and adaptive economic decisions.


Asunto(s)
Toma de Decisiones , Emociones , Recompensa , Adolescente , Afecto , Actitud , Conducta Cooperativa , Economía , Femenino , Felicidad , Humanos , Relaciones Interpersonales , Masculino , Adulto Joven
9.
Psychol Sci ; 24(1): 72-9, 2013 Jan 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23150274

RESUMEN

We hypothesized a phenomenon that we term myopic misery. According to our hypothesis, sadness increases impatience and creates a myopic focus on obtaining money immediately instead of later. This focus, in turn, increases intertemporal discount rates and thereby produces substantial financial costs. In three experiments, we randomly assigned participants to sad- and neutral-state conditions, and then offered intertemporal choices. Disgust served as a comparison condition in Experiments 1 and 2. Sadness significantly increased impatience: Relative to median neutral-state participants, median sad-state participants accepted 13% to 34% less money immediately to avoid waiting 3 months for payment. In Experiment 2, impatient thoughts mediated the effects. Experiment 3 revealed that sadness made people more present biased (i.e., wanting something immediately), but not globally more impatient. Disgusted participants were not more impatient than neutral participants, and that lack of difference implies that the same financial effects do not arise from all negative emotions. These results show that myopic misery is a robust and potentially harmful phenomenon.


Asunto(s)
Afecto , Conducta de Elección , Toma de Decisiones , Conducta Impulsiva/economía , Recompensa , Factores Socioeconómicos , Adulto , Anciano , Femenino , Humanos , Juicio , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Motivación , Solución de Problemas , Pensamiento , Adulto Joven
10.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38249753

RESUMEN

Patients with obsessive compulsive disorder (OCD) present as risk-averse and avoidant of feared stimuli, yet the literature examining risk aversion in OCD is conflicting. One possible explanation is that patients may exhibit aversion only on ambiguous tasks where the likelihood of possible outcomes is unknown. To test this idea, the current study assigned 30 patients with OCD versus 30 non-psychiatric controls (NPC) to conditions of known versus unknown risk (i.e., probabilities) on the Beads Task. Importantly, the task involved real financial stakes. We also examined self-reported intolerance of uncertainty (IU) as a mechanism. Results revealed a significant risk information x group interaction for certainty about the decision. Specifically, while NPCs felt significantly less certain on the unknown risk (versus known risk) task, the OCD group felt uncertain regardless of risk information. Results also revealed a significant main effect of group for distress after deciding, such that the OCD group was more distressed across all task versions compared to NPCs. Elevated trait IU was associated with higher task-related distress. Results indicate that even when patients with OCD are given information about likelihoods, they still feel uncertain and experience distress. Findings have clinical implications for addressing risk aversion and ambiguity/uncertainty in treatment.

11.
J Exp Psychol Gen ; 151(4): 960-965, 2022 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34472961

RESUMEN

Escalation of commitment-the tendency to remain committed to a course of action, often despite negative prospects-is common. Why does it persist? Across three preregistered experiments (N = 3,888), we tested the hypothesis that escalating commitment signals trustworthiness. Experiments 1-2, respectively, revealed that decision makers who escalated commitment were perceived as more trustworthy and entrusted with 29% more money by third-party observers. Experiment 3 revealed that decision makers who escalated commitment subsequently made more trustworthy choices, returning 15% more money than those who de-escalated. Decision makers were equally likely to escalate commitment in public versus in private, possibly because they previously internalized how others would evaluate them. Complementing research examining cognitive factors driving escalation of commitment, the present work reveals that accounting for the reputational causes and consequences of decisions to escalate enhances understanding of why escalation is so common and suggests how organizations might reduce it. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).


Asunto(s)
Toma de Decisiones , Confianza , Humanos
12.
Affect Sci ; 3(3): 577-602, 2022 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36185503

RESUMEN

The COVID-19 pandemic (and its aftermath) highlights a critical need to communicate health information effectively to the global public. Given that subtle differences in information framing can have meaningful effects on behavior, behavioral science research highlights a pressing question: Is it more effective to frame COVID-19 health messages in terms of potential losses (e.g., "If you do not practice these steps, you can endanger yourself and others") or potential gains (e.g., "If you practice these steps, you can protect yourself and others")? Collecting data in 48 languages from 15,929 participants in 84 countries, we experimentally tested the effects of message framing on COVID-19-related judgments, intentions, and feelings. Loss- (vs. gain-) framed messages increased self-reported anxiety among participants cross-nationally with little-to-no impact on policy attitudes, behavioral intentions, or information seeking relevant to pandemic risks. These results were consistent across 84 countries, three variations of the message framing wording, and 560 data processing and analytic choices. Thus, results provide an empirical answer to a global communication question and highlight the emotional toll of loss-framed messages. Critically, this work demonstrates the importance of considering unintended affective consequences when evaluating nudge-style interventions.

13.
J Asthma ; 48(10): 986-993, 2011 Dec.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21999750

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVE: Leukocyte persistence during chronic (quiescent) phases of asthma is a major hallmark of the disease. The mechanisms regulating these persistent leukocyte populations are not clearly understood. An alternative family of chemoattracting proteins, cyclophilins (Cyps), has recently been shown to contribute to leukocyte recruitment in animal models of allergic asthma. The goals of this study were to determine whether Cyps are present in asthma patients during the chronic phase of the disease and to investigate whether levels of Cyps associate with clinical parameters of disease severity. METHODS: Nasal wash samples from an urban cohort of 137 patients of age 6-20 years with physician-diagnosed asthma were examined for the presence of cyclophilin A (CypA), cyclophilin B (CypB), as well as several other classical chemokines. Linear, logistic, or ordinal regressions were performed to identify associations between Cyps, chemokines, and clinical parameters of asthma. The asthma cohort was further divided into previously established phenotypic clusters (cluster 1: n = 55; cluster 2: n = 31; and cluster 3: n = 51) and examined for associations. RESULTS: Levels of CypB in the asthma group were highly elevated compared to nonasthmatic controls, while a slight increase in Monocyte Chemotactic Protein-1 (MCP-1) was also observed. CypA and MCP-1 were associated with levels of eosinophil cationic protein (ECP; a marker of eosinophil activation). Cluster-specific associations were found for CypA and CypB and clinical asthma parameters [e.g. forced expiratory volume in 1 second (FEV(1)) and ECP]. CONCLUSIONS: Cyps are present in nasal wash samples of asthma patients and may be a novel biomarker for clinical parameters of asthma severity.


Asunto(s)
Asma/metabolismo , Ciclofilinas/análisis , Fenotipo , Adolescente , Asma/fisiopatología , Biomarcadores/análisis , Quimiocina CCL2/análisis , Quimiocinas/análisis , Niño , Proteína Catiónica del Eosinófilo/análisis , Espacio Extracelular/química , Femenino , Volumen Espiratorio Forzado , Humanos , Masculino , Líquido del Lavado Nasal/química , Adulto Joven
14.
Behav Med ; 37(4): 113-8, 2011 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22168327

RESUMEN

Although mammography can aid in the early detection and prevention of breast cancer, many women do not receive annual mammograms. It remains unclear whether anxiety about breast cancer inhibits or promotes mammography rates. The way in which women regulate their anxiety (ie, level of experiential avoidance) may play a role in predicting mammography adherence. A community sample of women (N = 84) completed a questionnaire which assessed mammography rates, experiential avoidance, and breast cancer anxiety. The results suggest that, while controlling for breast cancer anxiety, experiential avoidance (ß = .31, p < .01) significantly predicted mammography rates. When examining experiential avoidance as a moderator, a multiple regression analysis approached significance (R2 Δ = .04, p = .07), suggesting that a woman's level of experiential avoidance influences the relationship between anxiety and mammography. These findings will help enable health care practitioners to better identify women at risk of non-adherence to mammography recommendations.


Asunto(s)
Ansiedad/psicología , Reacción de Prevención , Neoplasias de la Mama/psicología , Mamografía/psicología , Aceptación de la Atención de Salud/psicología , Anciano , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Ansiedad/complicaciones , Ansiedad/diagnóstico por imagen , Neoplasias de la Mama/complicaciones , Neoplasias de la Mama/diagnóstico por imagen , Neoplasias de la Mama/prevención & control , Femenino , Humanos , Tamizaje Masivo/psicología , Persona de Mediana Edad
15.
Nat Hum Behav ; 5(8): 1089-1110, 2021 08.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34341554

RESUMEN

The COVID-19 pandemic has increased negative emotions and decreased positive emotions globally. Left unchecked, these emotional changes might have a wide array of adverse impacts. To reduce negative emotions and increase positive emotions, we tested the effectiveness of reappraisal, an emotion-regulation strategy that modifies how one thinks about a situation. Participants from 87 countries and regions (n = 21,644) were randomly assigned to one of two brief reappraisal interventions (reconstrual or repurposing) or one of two control conditions (active or passive). Results revealed that both reappraisal interventions (vesus both control conditions) consistently reduced negative emotions and increased positive emotions across different measures. Reconstrual and repurposing interventions had similar effects. Importantly, planned exploratory analyses indicated that reappraisal interventions did not reduce intentions to practice preventive health behaviours. The findings demonstrate the viability of creating scalable, low-cost interventions for use around the world. PROTOCOL REGISTRATION: The stage 1 protocol for this Registered Report was accepted in principle on 12 May 2020. The protocol, as accepted by the journal, can be found at https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.c.4878591.v1.


Asunto(s)
COVID-19/psicología , Regulación Emocional , Emociones , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino
16.
J Asthma ; 47(7): 728-34, 2010 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20684733

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVE: Asthma in children and adolescents is a heterogeneous syndrome comprised of multiple subgroups with variable disease expression and response to environmental exposures. The goal of this study was to define homogeneous phenotypic clusters within a cohort of children and adolescents with asthma and to determine overall and within-cluster associations between environmental tobacco smoke (ETS) exposure and asthma characteristics. METHODS: A combined hierarchical/k-means cluster analysis of principal component variables was used to define phenotypic clusters within a cohort of 6- to 20-year-old urban and largely minority subjects. RESULTS: Among the 154 subjects, phenotypic cluster analysis defined three independent clusters (Cluster 1 [n = 57]; Cluster 2 [n = 33]; Cluster 3 [n = 58]). A small fourth cluster (n = 6) was excluded. Patients in Cluster 1 were predominantly males, with a relative abundance of neutrophils in their nasal washes. Patients in Cluster 2 were predominantly females with high body mass index percentiles and later-onset asthma. Patients in Cluster 3 had higher eosinophil counts in their nasal washes and lower Asthma Control Test (ACT) scores. Within-cluster regression analysis revealed several significant associations between ETS exposure and phenotypic characteristics that were not present in the overall cohort. ETS exposure was associated with a significant increase in nasal wash neutrophils (beta coefficient = 0.73 [95% confidence interval, CI: 0.11 to 1.35]; p = .023) and a significant decrease in ACT score (-5.17 [-8.42 to -1.93]; p = .003) within Cluster 1 and a significant reduction in the bronchodilator-induced % change in forced expiratory volume in one second (FEV(1)) (-36.32 [-62.18 to -10.46]; p = .009) within Cluster 3. CONCLUSIONS: Clustering techniques defined more homogeneous subgroups, allowing for the detection of otherwise undetectable associations between environmental tobacco smoke exposure and asthma characteristics.


Asunto(s)
Asma/etiología , Negro o Afroamericano , Contaminación por Humo de Tabaco/efectos adversos , Adolescente , Adulto , Asma/etnología , Niño , Análisis por Conglomerados , Estudios de Cohortes , Femenino , Volumen Espiratorio Forzado , Humanos , Masculino , Fenotipo , Análisis de Componente Principal
17.
Health Psychol ; 39(12): 1109-1124, 2020 Dec.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32940529

RESUMEN

Objective: Appetitive risk behaviors (ARB), including tobacco use, alcohol consumption, consumption of calorie dense/nutrient-poor foods, and sexual risk behavior contribute substantially to morbidity and mortality. Affective states that arise from a wide array of unrelated circumstances (i.e., incidental affect) may carry over to influence ARB. A meta-analysis is needed to systematically examine causal evidence for the role of incidental affect (including specific emotions) in influencing ARB. Method: Integrating effect sizes from 91 published and unpublished experimental studies that include both an incidental-affect induction and neutral-control condition (k = 271 effect sizes: k = 183 negative affect, k = 78 positive affect), this meta-analysis examines how negative and positive affective states influenced ARB and related health cognitions (e.g., intentions, evaluations, craving, perceived control). Results: Negative affective states reliably increased ARB, in analyses where all negative affective states were analyzed (d = .29) and in stratified analyses of just negative mood (d = .30) and stress (d = .48). These effects were stronger among study populations coded as clinically at risk. Positive affective states generally did not influence ARB or related health cognitions, except in the presence of a craving cue. Design issues of extant literature largely precluded conclusions about the effects of specific positive and negative affective states. Conclusion: Taken together, findings suggest the importance of strategies to attenuate negative affect incidental to ARB to facilitate healthier behavioral patterns, especially among clinically at-risk individuals. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2020 APA, all rights reserved).


Asunto(s)
Afecto/fisiología , Apetito/fisiología , Conductas de Riesgo para la Salud/fisiología , Asunción de Riesgos , Femenino , Humanos , Incidencia , Masculino
18.
J Pain Symptom Manage ; 57(6): 1151-1165, 2019 06.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30794937

RESUMEN

CONTEXT: Being diagnosed with cancer often forces patients and families to make difficult medical decisions. How patients think they and others will feel in the future, termed affective predictions, may influence these decisions. These affective predictions are often biased, which may contribute to suboptimal care outcomes by influencing decisions related to palliative care and advance care planning. OBJECTIVES: This study aimed to translate perspectives from the decision sciences to inform future research about when and how affective predictions may influence decisions about palliative care and advance care planning. METHODS: A systematic search of two databases to evaluate the extent to which affective predictions have been examined in the palliative care and advance care planning context yielded 35 relevant articles. Over half utilized qualitative methodologies (n = 21). Most studies were conducted in the U.S. (n = 12), Canada (n = 7), or European countries (n = 10). Study contexts included end of life (n = 10), early treatment decisions (n = 10), pain and symptom management (n = 7), and patient-provider communication (n = 6). The affective processes of patients (n = 20), caregivers (n = 16), and/or providers (n = 12) were examined. RESULTS: Three features of the palliative care and advance care planning context may contribute to biased affective predictions: 1) early treatment decisions are made under heightened emotional states and with insufficient information; 2) palliative care decisions influence life domains beyond physical health; and 3) palliative care decisions involve multiple people. CONCLUSION: Biases in affective predictions may serve as a barrier to optimal palliative care delivery. Predictions are complicated by intense emotions, inadequate prognostic information, involvement of many individuals, and cancer's effect on non-health life domains. Applying decision science frameworks may generate insights about affective predictions that can be harnessed to solve challenges associated with optimal delivery of palliative care.


Asunto(s)
Planificación Anticipada de Atención/organización & administración , Afecto , Modelos Organizacionales , Cuidados Paliativos/organización & administración , Predicción , Humanos , Neoplasias/terapia
19.
PLoS One ; 13(6): e0199433, 2018.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29949645

RESUMEN

Sadness increases how much decision makers pay to acquire goods, even when decision makers are unaware of it. This effect is coined the "misery-is-not-miserly effect". The paper that first established this effect is the second most-cited article appearing in Psychological Science in 2004. In light of its impact, the present study sought to assess whether the misery-is-not-miserly effect would replicate (a) in a novel context and (b) even when another way of alleviating a sense of loss (i.e., compensatory consumption) was available. Results revealed that the effect replicated in the novel context and, despite a prediction otherwise, even when individuals had an opportunity to engage in compensatory consumption. Moreover, a meta-analysis of the original effect and that observed in the present study yielded a small-to-medium effect (Cohen's d = 0.43). As such, the present study lends evidentiary support to the misery-is-not-miserly effect and provides impetus for future research exploring the impact of sadness on consumer decision-making, specifically, and of emotion on decision processes, more generally.


Asunto(s)
Toma de Decisiones , Psicología , Adulto , Análisis de Varianza , Comportamiento del Consumidor , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Metaanálisis como Asunto , Psicología/métodos , Adulto Joven
20.
Addict Behav ; 84: 53-56, 2018 09.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29626792

RESUMEN

Alcohol consumption among young adult college students represents a significant public health problem. The presence of alcohol-related cues in drinkers' environments can trigger powerful alcohol cravings, which may influence drinking outcomes. Less is known about how this cue-induced craving influences behavioral economic demand for alcohol. In addition, research has suggested that trait mindfulness may be an important buffer of the effects of internal states of craving on drinking decisions. Based on this literature, we hypothesized that cue-induced cravings would be associated with increased alcohol demand, an effect that would be attenuated among drinkers who have higher levels of mindfulness. Young adult college student drinkers (n = 69) completed a laboratory-based cue-induced craving assessment, a self-report assessment of trait mindfulness, and an alcohol purchase task. Findings revealed that cue-induced craving was related to higher alcohol demand. Consistent with the study hypothesis, acceptance, a component of mindfulness, buffered the effects of cue-induced craving on alcohol demand. Results raise the possibility that mindfulness-based interventions may be useful in helping disrupt the link between internal states of craving and drinking decisions in young adult college student drinkers.


Asunto(s)
Consumo de Alcohol en la Universidad/psicología , Ansia , Señales (Psicología) , Atención Plena , Consumo de Bebidas Alcohólicas , Economía del Comportamiento , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Adulto Joven
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA