Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 23
Filtrar
1.
Bull Tokyo Dent Coll ; 59(2): 133-137, 2018.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29962421

RESUMEN

Parkinson's disease (PD) is a highly prevalent, long-term neurodegenerative disorder that is sometimes treated by deep brain stimulation (DBS), which significantly reduces the need for dopaminergic drug therapy and improves quality of life. Such patients are cautioned, however, that dental instruments such as a dental turbine or ultrasonic scaler may adversely affect the functioning of such a system. Here, we report dental treatment for right maxillary tooth pain in a 65-year-old woman undergoing DBS for PD. The diagnosis was occlusal trauma. After verification with occluding paper each time, treatment comprised milling of the high contact points of tooth #17, followed by scaling with an ultrasonic scaler. This treatment was spread out over 3 visits, and its course was uneventful. To our knowledge, there are no previous reports on the interaction between dental instruments and DBS systems. Although no interference with the DBS system was observed here, we believe that the dentist should be aware of the potential for such, especially with the use of devices used to measure root canal length, dental lasers, and electrical scalpels.


Asunto(s)
Estimulación Encefálica Profunda/instrumentación , Atención Odontológica/efectos adversos , Enfermedad de Parkinson/terapia , Anciano , Atención Odontológica/instrumentación , Raspado Dental/efectos adversos , Raspado Dental/instrumentación , Femenino , Humanos , Maxilar/diagnóstico por imagen , Calidad de Vida , Resultado del Tratamiento , Terapia por Ultrasonido/efectos adversos , Terapia por Ultrasonido/instrumentación
2.
Cureus ; 14(5): e24871, 2022 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35698667

RESUMEN

Sialorrhea, a common symptom of Parkinson's disease (PD), is related to reduced oromotor control and autonomic dysfunction. However, neuromuscular electrical stimulation (NMES) helps improve overall swallowing function. We performed NMES for eight weeks in an 84-year-old woman with stage 5 PD and severe sialorrhea. The severity and frequency of drooling improved within one to three weeks of NMES. NMES can be used for patients who have lost the will to be rehabilitated or are unable to control an appropriate rehabilitation load themselves. It may also be useful for patients with multiple complications who are unable to commence new medications or injections.

3.
PLoS One ; 17(9): e0274294, 2022.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36094944

RESUMEN

This study aimed to investigate the nature of a specific body manipulation named Suichoku-Ririku (SR) in Japanese martial arts. SR is regarded as a method to change the way of stance and to distort the balance control of the opponent, but its nature and mechanism are unknown. In the present study, we attempted to determine the effect of SR in the cases that a person stood alone (Expt. 1) and that two persons stood in contact (Expt. 2). We compared several center of pressure (COP) measures between the normal stance and SR stance conditions. When participants stood independently (Expt. 1), the COP path length, standard deviation of COP velocity and permutation entropy of the COP increased with the SR stance, which suggested that the SR maneuver destabilized a quiet stance. When two participants stood (with normal stance) in contact by wrist-holding or by a light touch (Expt. 2), their COP motions were correlated with each other, as previously reported. When one of the participants took the SR maneuver, their correlation and mutual information were maintained, denying the view that SR would diminish the interpersonal correlation of body sway. On the other hand, a fluctuation in the COP increased only for the participant taking the SR maneuver, and not for the other participant. This asymmetric effect of the SR maneuver between two participants, irrespective of maintained mutual correlation, suggest that the relationship between balance controls of two participants was partly disrupted. We discuss possible mechanisms for the present results.


Asunto(s)
Artes Marciales , Equilibrio Postural , Humanos , Japón , Tacto , Extremidad Superior
4.
J Neurophysiol ; 104(6): 3625-43, 2010 Dec.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20943943

RESUMEN

We used mutual information analysis of neuronal activity in the macaque anterior intraparietal area (AIP) to examine information processing during a hand manipulation task. The task was to reach-to-grasp a three-dimensional (3D) object after presentation of a go signal. Mutual information was calculated between the spike counts of individual neurons in 50-ms-wide time bins and six unique shape classifications or 15 one-versus-one classifications of these shapes. The spatiotemporal distribution of mutual information was visualized as a two-dimensional image ("information map") to better observe global profiles of information representation. In addition, a nonnegative matrix factorization technique was applied for extracting its structure. Our major finding was that the time course of mutual information differed significantly according to different classes of task-related neurons. This strongly suggests that different classes of neurons were engaged in different information processing stages in executing the hand manipulation task. On the other hand, our analysis revealed the heterogeneous nature of information representation of AIP neurons. For example, "information latency" (or information onset) varied among individual neurons even in the same neuron class and the same shape classification. Further, some neurons changed "information preference" (i.e., shape classification with the largest amount of information) across different task periods. These suggest that neurons encode different information in the different task periods. Taking the present result together with previous findings, we used a Gantt chart to propose a hypothetical scheme of the dynamic interactions between different types of AIP neurons.


Asunto(s)
Fuerza de la Mano/fisiología , Mano/fisiología , Neuronas/fisiología , Lóbulo Parietal/fisiología , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Potenciales de Acción/fisiología , Animales , Mapeo Encefálico , Forma de la Célula , Presentación de Datos , Teoría de la Información , Macaca , Matemática , Neuronas/clasificación , Neuronas/ultraestructura , Lóbulo Parietal/citología , Factores de Tiempo
5.
PLoS One ; 14(8): e0221584, 2019.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31454393

RESUMEN

To examine how individuals perceive synchrony between music and body motion, we investigated the characteristics of synchrony perception during observation of a Japanese Radio Calisthenics routine. We used the constant stimuli method to present video clips of an individual performing an exercise routine. We generated stimuli with a range of temporal shifts between the visual and auditory streams, and asked participants to make synchrony judgments. We then examined which movement-feature points agreed with music beats when the participants perceived synchrony. We found that extremities (e.g., hands and feet) reached the movement endpoint or moved through the lowest position at music beats associated with synchrony. Movement onsets never agreed with music beats. To investigate whether visual information about the feature points was necessary for synchrony perception, we conducted a second experiment where only limited portions of video clips were presented to the participants. Participants consistently judged synchrony even when the video image did not contain the critical feature points, suggesting that a prediction mechanism contributes to synchrony perception. To discuss the meaning of these feature points with respect to synchrony perception, we examined the temporal relationship between the motion of body parts and the ground reaction force (GRF) of exercise performers, which reflected the total force acting on the performer. Interestingly, vertical GRF showed local peaks consistently synchronized with music beats for most exercises, with timing that was closely correlated with the timing of movement feature points. This result suggests that synchrony perception in humans is based on some global variable anticipated from visual information, instead of the feature points found in the motion of individual body parts. In summary, the present results indicate that synchrony perception during observation of human motion to music depends largely on spatiotemporal prediction of the performer's motion.


Asunto(s)
Percepción Auditiva/fisiología , Movimiento (Física) , Música , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Estimulación Acústica , Fenómenos Biomecánicos , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Estimulación Luminosa , Factores de Tiempo , Adulto Joven
6.
Neural Netw ; 21(7): 936-44, 2008 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18280701

RESUMEN

We propose a computational model of perceptual grouping for explaining the 3D shape representation of an illusory percept called "mime effect." This effect is associated with the generation of an illusory, volumetric perception that can be induced by particular distributions of inducing stimuli such as cones, whose orientations affect the stability of illusory perception. The authors have attempted to explain the characteristics of the shape representation of the mime effect using a neural network model that consists of four types of cells-encoding (E), normalizing (N), energetic (EN), and geometric (G) cells. E cells represent both the positions and orientations of inducing stimuli and the mime-effect shape, and N cells regulate the activity of E cells. The interactions of E cells generate dynamics whose mode indicates the stability of illusory perception; a stable dynamics mode indicates a stable perception, whereas a chaotic dynamics mode indicates an unstable perception. EN cells compute the Liapunov energetic exponent (LEE) from an energy function of the system of E cells. The stable and chaotic dynamics modes are identified by strictly negative and strictly positive values of LEE, respectively. In case of stability, G cells perform a particular surface interpolation for completing the mime effect shape. The authors confirm the model behaviour by means of computer-simulated experiments. The relation between the model behaviour and the shape representation in the human brain is also discussed.


Asunto(s)
Simulación por Computador , Ilusiones/fisiología , Redes Neurales de la Computación , Percepción/fisiología , Percepción Visual , Algoritmos , Humanos , Modelos Neurológicos , Dinámicas no Lineales , Orientación , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos
7.
Front Psychol ; 9: 2192, 2018.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30524330

RESUMEN

When sight-reading music, pianists have to decode a large number of notes and immediately transform them into finger actions. How do they achieve such fast decoding? Pianists may use geometrical features contained in the musical score, such as the distance between notes, to improve their efficiency in reading them. The aim of this study is to investigate the visual information pianists rely on when reading music. We measured the accuracy of the musical score reading of 16 skilled pianists and investigated its relationship with the geometrical features. When a single note was presented, pianists easily read it when it was located within three ledger lines. When two notes with an octave interval were presented, interestingly, their readable range was extended compared to that of the single note. The pianists were also able to recognize the octave interval correctly even if they misread the height (or pitch) of the target notes. These results suggest that the pianists decoded two notes composing an octave interval as a single "two-tone geometric pattern." Analyzing the characteristics of incorrect responses, we also found that pianists used the geometrical features of the spatial relationship between the note head and the ledger line, and that the cause of the misreading could be categorized into four types: [Type I] Confusion to a neighboring note having the same ledger line configuration; [Type II] Interference from a commonly used height note having the same note name; [Type III] Misunderstanding based on the appearance probability; [Type IV] Combination of the above three. These results all indicate that the pianists' abilities in score reading rely greatly on the correlation between the geometric features and playing action, which the pianists acquired through long-time training.

9.
Neurosci Res ; 57(2): 259-67, 2007 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17141906

RESUMEN

We developed a novel motoneuron model to examine the role of voltage-independent, Ca(2+)-activated potassium conductance (AHP conductance, or gAHP) in regulating repetitive firing. In addition to gAHP, the model also includes five voltage-gated conductances and a system that can reproduce Ca(2+) dynamics in the cytoplasm. Conductance kinetics were based on empirical data, and the model reproduced the piecewise linear, steady-state frequency-current relationship (f-I curve). The model revealed that gAHP has a "braking effect" that suppresses spike generation and thereby reduces firing frequency; the magnitude of the reduction in firing frequency is proportional to the temporal average of gAHP activation; and the level of activation depends on the magnitude of the injected current. Moreover, plotting the activation level as a function of injected current produced a bell-shaped curve, and this relationship was essential to the transition of the f-I curve. In conclusion, our study confirms the importance of gAHP to repetitive firing and presents a novel explanation for the piecewise linear f-I curve of motoneurons.


Asunto(s)
Potenciales de Acción/fisiología , Modelos Neurológicos , Neuronas Motoras/fisiología , Conducción Nerviosa/efectos de la radiación , Canales de Potasio Calcio-Activados/fisiología , Animales , Relación Dosis-Respuesta en la Radiación , Estimulación Eléctrica/métodos , Potenciales de la Membrana/fisiología , Potenciales de la Membrana/efectos de la radiación , Conducción Nerviosa/fisiología
10.
Vision Res ; 46(20): 3304-12, 2006 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16828837

RESUMEN

Two experiments were conducted to investigate how stimulus contrast affected the time required for perceptual filling-in. The stimuli consisted of a Gabor patch (target) and a circular grating region (surround). In Experiment 1, the target contrast was manipulated, and the surround contrast was fixed. Filling-in was significantly delayed with higher target contrast, but this delay was observed only when the target contrast exceeded the surround contrast. In Experiment 2, however, a much smaller effect of changing the surround contrast occurred. Possible reasons for this asymmetric effect are discussed.


Asunto(s)
Sensibilidad de Contraste/fisiología , Ilusiones Ópticas/fisiología , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología , Adulto , Área de Dependencia-Independencia , Fijación Ocular , Humanos , Orientación/fisiología , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Tiempo de Reacción
11.
Sci Rep ; 6: 30592, 2016 07 28.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27464844

RESUMEN

Back-and-forth motion induces perceptual shrinkage of the motion path, but such shrinkage is hardly perceived for one-way motion. If the shrinkage is caused by temporal averaging of stimulus position around the endpoints, it should also be induced for one-way motion at higher motion speeds. In psychophysical experiments with a high-speed projector, we tested this conjecture for a one-way motion stimulus at various speeds (4-100 deg/s) along a straight path. Results showed that perceptual shrinkage of the motion path was robustly observed in higher-speed motion (faster than 66.7 deg/s). In addition, the amount of the forwards shift at the onset position was larger than that of the backwards shift at the offset position. These results demonstrate that high-speed motion can induce shrinkage, even for a one-way motion path. This can be explained by the view that perceptual position is represented by the integration of the temporal average of instantaneous position and the motion representation.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Movimiento/fisiología , Adulto , Humanos , Masculino , Experimentación Humana no Terapéutica , Estimulación Luminosa , Psicofísica/instrumentación , Psicofísica/métodos
12.
Front Hum Neurosci ; 10: 381, 2016.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27516736

RESUMEN

Although there is growing evidence that breathing is modulated by various motor and cognitive activities, the nature of breathing in musical performance has been little explored. The present study examined the temporal breath pattern in piano performance, aiming to elucidate how breath timing is related to musical organization/events and performance. In the experiments, the respiration of 15 professional and amateur pianists, playing 10 music excerpts in total (from four-octave C major scale, Hanon's exercise, J. S. Bach's Invention, Mozart's Sonatas, and Debussy's Clair de lune), was monitored by capnography. The relationship between breathing and musical characteristics was analyzed. Five major results were obtained. (1) Mean breath interval was shortened for excerpts in faster tempi. (2) Fluctuation of breath intervals was reduced for the pieces for finger exercise and those in faster tempi. Pianists showing large within-trial fluctuation also exhibited large inter-excerpt difference. (3) Inter-trial consistency of the breath patterns depended on the excerpts. Consistency was generally reduced for the excerpts that could be performed mechanically (i.e., pieces for finger exercise), but interestingly, one third of the participant showed consistent patterns for the simple scale, correlated with the ascending/descending sequences. (4) Pianists tended to exhale just after the music onsets, inhale at the rests, and inhibit inhale during the slur parts. There was correlation between breathing pattern and two-voice polyphonic structure for several participants. (5) Respiratory patterns were notably different among the pianists. Every pianist showed his or her own characteristic features commonly for various musical works. These findings suggest that breathing in piano performance depends not only on musical parameters and organization written in the score but also some pianist-dependent factors which might be ingrained to individual pianists.

13.
Case Rep Dent ; 2016: 9437514, 2016.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27957349

RESUMEN

We report a case of buccal abscess caused by an impacted wisdom tooth in an extremely elderly person with malnutrition. The patient was a 94-year-old man, who complained that he had found it hard to open his mouth and that his cheek had been swollen for the previous 2 weeks. He had a shallow oral wound caused by an improperly fitting denture; however, the wound became infected. We performed incisional drainage of the abscess under local anesthesia. The swelling disappeared and he was able to open his mouth 55 mm. The elderly have a high risk of healing failure of injuries and it has been reported that infection in a host in a compromised state is severely intractable. This elderly patient was in a compromised state because of malnutrition. Cases such as this one will increase as the elderly population increases. Dentists need to consider the quality of life of patients with a longer life expectancy and should offer patients several treatment options before their general condition deteriorates.

14.
Atten Percept Psychophys ; 77(8): 2711-27, 2015 Nov.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26198710

RESUMEN

The critical fusion frequency (CFF) is a threshold that represents the temporal limits of the human visual system. If two flickering stimuli with equal subjective luminances are presented simultaneously at different locations, the CFF is the temporal frequency above which they cannot be distinguished. However, when the stimuli are presented sequentially at the same position, a transient twinkle can be perceived around the moment of the changeover. To investigate the mechanism underlying this transient twinkle perception (TTP), we independently manipulated the luminance contrast and temporal frequency of the flicker, as well as the interstimulus interval (ISI). We found that TTP was greater as the luminance step was larger, was stably perceived for flicker frequencies up to 200 Hz, and was robust for all ISIs if flicker frequencies were below 250 Hz. For 250- and 300-Hz flickers, TTP was attenuated in conditions in which one-frame and two-frame ISIs were inserted. These results can be explained by a simple filtering model: TTP occurs if the temporal change in the weighted moving average of stimulus luminance exceeds a certain threshold. TTP gives additional evidence that the human visual system can detect the transient change of flicker stimuli at much higher temporal frequencies than the CFF, by an averaging mechanism of luminance.


Asunto(s)
Fusión de Flicker/fisiología , Estimulación Luminosa , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Adulto , Humanos , Masculino , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Percepción del Tiempo/fisiología , Adulto Joven
15.
Neural Netw ; 62: 91-101, 2015 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24866293

RESUMEN

Human motor behavior often shows intermittent discontinuities even when people try to follow a continuously moving target. Although most previous studies revealed common characteristics of this "motor intermittency" using frequency analysis, this technique is not always appropriate because the nature of the intermittency is not stationary, i.e., the temporal intervals between the discontinuities may vary irregularly. In the present paper, we propose a novel method for extracting intermittent discontinuities using a continuous wavelet transform (CWT). This method is equivalent to the detection of peak of the jerk profile in principle, but it successfully and stably detects discontinuities using the amplitude and phase information of the complex wavelet transform. More specifically, the singularity point on the time-scale plane plays a key role in detecting the discontinuities. Another important feature is that the proposed method does not require parameter tuning because it is based on the nature of hand movement. In addition, this method does not contain any optimization process, which avoids explosive increase in computational cost for long time-series data. The performance of the proposed method was examined using an artificial trajectory composed of several primitive movements, and an actual hand trajectory in a continuous target-tracking task. The functional rationale of the proposed method is discussed.


Asunto(s)
Movimiento/fisiología , Análisis de Ondículas , Adulto , Algoritmos , Simulación por Computador , Mano/fisiología , Humanos , Masculino , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Adulto Joven
16.
Neural Netw ; 67: 92-109, 2015 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25897510

RESUMEN

It is a fundamental question how our brain performs a given motor task in a real-time fashion with the slow sensorimotor system. Computational theory proposed an influential idea of feed-forward control, but it has mainly treated the case that the movement is ballistic (such as reaching) because the motor commands should be calculated in advance of movement execution. As a possible mechanism for operating feed-forward control in continuous motor tasks (such as target tracking), we propose a control model called "adaptive intermittent control" or "segmented control," that brain adaptively divides the continuous time axis into discrete segments and executes feed-forward control in each segment. The idea of intermittent control has been proposed in the fields of control theory, biological modeling and nonlinear dynamical system. Compared with these previous models, the key of the proposed model is that the system speculatively determines the segmentation based on the future prediction and its uncertainty. The result of computer simulation showed that the proposed model realized faithful visuo-manual tracking with realistic sensorimotor delays and with less computational costs (i.e., with fewer number of segments). Furthermore, it replicated "motor intermittency", that is, intermittent discontinuities commonly observed in human movement trajectories. We discuss that the temporally segmented control is an inevitable strategy for brain which has to achieve a given task with small computational (or cognitive) cost, using a slow control system in an uncertain variable environment, and the motor intermittency is the side-effect of this strategy.


Asunto(s)
Conducta/fisiología , Modelos Neurológicos , Redes Neurales de la Computación , Algoritmos , Encéfalo/fisiología , Simulación por Computador , Humanos , Masculino , Procesos Mentales/fisiología , Dinámicas no Lineales , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Adulto Joven
17.
Neural Netw ; 62: 47-51, 2015 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25150125

RESUMEN

Although the phase shifts in ongoing oscillations seen in electroencephalograms (EEGs) and magnetoencephalograms are an important factor in discussions of phase dynamics, such as synchrony and reset, few studies have focused specifically on the phase shift. Here we investigate the relationship between phase shifts in alpha-frequency rhythms and reaction times during a visual simple reaction task by applying our previously described method (Naruse et al., 2013), which enables detection of phase shifts from a single EEG trial. In the left, parietal, and occipital areas, the reaction times in the trials in which phase shifts were detected before the button press were significantly longer than in those in which phase shifts were not so detected. These results indicate that phase shifts in the alpha and mu rhythms relate to variability in reaction times.


Asunto(s)
Ritmo alfa/fisiología , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Electroencefalografía , Lateralidad Funcional/fisiología , Humanos , Masculino , Distribución Normal , Adulto Joven
18.
Vision Res ; 43(19): 2029-38, 2003 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12842156

RESUMEN

Four experiments were performed to investigate how the time required for perceptual filling-in varies with the position of the target in the visual field. Conventional studies have revealed that filling-in is facilitated by a target with greater eccentricity, while no systematic studies have examined the effect of polar angle. Experiment 1 examined the effect of polar angle when the target and surround differed in luminance. Filling-in was facilitated as the target position changed from the horizontal to the vertical meridian. This dependency was more prominent in the upper field than in the lower, although no asymmetry was found between the left and right visual fields. These features were observed in both monocular and binocular viewing. These results were replicated in a modified stimulus configuration, in which the surround was a circular region concentric with the target (Experiment 2). Moreover, it was confirmed that the asymmetry was not due to fluctuation in the retinal image (i.e., eye movement) (Experiment 3). Finally, Experiment 4 examined whether this anisotropy was observed when two differently oriented gratings were presented in the target and surround regions. Again, filling-in was facilitated for a target close to the vertical meridian, irrespective of the relationship between the target and surround orientations. The underlying mechanism of this anisotropy is discussed from the viewpoints of cortical magnification and neural connections in the visual cortex.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Cercanía/fisiología , Agudeza Visual/fisiología , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Análisis de Varianza , Anisotropía , Percepción de Profundidad/fisiología , Humanos , Visión Binocular/fisiología , Visión Monocular/fisiología , Corteza Visual/fisiología , Campos Visuales/fisiología
19.
Neural Netw ; 17(7): 935-52, 2004 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15312837

RESUMEN

This article proposes an adaptive action-selection method for a model-free reinforcement learning system, based on the concept of the 'reliability of internal prediction/estimation'. This concept is realized using an internal variable, called the Reliability Index (RI), which estimates the accuracy of the internal estimator. We define this index for a value function of a temporal difference learning system and substitute it for the temperature parameter of the Boltzmann action-selection rule. Accordingly, the weight of exploratory actions adaptively changes depending on the uncertainty of the prediction. We use this idea for tabular and weighted-sum type value functions. Moreover, we use the RI to adjust the learning coefficient in addition to the temperature parameter, meaning that the reliability becomes a general basis for meta-learning. Numerical experiments were performed to examine the behavior of the proposed method. The RI-based Q-learning system demonstrated its features when the adaptive learning coefficient and large RI-discount rate (which indicate how the RI values of future states are reflected in the RI value of the current state) were introduced. Statistical tests confirmed that the algorithm spent more time exploring in the initial phase of learning, but accelerated learning from the midpoint of learning. It is also shown that the proposed method does not work well with the actor-critic models. The limitations of the proposed method and its relationship to relevant research are discussed.


Asunto(s)
Aclimatación/fisiología , Aprendizaje/fisiología , Modelos Psicológicos , Refuerzo en Psicología , Algoritmos , Animales , Inteligencia Artificial , Humanos , Aprendizaje por Laberinto , Procesos Mentales , Redes Neurales de la Computación , Reproducibilidad de los Resultados , Estadísticas no Paramétricas
20.
Hum Mov Sci ; 33: 211-26, 2014 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24355067

RESUMEN

When one performs visuo-manual tracking tasks, velocity profile of hand movements shows discontinuous patterns even if the target moves smoothly. A crucial factor of this "intermittency" is considerable delay in the sensorimotor feedback loop, and several researchers have suggested that the cause is intermittent correction of motor commands. However, when and how the brain monitors task performance and updates motor commands in a continuous motor task is uncertain. We examined how tracking error was affected by the timing of target disappearance during a tracking task. Results showed that tracking error, defined as the average phase difference between target and hand, varied periodically in all conditions. Hand preceded target at one specific phase but followed it at another, implying that motor control was not performed in a temporally uniform manner. Tracking stability was evaluated by the variance in phase difference, and changed depending on the timing of target-removal. The variability was larger when target disappeared around turning points than that when it disappeared around the center of motion. This shows that visual information at turning points is more effectively exploited for motor control of sinusoidal target tracking, suggesting that our brain controls hand movements with intermittent reference to visual information.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Movimiento , Orientación , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos , Desempeño Psicomotor , Tiempo de Reacción , Atención , Fenómenos Biomecánicos , Humanos , Masculino , Enmascaramiento Perceptual , Adulto Joven
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA