Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 70
Filtrar
Más filtros

Banco de datos
Tipo del documento
Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
Nature ; 630(8017): 575-586, 2024 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38898296

RESUMEN

Language is a defining characteristic of our species, but the function, or functions, that it serves has been debated for centuries. Here we bring recent evidence from neuroscience and allied disciplines to argue that in modern humans, language is a tool for communication, contrary to a prominent view that we use language for thinking. We begin by introducing the brain network that supports linguistic ability in humans. We then review evidence for a double dissociation between language and thought, and discuss several properties of language that suggest that it is optimized for communication. We conclude that although the emergence of language has unquestionably transformed human culture, language does not appear to be a prerequisite for complex thought, including symbolic thought. Instead, language is a powerful tool for the transmission of cultural knowledge; it plausibly co-evolved with our thinking and reasoning capacities, and only reflects, rather than gives rise to, the signature sophistication of human cognition.


Asunto(s)
Encéfalo , Cognición , Comunicación , Lenguaje , Pensamiento , Animales , Humanos , Encéfalo/fisiología , Cognición/fisiología , Cultura , Pensamiento/fisiología , Lingüística
2.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 120(35): e2215999120, 2023 08 29.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37603761

RESUMEN

We investigate number and arithmetic learning among a Bolivian indigenous people, the Tsimane', for whom formal schooling is comparatively recent in history and variable in both extent and consistency. We first present a large-scale meta-analysis on child number development involving over 800 Tsimane' children. The results emphasize the impact of formal schooling: Children are only found to be full counters when they have attended school, suggesting the importance of cultural support for early mathematics. We then test especially remote Tsimane' communities and document the development of specialized arithmetical knowledge in the absence of direct formal education. Specifically, we describe individuals who succeed on arithmetic problems involving the number five-which has a distinct role in the local economy-even though they do not succeed on some lower numbers. Some of these participants can perform multiplication with fives at greater accuracy than addition by one. These results highlight the importance of cultural factors in early mathematics and suggest that psychological theories of number where quantities are derived from lower numbers via repeated addition (e.g., a successor function) are unlikely to explain the diversity of human mathematical ability.


Asunto(s)
Desarrollo Infantil , Cognición , Niño , Humanos , Bolivia , Pueblos Indígenas , Conocimiento
3.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 119(5)2022 02 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35074868

RESUMEN

A major goal of linguistics and cognitive science is to understand what class of learning systems can acquire natural language. Until recently, the computational requirements of language have been used to argue that learning is impossible without a highly constrained hypothesis space. Here, we describe a learning system that is maximally unconstrained, operating over the space of all computations, and is able to acquire many of the key structures present in natural language from positive evidence alone. We demonstrate this by providing the same learning model with data from 74 distinct formal languages which have been argued to capture key features of language, have been studied in experimental work, or come from an interesting complexity class. The model is able to successfully induce the latent system generating the observed strings from small amounts of evidence in almost all cases, including for regular (e.g., an , [Formula: see text], and [Formula: see text]), context-free (e.g., [Formula: see text], and [Formula: see text]), and context-sensitive (e.g., [Formula: see text], and xx) languages, as well as for many languages studied in learning experiments. These results show that relatively small amounts of positive evidence can support learning of rich classes of generative computations over structures. The model provides an idealized learning setup upon which additional cognitive constraints and biases can be formalized.


Asunto(s)
Aprendizaje/fisiología , Lingüística/métodos , Humanos , Lenguaje
4.
Eur J Neurosci ; 2024 Jun 10.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38858853

RESUMEN

We develop a mathematical approach to formally proving that certain neural computations and representations exist based on patterns observed in an organism's behaviour. To illustrate, we provide a simple set of conditions under which an ant's ability to determine how far it is from its nest would logically imply neural structures isomorphic to the natural numbers ℕ $$ \mathrm{\mathbb{N}} $$ . We generalise these results to arbitrary behaviours and representations and show what mathematical characterisation of neural computation and representation is simplest while being maximally predictive of behaviour. We develop this framework in detail using a path integration example, where an organism's ability to search for its nest in the correct location implies representational structures isomorphic to two-dimensional coordinates under addition. We also study a system for processing a n b n $$ {a}^n{b}^n $$ strings common in comparative work. Our approach provides an objective way to determine what theory of a physical system is best, addressing a fundamental challenge in neuroscientific inference. These results motivate considering which neurobiological structures have the requisite formal structure and are otherwise physically plausible given relevant physical considerations such as generalisability, information density, thermodynamic stability and energetic cost.

5.
Child Dev ; 94(6): 1472-1490, 2023.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37984061

RESUMEN

The study of how children learn numbers has yielded one of the most productive research programs in cognitive development, spanning empirical and computational methods, as well as nativist and empiricist philosophies. This paper provides a tutorial on how to think computationally about learning models in a domain like number, where learners take finite data and go far beyond what they directly observe or perceive. To illustrate, this paper then outlines a model which acquires a counting procedure using observations of sets and words, extending the proposal of Piantadosi et al. (2012). This new version of the model responds to several critiques of the original work and outlines an approach which is likely appropriate for acquiring further aspects of mathematics.


Asunto(s)
Cognición , Aprendizaje , Niño , Humanos , Matemática
6.
Psychol Sci ; 33(3): 371-381, 2022 03.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35132893

RESUMEN

Previous findings suggest that mentally representing exact numbers larger than four depends on a verbal count routine (e.g., "one, two, three . . ."). However, these findings are controversial because they rely on comparisons across radically different languages and cultures. We tested the role of language in number concepts within a single population-the Tsimane' of Bolivia-in which knowledge of number words varies across individual adults. We used a novel data-analysis model to quantify the point at which participants (N = 30) switched from exact to approximate number representations during a simple numerical matching task. The results show that these behavioral switch points were bounded by participants' verbal count ranges; their representations of exact cardinalities were limited to the number words they knew. Beyond that range, they resorted to numerical approximation. These results resolve competing accounts of previous findings and provide unambiguous evidence that large exact number concepts are enabled by language.


Asunto(s)
Cognición , Lenguaje , Adulto , Humanos , Desarrollo del Lenguaje , Matemática
7.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 116(36): 17729-17734, 2019 09 03.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31427541

RESUMEN

The approximate number system (ANS) has attracted broad interest due to its potential importance in early mathematical development and the fact that it is conserved across species. Models of the ANS and behavioral measures of ANS acuity both assume that quantity estimation is computed rapidly and in parallel across an entire view of the visual scene. We present evidence instead that ANS estimates are largely the product of a serial accumulation mechanism operating across visual fixations. We used an eye-tracker to collect data on participants' visual fixations while they performed quantity-estimation and -discrimination tasks. We were able to predict participants' numerical estimates using their visual fixation data: As the number of dots fixated increased, mean estimates also increased, and estimation error decreased. A detailed model-based analysis shows that fixated dots contribute twice as much as peripheral dots to estimated quantities; people do not "double count" multiply fixated dots; and they do not adjust for the proportion of area in the scene that they have fixated. The accumulation mechanism we propose explains reported effects of display time on estimation and earlier findings of a bias to underestimate quantities.


Asunto(s)
Movimientos Oculares/fisiología , Fóvea Central/fisiología , Modelos Neurológicos , Visión Ocular/fisiología , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Matemática
8.
Lang Resour Eval ; 55(1): 63-77, 2021.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34720781

RESUMEN

It is now a common practice to compare models of human language processing by comparing how well they predict behavioral and neural measures of processing difficulty, such as reading times, on corpora of rich naturalistic linguistic materials. However, many of these corpora, which are based on naturally-occurring text, do not contain many of the low-frequency syntactic constructions that are often required to distinguish between processing theories. Here we describe a new corpus consisting of English texts edited to contain many low-frequency syntactic constructions while still sounding fluent to native speakers. The corpus is annotated with hand-corrected Penn Treebank-style parse trees and includes self-paced reading time data and aligned audio recordings. We give an overview of the content of the corpus, review recent work using the corpus, and release the data.

11.
12.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 114(40): 10785-10790, 2017 10 03.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28923921

RESUMEN

What determines how languages categorize colors? We analyzed results of the World Color Survey (WCS) of 110 languages to show that despite gross differences across languages, communication of chromatic chips is always better for warm colors (yellows/reds) than cool colors (blues/greens). We present an analysis of color statistics in a large databank of natural images curated by human observers for salient objects and show that objects tend to have warm rather than cool colors. These results suggest that the cross-linguistic similarity in color-naming efficiency reflects colors of universal usefulness and provide an account of a principle (color use) that governs how color categories come about. We show that potential methodological issues with the WCS do not corrupt information-theoretic analyses, by collecting original data using two extreme versions of the color-naming task, in three groups: the Tsimane', a remote Amazonian hunter-gatherer isolate; Bolivian-Spanish speakers; and English speakers. These data also enabled us to test another prediction of the color-usefulness hypothesis: that differences in color categorization between languages are caused by differences in overall usefulness of color to a culture. In support, we found that color naming among Tsimane' had relatively low communicative efficiency, and the Tsimane' were less likely to use color terms when describing familiar objects. Color-naming among Tsimane' was boosted when naming artificially colored objects compared with natural objects, suggesting that industrialization promotes color usefulness.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Color , Color/normas , Comparación Transcultural , Lenguaje , Adolescente , Adulto , Anciano , Conducta de Elección , Bases de Datos Factuales , Discriminación en Psicología , Femenino , Humanos , Lingüística , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Encuestas y Cuestionarios , Adulto Joven
13.
Cereb Cortex ; 28(11): 3965-3975, 2018 11 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29040494

RESUMEN

Humans naturally group the world into coherent categories defined by membership rules. Rules can be learned implicitly by building stimulus-response associations using reinforcement learning or by using explicit reasoning. We tested if the striatum, in which activation reliably scales with reward prediction error, would track prediction errors in a task that required explicit rule generation. Using functional magnetic resonance imaging during a categorization task, we show that striatal responses to feedback scale with a "surprise" signal derived from a Bayesian rule-learning model and are inconsistent with RL prediction error. We also find that striatum and caudal inferior frontal sulcus (cIFS) are involved in updating the likelihood of discriminative rules. We conclude that the striatum, in cooperation with the cIFS, is involved in updating the values assigned to categorization rules when people learn using explicit reasoning.


Asunto(s)
Aprendizaje por Asociación , Cuerpo Estriado/fisiología , Recompensa , Adulto , Teorema de Bayes , Mapeo Encefálico , Femenino , Humanos , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Masculino , Modelos Neurológicos , Modelos Psicológicos , Adulto Joven
14.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 113(25): 6874-9, 2016 06 21.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27217560

RESUMEN

We present evidence that pressures for early childcare may have been one of the driving factors of human evolution. We show through an evolutionary model that runaway selection for high intelligence may occur when (i) altricial neonates require intelligent parents, (ii) intelligent parents must have large brains, and (iii) large brains necessitate having even more altricial offspring. We test a prediction of this account by showing across primate genera that the helplessness of infants is a particularly strong predictor of the adults' intelligence. We discuss related implications, including this account's ability to explain why human-level intelligence evolved specifically in mammals. This theory complements prior hypotheses that link human intelligence to social reasoning and reproductive pressures and explains how human intelligence may have become so distinctive compared with our closest evolutionary relatives.


Asunto(s)
Evolución Biológica , Inteligencia , Animales , Encéfalo , Humanos , Lactante , Modelos Teóricos , Primates
16.
J Neurophysiol ; 119(4): 1305-1318, 2018 04 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29212924

RESUMEN

Classification of neurons into clusters based on their response properties is an important tool for gaining insight into neural computations. However, it remains unclear to what extent neurons fall naturally into discrete functional categories. We developed a Bayesian method that models the tuning properties of neural populations as a mixture of multiple types of task-relevant response patterns. We applied this method to data from several cortical and striatal regions in economic choice tasks. In all cases, neurons fell into only two clusters: one multiple-selectivity cluster containing all cells driven by task variables of interest and another of no selectivity for those variables. The single cluster of task-sensitive cells argues against robust categorical tuning in these areas. The no-selectivity cluster was unanticipated and raises important questions about what distinguishes these neurons and what role they play. Moreover, the ability to formally identify these nonselective cells allows for more accurate measurement of ensemble effects by excluding or appropriately down-weighting them in analysis. Our findings provide a valuable tool for analysis of neural data, challenge simple categorization schemes previously proposed for these regions, and place useful constraints on neurocomputational models of economic choice and control. NEW & NOTEWORTHY We present a Bayesian method for formally detecting whether a population of neurons can be naturally classified into clusters based on their response tuning properties. We then examine several data sets of reward system neurons for variables and find in all cases that neurons can be classified into only two categories: a functional class and a non-task-driven class. These results provide important constraints for neural models of the reward system.


Asunto(s)
Conducta de Elección/fisiología , Modelos Neurológicos , Neuronas/clasificación , Neuronas/fisiología , Corteza Prefrontal/fisiología , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Recompensa , Animales , Teorema de Bayes , Conducta Animal/fisiología , Medidas del Movimiento Ocular , Macaca mulatta , Masculino , Asunción de Riesgos
17.
Am J Hum Biol ; 30(1)2018 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28901592

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVES: We assessed associations between child stunting, recovery, and faltering with schooling and human capital skills in a native Amazonian society of horticulturalists-foragers (Tsimane'). METHODS: We used cross-sectional data (2008) from 1262 children aged 6 to 16 years in 53 villages to assess contemporaneous associations between three height categories: stunted (height-for-age Z score, HAZ<-2), moderately stunted (-2 ≤ HAZ≤-1), and nonstunted (HAZ>-1), and three categories of human capital: completed grades of schooling, test-based academic skills (math, reading, writing), and local plant knowledge. We used annual longitudinal data (2002-2010) from all children (n = 853) in 13 villages to estimate the association between changes in height categories between the first and last years of measure and schooling and academic skills. RESULTS: Stunting was associated with 0.4 fewer completed grades of schooling (∼24% less) and with 13-15% lower probability of showing any writing or math skills. Moderate stunting was associated with ∼20% lower scores in local plant knowledge and 9% lower probability of showing writing skills, but was not associated with schooling or math and writing skills. Compared with nonstunted children, children who became stunted had 18-21% and 15-21% lower probabilities of showing math and writing skills, and stunted children had 0.4 fewer completed grades of schooling. Stunted children who recovered showed human capital outcomes that were indistinguishable from nonstunted children. CONCLUSIONS: The results confirm adverse associations between child stunting and human capital skills. Predictors of growth recovery and faltering can affect human capital outcomes, even in a remote, economically self-sufficient society.


Asunto(s)
Desarrollo del Adolescente , Desarrollo Infantil , Trastornos del Crecimiento/economía , Indígenas Sudamericanos/estadística & datos numéricos , Adolescente , Bolivia/epidemiología , Niño , Estudios Transversales , Femenino , Trastornos del Crecimiento/epidemiología , Humanos , Masculino
18.
Ann Hum Biol ; 45(4): 299-313, 2018 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30328382

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Seasons affect many social, economic, and biological outcomes, particularly in low-resource settings, and some studies suggest that birth season affects child growth. AIM: To study a predictor of stunting that has received limited attention: birth season. SUBJECTS AND METHODS: This study uses cross-sectional data collected during 2008 in a low-resource society of horticulturists-foragers in the Bolivian Amazon, Tsimane'. It estimates the associations between birth months and height-for-age Z-scores (HAZ) for 562 girls and 546 boys separately, from birth until age 11 years or pre-puberty, which in this society occurs ∼13-14 years. RESULTS: Children born during the rainy season (February-May) were shorter, while children born during the end of the dry season and the start of the rainy season (August-November) were taller, both compared with their age-sex peers born during the rest of the year. The correlations of birth season with HAZ were stronger for boys than for girls. Controlling for birth season, there is some evidence of eventual partial catch-up growth, with the HAZ of girls or boys worsening until ∼ age 4-5 years, but improving thereafter. By age 6 years, many girls and boys had ceased to be stunted, irrespective of birth season. CONCLUSION: The results suggest that redressing stunting will require attention to conditions in utero, infancy and late childhood.


Asunto(s)
Estatura , Desarrollo Infantil , Trastornos del Crecimiento/epidemiología , Indígenas Sudamericanos/estadística & datos numéricos , Bolivia/epidemiología , Niño , Preescolar , Estudios Transversales , Femenino , Trastornos del Crecimiento/etiología , Humanos , Lactante , Recién Nacido , Masculino , Parto , Pubertad , Estaciones del Año
19.
Infancy ; 23(3): 310-324, 2018.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32884496

RESUMEN

Complex systems are often built from a relatively small set of basic features or operations that can be combined in myriad ways. We investigated the developmental origins of this compositional architecture in 9-month-old infants, extending recent work that demonstrated rudimentary compositional abilities in preschoolers. Infants viewed two separate object-occlusion events that depicted a single featurechange operation. They were then tested with a combined operation to determine whether they expected the outcome of the two feature changes, even though this combination was unfamiliar. In contrast to preschoolers, infants did not appear to predictively compose these simple feature-change operations. A second experiment demonstrated the ability of infants to track two operations when not combined. The failure to compose basic operations is consistent with limitations on object tracking and early numerical cognition (Feigenson & Yamaguchi, 2009). We suggest that these results can be unified via a general principle: infants have difficulty with multiple updates to a representation of an unobservable.

20.
Psychol Sci ; 28(4): 462-469, 2017 04.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28406373

RESUMEN

Cognitive and neural research over the past few decades has produced sophisticated models of the representations and algorithms underlying numerical reasoning in humans and other animals. These models make precise predictions for how humans and other animals should behave when faced with quantitative decisions, yet primarily have been tested only in laboratory tasks. We used data from wild baboons' troop movements recently reported by Strandburg-Peshkin, Farine, Couzin, and Crofoot (2015) to compare a variety of models of quantitative decision making. We found that the decisions made by these naturally behaving wild animals rely specifically on numerical representations that have key homologies with the psychophysics of human number representations. These findings provide important new data on the types of problems human numerical cognition was designed to solve and constitute the first robust evidence of true numerical reasoning in wild animals.


Asunto(s)
Conducta Animal/fisiología , Cognición/fisiología , Toma de Decisiones/fisiología , Conceptos Matemáticos , Papio/fisiología , Animales
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA