Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 51
Filtrar
Más filtros

Banco de datos
Tipo del documento
Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
Infancy ; 25(5): 593-617, 2020 09.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32857444

RESUMEN

In contrast to the anecdotal claim that "male infants like cars and female infants like dolls," previous studies have reported mixed findings for gender-related toy preferences in infancy. In Experiment 1, we explored the emergence of gender-related preferences using face-car pairs (Experiment 1a, n = 51, 6-20 months) or face-stove pairs (Experiment 1b, n = 54, 6-20 months). In Experiment 2 (n = 42, 14-16 months), we explore the effect of toy properties, infants' past toy exposure, activity levels, and parental attitudes on such preferences using a wider range of toys. For both studies, infants demonstrated a general preference for faced stimuli over other objects, except for male infants who showed no preference between dolls and cars at around 15 months. Infants' prior experience participating in motor-intensive activities, with wheeled toys and parental attitudes appeared to relate to female infants' preferences for dynamic toys. These results indicate a range of factors influence gendered toy preferences and suggest that nurture plays an important role.


Asunto(s)
Desarrollo Infantil/fisiología , Conducta de Elección/fisiología , Conducta del Lactante/fisiología , Responsabilidad Parental , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología , Factores de Edad , Actitud , Reconocimiento Facial/fisiología , Femenino , Humanos , Lactante , Masculino , Juego e Implementos de Juego , Factores Sexuales
2.
Am Fam Physician ; 99(2): 88-94, 2019 01 15.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30633480

RESUMEN

Patellofemoral pain syndrome (PFPS) is one of the most common causes of anterior knee pain encountered in the outpatient setting in adolescents and adults younger than 60 years. The incidence in the United States is between 3% and 6%. The cardinal feature of PFPS is pain in or around the anterior knee that intensifies when the knee is flexed during weight-bearing activities. The pain of PFPS often worsens with prolonged sitting or descending stairs. The most sensitive physical examination finding is pain with squatting. Examining a patient's gait, posture, and footwear can help identify contributing causes. Plain radiographs of the knee are not necessary for the diagnosis of PFPS but can exclude other diagnoses, such as osteoarthritis, patellar fracture, and osteochondritis. If conservative treatment measures are unsuccessful, plain radiography is recommended. Treatment of PFPS includes rest, a short course of nonsteroidal anti-inflammatory drugs, and physical therapy directed at strengthening the hip flexor, trunk, and knee muscle groups. Patellar kinesiotaping may provide additional short-term pain relief; however, evidence is insufficient to support its routine use. Surgery is considered a last resort.


Asunto(s)
Articulación de la Rodilla/anatomía & histología , Síndrome de Dolor Patelofemoral/diagnóstico , Síndrome de Dolor Patelofemoral/terapia , Adolescente , Adulto , Diagnóstico Diferencial , Femenino , Humanos , Articulación de la Rodilla/fisiopatología , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Factores de Riesgo , Adulto Joven
3.
J Vis ; 19(6): 18, 2019 06 03.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31215978

RESUMEN

Previous studies of age-related macular degeneration (AMD) report impaired facial expression recognition even with enlarged face images. Here, we test potential benefits of caricaturing (exaggerating how the expression's shape differs from neutral) as an image enhancement procedure targeted at mid- to high-level cortical vision. Experiment 1 provides proof-of-concept using normal vision observers shown blurred images as a partial simulation of AMD. Caricaturing significantly improved expression recognition (happy, sad, anger, disgust, fear, surprise) by ∼4%-5% across young adults and older adults (mean age 73 years); two different severities of blur; high, medium, and low intensity of the original expression; and all intermediate accuracy levels (impaired but still above chance). Experiment 2 tested AMD patients, running 19 eyes monocularly (from 12 patients, 67-94 years) covering a wide range of vision loss (acuities 6/7.5 to poorer than 6/360). With faces pre-enlarged, recognition approached ceiling and was only slightly worse than matched controls for high- and medium-intensity expressions. For low-intensity expressions, recognition of veridical expressions remained impaired and was significantly improved with caricaturing across all levels of vision loss by 5.8%. Overall, caricaturing benefits emerged when improvement was most needed, that is, when initial recognition of uncaricatured expressions was impaired.


Asunto(s)
Emociones/fisiología , Reconocimiento Facial/fisiología , Degeneración Macular/fisiopatología , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología , Adulto , Anciano , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Expresión Facial , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Adulto Joven
5.
Cogn Neuropsychol ; 34(5): 253-268, 2017 07.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28906173

RESUMEN

The Cambridge Face Memory Test (CFMT) is widely accepted as providing a valid and reliable tool in diagnosing prosopagnosia (inability to recognize people's faces). Previously, large-sample norms have been available only for Caucasian-face versions, suitable for diagnosis in Caucasian observers. These are invalid for observers of different races due to potentially severe other-race effects. Here, we provide large-sample norms (N = 306) for East Asian observers on an Asian-face version (CFMT-Chinese). We also demonstrate methodological suitability of the CFMT-Chinese for prosopagnosia diagnosis (high internal reliability, approximately normal distribution, norm-score range sufficiently far above chance). Additional findings were a female advantage on mean performance, plus a difference between participants living in the East (China) or the West (international students, second-generation children of immigrants), which we suggest might reflect personality differences associated with willingness to emigrate. Finally, we demonstrate suitability of the CFMT-Chinese for individual differences studies that use correlations within the normal range.


Asunto(s)
Pueblo Asiatico , Reconocimiento Facial , Prosopagnosia/diagnóstico , Reconocimiento en Psicología , Adolescente , Adulto , China , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Valores de Referencia , Reproducibilidad de los Resultados , Población Blanca , Adulto Joven
6.
Can Vet J ; 58(10): 1100-1104, 2017 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28966361

RESUMEN

This report describes hypocholesterolemia in 3 dogs with nonregenerative, suspected immune-mediated anemias. Common causes of hypocholesterolemia were ruled out, raising suspicion for a mechanistic link between anemia and hypocholesterolemia in dogs. As observed in humans with concurrent anemia and hypocholesterolemia, cholesterol concentrations increased to within the reference interval once the dogs' anemia resolved.


Hypocholestérolémie et anémie non régénérative suspectée d'origine immunitaire : rapport de 3 cas canins. Ce rapport décrit l'hypocholestérolémie chez trois chiens atteints d'une anémie non régénérative suspectée d'origine immunitaire. Les causes communes d'hypocholestérolémie ont été écartées, soulevant des doutes pour un lien mécanistique entre l'anémie et l'hypocholestérolémie chez les chiens. Tel qu'il a été observé chez les humains atteints d'anémie et d'hypocholestérolémie concomitante, les concentrations de cholestérol ont augmenté dans l'intervalle de référence une fois que l'anémie des chiens s'est résorbée.(Traduit par Isabelle Vallières).


Asunto(s)
Anemia/veterinaria , Colesterol/sangre , Anemia/sangre , Animales , Enfermedad Crónica , Perros , Femenino , Masculino , Valores de Referencia
7.
J Cogn Neurosci ; 27(3): 614-22, 2015 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25313655

RESUMEN

Human extrastriate cortex contains functional regions that are selective for particular categories such as faces, bodies, and places, but it is unclear whether these category-selective regions are necessary for normal perception of their preferred stimuli. One of these regions is the right fusiform body area (FBA), which is selectively involved in body perception. Do loss of the right fusiform gyrus and the absence of the right FBA necessarily lead to deficits in body perception? Here we report the performance of Galen, a brain-damaged patient who lost the right fusiform gyrus and has no right FBA, on eight tasks of body perception. Despite his lesion, Galen showed normal performance on all tasks. Galen's results demonstrate that damage to the right fusiform gyrus and the lack of the right FBA do not necessarily lead to persisting deficits in body perception.


Asunto(s)
Cuerpo Humano , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología , Lóbulo Temporal/patología , Lóbulo Temporal/fisiología , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Masculino , Adulto Joven
8.
Am Nat ; 185(4): 562-70, 2015 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25811089

RESUMEN

There is no conclusive evidence of any nonhuman animal using the sun as part of its predation strategy. Here, we show that the world's largest predatory fish-the white shark (Carcharodon carcharias)-exploits the sun when approaching baits by positioning the sun directly behind them. On sunny days, sharks reversed their direction of approach along an east-west axis from morning to afternoon but had uniformly distributed approach directions during overcast conditions. These results show that white sharks have sufficient behavioral flexibility to exploit fluctuating environmental features when predating. This sun-tracking predation strategy has a number of potential functional roles, including improvement of prey detection, avoidance of retinal overstimulation, and predator concealment.


Asunto(s)
Conducta Predatoria , Tiburones/fisiología , Animales , Orientación , Sistema Solar , Conducta Espacial
9.
J Exp Child Psychol ; 138: 1-14, 2015 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26002490

RESUMEN

Children have been shown to be worse at face recognition than adults even into their early teens. However, there is debate about whether this is due to face-specific mechanisms or general perceptual and memory development. Here, we considered a slightly different option--that children use different cues to recognition. To test this, we showed 8-year-olds, 10-year-olds, and adults whole body, head only, and body only stimuli that were either moving or static. These were shown in two tasks, a match-to-sample task with unfamiliar people and a learning task, to test recognition of experimentally familiar people. On the match-to-sample task, children were worse overall, but the pattern of results was the same for each age group. Matching was best with all cues or head available, and there was no effect of movement. However, matching was generally slower with moving stimuli, and 8-year-olds, but not 10-year-olds, were slower than adults. In general, more cues were faster than heads or bodies alone, but 8-year-olds were surprisingly slow when still bodies were shown alone. On the learning task, again all age groups showed similar patterns, with better performance for all cues. Both 8- and 10-year-olds were more likely to say that they knew someone unfamiliar. Again, movement did not provide a clear advantage. Overall, this study suggests that any differences in face recognition between adults and children are not due to differences in cue use and that instead these results are consistent with general improvements in memory.


Asunto(s)
Desarrollo Infantil/fisiología , Señales (Psicología) , Reconocimiento Facial/fisiología , Reconocimiento en Psicología/fisiología , Adolescente , Adulto , Niño , Femenino , Cabeza , Humanos , Masculino , Movimiento/fisiología , Torso , Adulto Joven
11.
J Exp Child Psychol ; 126: 103-11, 2014 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24907631

RESUMEN

Performance on laboratory face tasks improves across childhood, not reaching adult levels until adolescence. Debate surrounds the source of this development, with recent reviews suggesting that underlying face processing mechanisms are mature early in childhood and that the improvement seen on experimental tasks instead results from general cognitive/perceptual development. One face processing mechanism that has been argued to develop slowly is the ability to encode faces in a view-invariant manner (i.e., allowing recognition across changes in viewpoint). However, many previous studies have not controlled for general cognitive factors. In the current study, 8-year-olds and adults performed a recognition memory task with two study-test viewpoint conditions: same view (study front view, test front view) and change view (study front view, test three-quarter view). To allow quantitative comparison between children and adults, performance in the same view condition was matched across the groups by increasing the learning set size for adults. Results showed poorer memory in the change view condition than in the same view condition for both adults and children. Importantly, there was no quantitative difference between children and adults in the size of decrement in memory performance resulting from a change in viewpoint. This finding adds to growing evidence that face processing mechanisms are mature early in childhood.


Asunto(s)
Desarrollo Infantil , Cara , Reconocimiento en Psicología , Adolescente , Adulto , Niño , Femenino , Historia Antigua , Humanos , Estimulación Luminosa , Adulto Joven
12.
Mem Cognit ; 42(5): 755-67, 2014 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24554278

RESUMEN

Dance-like actions are complex visual stimuli involving multiple changes in body posture across time and space. Visual perception research has demonstrated a difference between the processing of dynamic body movement and the processing of static body posture. Yet, it is unclear whether this processing dissociation continues during the retention of body movement and body form in visual working memory (VWM). When observing a dance-like action, it is likely that static snapshot images of body posture will be retained alongside dynamic images of the complete motion. Therefore, we hypothesized that, as in perception, posture and movement would differ in VWM. Additionally, if body posture and body movement are separable in VWM, as form- and motion-based items, respectively, then differential interference from intervening form and motion tasks should occur during recognition. In two experiments, we examined these hypotheses. In Experiment 1, the recognition of postures and movements was tested in conditions in which the formats of the study and test stimuli matched (movement-study to movement-test, posture-study to posture-test) or mismatched (movement-study to posture-test, posture-study to movement-test). In Experiment 2, the recognition of postures and movements was compared after intervening form and motion tasks. These results indicated that (1) the recognition of body movement based only on posture is possible, but it is significantly poorer than recognition based on the entire movement stimulus, and (2) form-based interference does not impair memory for movements, although motion-based interference does. We concluded that, whereas static posture information is encoded during the observation of dance-like actions, body movement and body posture differ in VWM.


Asunto(s)
Baile/fisiología , Memoria a Corto Plazo/fisiología , Movimiento/fisiología , Postura/fisiología , Reconocimiento en Psicología/fisiología , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Adulto Joven
13.
Dev Psychobiol ; 56(7): 1454-81, 2014 Nov.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25132626

RESUMEN

There are obvious differences between recognizing faces and recognizing spoken words or phonemes that might suggest development of each capability requires different skills. Recognizing faces and perceiving spoken language, however, are in key senses extremely similar endeavors. Both perceptual processes are based on richly variable, yet highly structured input from which the perceiver needs to extract categorically meaningful information. This similarity could be reflected in the perceptual narrowing that occurs within the first year of life in both domains. We take the position that the perceptual and neurocognitive processes by which face and speech recognition develop are based on a set of common principles. One common principle is the importance of systematic variability in the input as a source of information rather than noise. Experience of this variability leads to perceptual tuning to the critical properties that define individual faces or spoken words versus their membership in larger groupings of people and their language communities. We argue that parallels can be drawn directly between the principles responsible for the development of face and spoken language perception.


Asunto(s)
Desarrollo Infantil/fisiología , Discriminación en Psicología/fisiología , Cara , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología , Percepción Social , Percepción del Habla/fisiología , Niño , Humanos , Lactante
14.
MedEdPORTAL ; 20: 11443, 2024.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39268085

RESUMEN

Introduction: Recognizing the need for more opportunities to learn about health equity within military graduate medical education (GME), we developed a resident-led curriculum to introduce these concepts from a military cultural competency lens. The Impact of Racism on Health module focuses on structural racism and health disparities. Methods: This 60-minute module was presented to ear, nose, and throat (ENT) and pediatrics residents and fellows. It includes a case presentation of an adolescent with an asthma exacerbation, a large-group discussion about social determinants of health and structural racism, and a small-group discussion/debrief conceptualizing the case. Results: Thirty pediatrics residents and 15 ENT residents participated in this activity with a 46% and 60% pretest response rate, respectively. A two-sample Mann-Whitney U test showed statistically significant improvement (p = .005) in knowledge related to structural racism between the pretest (M = 0.5, SD = 0.3) and posttest (M = 0.7, SD = 0.1) knowledge assessments with a small effect size (r = 0.4; Z = 2.8). Discussion: We demonstrated that interactive teaching methods can be used to educate military GME trainees on the impact of structural racism on health outcomes for military health care beneficiaries. Understanding the role of structural racism in the context of military health care using curricula that highlight military-specific health disparities is essential to understanding the role of the military physician in systemically addressing health disparities.


Asunto(s)
Curriculum , Educación de Postgrado en Medicina , Equidad en Salud , Internado y Residencia , Personal Militar , Humanos , Internado y Residencia/métodos , Personal Militar/educación , Personal Militar/estadística & datos numéricos , Racismo , Racismo Sistemático
15.
J Exp Child Psychol ; 116(2): 367-79, 2013 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23933180

RESUMEN

Findings of previous studies demonstrate sex-related preferences for toys in 6-month-old infants; boys prefer nonsocial or mechanical toys such as cars, whereas girls prefer social toys such as dolls. Here, we explored the innate versus learned nature of this sex-related preferences using multiple pictures of doll and real faces (of men and women) as well as pictures of toy and real objects (cars and stoves). In total, 48 4- and 5-month-old infants (24 girls and 24 boys) and 48 young adults (24 women and 24 men) saw six trials of all relevant pairs of faces and objects, with each trial containing a different exemplar of a stimulus type. The infant results showed no sex-related preferences; infants preferred faces of men and women regardless of whether they were real or doll faces. Similarly, adults did not show sex-related preferences for social versus nonsocial stimuli, but unlike infants they preferred faces of the opposite sex over objects. These results challenge claims of an innate basis for sex-related preferences for toy real stimuli and suggest that sex-related preferences result from maturational and social development that continues into adulthood.


Asunto(s)
Juego e Implementos de Juego/psicología , Psicología Infantil , Adulto , Factores de Edad , Cara , Femenino , Humanos , Lactante , Masculino , Estimulación Luminosa , Factores Sexuales , Adulto Joven
16.
Mil Med ; 188(11-12): 283-285, 2023 11 03.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37738204

RESUMEN

Transitioning from medical residency to an operational role challenges junior medical officers as their leadership skills are put to the test. In the multifaceted role of Military Medical Corps Officers, patient care remains paramount, and effective leadership hinges on core values. From clinical competence and mentorship to modeling behavior and fostering adaptability, this article underscores the importance of leadership development for junior officers as they transition from the training centers into the operational environment. Effective junior officer leaders become force multipliers, empowering their teams and cultivating future leaders to uphold the values essential to mission success.


Asunto(s)
Liderazgo , Personal Militar , Humanos , Competencia Clínica , Personal de Salud , Predicción
17.
Arthritis Care Res (Hoboken) ; 75(4): 734-742, 2023 04.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35381122

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVE: To determine the quality of published rheumatology-focused continuing professional development (CPD) for primary care clinicians (PCCs) for improving the care of patients with rheumatic diseases. METHODS: The authors conducted a systematic review of CPD focused on rheumatology topics for PCCs. A librarian systematically searched PubMed, Embase, Web of Science, ERIC, the Cumulative Index to Nursing and Allied Health Literature (CINAHL), and Sinico. Studies were limited to those conducted in North America after 1993. An extraction form that included the Medical Education Research Study Quality Instrument and the Kirkpatrick levels of learning outcomes was created through an iterative process and applied to the included articles. RESULTS: In total, 725 articles were retrieved, of which 9 were included. Results showed that CPD was directed more at noninflammatory arthritis than inflammatory arthritis. Autoimmune diseases were underrepresented; 4 studies discussed rheumatoid arthritis, and 1 study examined rheumatologic topics broadly. Newer research tended to include multimodal approaches that combined didactic and active learning strategies, showing an evolution toward more active learning. Although online learning is increasingly popular, interventions were predominantly face-to-face, with only a single example of e-learning. Studies were predominantly of moderate quality. CONCLUSION: Published studies of rheumatology-focused CPD are moving toward more interactive teaching modalities and are typically conducted in person, although virtual options for rheumatology-focused CPD should be explored to improve access to CPD. Autoimmune disease is an uncommon topic in CPD and represents an area for future expansion. Efficacy was difficult to assess given that most of the studies assessed for learner satisfaction, knowledge acquisition, or behavior change, whereas only 1 study focused on patient outcomes.


Asunto(s)
Reumatología , Humanos , Atención Primaria de Salud , América del Norte
18.
Br J Psychol ; 114 Suppl 1: 230-252, 2023 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34010458

RESUMEN

What happens to everyday social interactions when other-race recognition fails? Here, we provide the first formal investigation of this question. We gave East Asian international students (N = 89) a questionnaire concerning their experiences of the other-race effect (ORE) in Australia, and a laboratory test of their objective other-race face recognition deficit using the Cambridge Face Memory Test (CFMT). As a 'perpetrator' of the ORE, participants reported that their problems telling apart Caucasian people contributed significantly to difficulties socializing with them. Moreover, the severity of this problem correlated with their ORE on the CFMT. As a 'victim' of the ORE, participants reported that Caucasians' problems telling them apart also contributed to difficulties socializing. Further, 81% of participants had been confused with other Asians by a Caucasian authority figure (e.g., university tutor, workplace boss), resulting in varying levels of upset/difficulty. When compared to previously established contributors to international students' high rates of social isolation, ORE-related problems were perceived as equally important as the language barrier and only moderately less important than cultural differences. We conclude that the real-world impact of the ORE extends beyond previously identified specialized settings (eyewitness testimony, security), to common everyday situations experienced by all humans.


Asunto(s)
Reconocimiento Facial , Interacción Social , Humanos , Pueblo Asiatico , Australia , Reconocimiento en Psicología , Población Blanca , Pueblos del Este de Asia
19.
Diagnosis (Berl) ; 10(3): 205-214, 2023 08 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37079281

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Medical errors account for up to 440,000 deaths annually, and cognitive errors outpace knowledge deficits as causes of error. Cognitive biases are predispositions to respond in predictable ways, and they don't always result in error. We conducted a scoping review exploring which biases are most prevalent in Internal Medicine (IM), if and how they influence patient outcomes, and what, if any, debiasing strategies are effective. CONTENT: We searched PubMed, OVID, ERIC, SCOPUS, PsychINFO, and CINAHL. Search terms included variations of "bias", "clinical reasoning", and IM subspecialties. Inclusion criteria were: discussing bias, clinical reasoning, and physician participants. SUMMARY: Fifteen of 334 identified papers were included. Two papers looked beyond general IM: one each in Infectious Diseases and Critical Care. Nine papers distinguished bias from error, whereas four referenced error in their definition of bias. The most commonly studied outcomes were diagnosis, treatment, and physician impact in 47 % (7), 33 % (5), and 27 % (4) of studies, respectively. Three studies directly assessed patient outcomes. The most commonly cited biases were availability bias (60 %, 9), confirmation bias (40 %, 6), anchoring (40 %, 6), and premature closure (33 %, 5). Proposed contributing features were years of practice, stressors, and practice setting. One study found that years of practice negatively correlated with susceptibility to bias. Ten studies discussed debiasing; all reported weak or equivocal efficacy. OUTLOOK: We found 41 biases in IM and 22 features that may predispose physicians to bias. We found little evidence directly linking biases to error, which could account for the weak evidence of bias countermeasure efficacy. Future study clearly delineating bias from error and directly assessing clinical outcomes would be insightful.


Asunto(s)
Razonamiento Clínico , Medicina Interna , Humanos , Sesgo , Errores Médicos , Cognición
20.
Dev Sci ; 15(2): 194-203, 2012 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22356175

RESUMEN

We used opposing figural aftereffects to investigate whether there are at least partially separable representations of upright and inverted faces in patients who missed early visual experience because of bilateral congenital cataracts (mean age at test 19.5 years). Visually normal adults and 10-year-olds were tested for comparison. Adults showed the expected opposing aftereffects for upright and inverted faces. Ten-year-olds showed an adultlike aftereffect for upright faces but, unlike the adult group, no aftereffect for inverted faces. Patients failed to show an aftereffect for either upright or inverted faces. Overall, the results suggest that early visual input is necessary for the later development of (at least partially) separable representations of upright and inverted faces, a developmental process that takes many years to reach an adult-like refinement.


Asunto(s)
Desarrollo Infantil/fisiología , Cara , Efecto Tardío Figurativo/fisiología , Orientación/fisiología , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología , Trastornos de la Visión/fisiopatología , Adolescente , Adulto , Factores de Edad , Análisis de Varianza , Niño , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA