Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 17 de 17
Filtrar
1.
Cell ; 145(6): 956-68, 2011 Jun 10.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21663797

RESUMEN

How complex networks of activators and repressors lead to exquisitely specific cell-type determination during development is poorly understood. In the Drosophila eye, expression patterns of Rhodopsins define at least eight functionally distinct though related subtypes of photoreceptors. Here, we describe a role for the transcription factor gene defective proventriculus (dve) as a critical node in the network regulating Rhodopsin expression. dve is a shared component of two opposing, interlocked feedforward loops (FFLs). Orthodenticle and Dve interact in an incoherent FFL to repress Rhodopsin expression throughout the eye. In R7 and R8 photoreceptors, a coherent FFL relieves repression by Dve while activating Rhodopsin expression. Therefore, this network uses repression to restrict and combinatorial activation to induce cell-type-specific expression. Furthermore, Dve levels are finely tuned to yield cell-type- and region-specific repression or activation outcomes. This interlocked FFL motif may be a general mechanism to control terminal cell-fate specification.


Asunto(s)
Proteínas de Drosophila/genética , Drosophila/embriología , Drosophila/metabolismo , Regulación del Desarrollo de la Expresión Génica , Redes Reguladoras de Genes , Células Fotorreceptoras de Invertebrados/metabolismo , Rodopsina/genética , Animales , Drosophila/citología , Proteínas de Drosophila/metabolismo , Ojo/embriología , Retroalimentación Fisiológica , Proteínas de Homeodominio/metabolismo , Factores de Transcripción/metabolismo
2.
PLoS Biol ; 16(2): e2003113, 2018 02.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29438378

RESUMEN

It is widely accepted for humans and higher animals that vision is an active process in which the organism interprets the stimulus. To find out whether this also holds for lower animals, we designed an ambiguous motion stimulus, which serves as something like a multi-stable perception paradigm in Drosophila behavior. Confronted with a uniform panoramic texture in a closed-loop situation in stationary flight, the flies adjust their yaw torque to stabilize their virtual self-rotation. To make the visual input ambiguous, we added a second texture. Both textures got a rotatory bias to move into opposite directions at a constant relative angular velocity. The results indicate that the fly now had three possible frames of reference for self-rotation: either of the two motion components as well as the integrated motion vector of the two. In this ambiguous stimulus situation, the flies generated a continuous sequence of behaviors, each one adjusted to one or another of the three references.


Asunto(s)
Conducta Animal , Drosophila melanogaster/fisiología , Vuelo Animal/fisiología , Orientación Espacial , Estimulación Luminosa , Animales , Femenino , Procesos Estocásticos , Percepción Visual
3.
J Neurogenet ; 34(1): 9-20, 2020 03.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32233838

RESUMEN

We present here our reflections on the scientific work of the late Troy D. Zars (1967 - 2018), on what it was like to work with him, and what it means to us. A common theme running through his work is that memory systems are not for replaying the past. Rather, they are forward-looking systems, providing whatever guidance past experience has to offer for anticipating the outcome of future actions. And in situations where no such guidance is available trying things out is the best option. Working with Troy was inspiring precisely because of the optimism inherent in this concept and that he himself embodied. Our reflections highlight what this means to us as his former mentors, colleagues, and mentees, respectively, and what it might mean for the future of neurogenetics.


Asunto(s)
Genética/historia , Neurología/historia , Animales , Anticipación Psicológica/fisiología , Drosophila melanogaster/fisiología , Historia del Siglo XX , Historia del Siglo XXI , Humanos , Aprendizaje/fisiología , Memoria/fisiología , Mentores
4.
Learn Mem ; 24(7): 318-321, 2017 07.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28620079

RESUMEN

Starvation causes a motivational state that facilitates diverse behaviors such as feeding, walking, and search. Starved Drosophila can form odor/feeding-time associations but the role of starvation in encoding of "time" is poorly understood. Here we show that the extent of starvation is correlated with the fly's ability to establish odor/feeding-time memories. Prolonged starvation promotes odor/feeding-time associations after just a single cycle of reciprocal training. We also show that starvation is required for acquisition but is dispensable for retrieval of odor/feeding-time memory. Finally, even with extended starvation, a functional circadian oscillator is indispensable for establishing odor/feeding-time memories.


Asunto(s)
Aprendizaje por Asociación/fisiología , Conducta Alimentaria/fisiología , Privación de Alimentos/fisiología , Memoria/fisiología , Odorantes , Animales , Animales Modificados Genéticamente , Proteínas de Drosophila/genética , Proteínas de Drosophila/metabolismo , Drosophila melanogaster , Femenino , Masculino , Mutación/genética , Proteínas Circadianas Period/genética , Proteínas Circadianas Period/metabolismo , Factores de Tiempo
5.
J Neurogenet ; 29(1): 30-7, 2015 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25585638

RESUMEN

Novelty choice, a visual paired-comparison task, for the fly Drosophila melanogaster is studied with severely restrained single animals in a flight simulator. The virtual environment simulates free flight for rotation in the horizontal plane. The behavior has three functional components: visual azimuth orientation, working memory, and pattern discrimination (perception). Here we study novelty choice in relation to its neural substrate in the brain and show that it requires the central complex and, in particular, the ring neurons of the ellipsoid body. Surprisingly, it also involves the mushroom bodies which are needed specifically in the comparison of patterns of different sizes.


Asunto(s)
Conducta de Elección/fisiología , Cuerpos Pedunculados/citología , Cuerpos Pedunculados/fisiología , Neuronas/fisiología , Vías Visuales/fisiología , Animales , Animales Modificados Genéticamente , Drosophila , Proteínas de Drosophila/genética , Inhibidores Enzimáticos/farmacología , Hidroxiurea/farmacología , Memoria a Corto Plazo/fisiología , Neuronas/efectos de los fármacos , Orientación/fisiología , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología , Estimulación Luminosa
6.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 108(17): 7230-5, 2011 Apr 26.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21482795

RESUMEN

Organisms with complex visual systems rarely respond to just the sum of all visual stimuli impinging on their eyes. Often, they restrict their responses to stimuli in a temporarily selected region of the visual field (selective visual attention). Here, we investigate visual attention in the fly Drosophila during tethered flight at a torque meter. Flies can actively shift their attention; however, their attention can be guided to a certain location by external cues. Using visual cues, we can direct the attention of the fly to one or the other of the two visual half-fields. The cue can precede the test stimulus by several seconds and may also be spatially separated from the test by at least 20° and yet attract attention. This kind of external guidance of attention is found only in the lower visual field.


Asunto(s)
Atención/fisiología , Conducta Animal/fisiología , Vuelo Animal/fisiología , Animales , Drosophila , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos
7.
Nature ; 439(7076): 551-6, 2006 Feb 02.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16452971

RESUMEN

The fly Drosophila melanogaster can discriminate and remember visual landmarks. It analyses selected parts of its visual environment according to a small number of pattern parameters such as size, colour or contour orientation, and stores particular parameter values. Like humans, flies recognize patterns independently of the retinal position during acquisition of the pattern (translation invariance). Here we show that the central-most part of the fly brain, the fan-shaped body, contains parts of a network mediating visual pattern recognition. We have identified short-term memory traces of two pattern parameters--elevation in the panorama and contour orientation. These can be localized to two groups of neurons extending branches as parallel, horizontal strata in the fan-shaped body. The central location of this memory store is well suited to mediate translational invariance.


Asunto(s)
Encéfalo/fisiología , Drosophila/fisiología , Memoria/fisiología , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología , Adenilil Ciclasas/genética , Adenilil Ciclasas/metabolismo , Animales , Señales (Psicología) , Drosophila/genética , Proteínas de Drosophila/genética , Proteínas de Drosophila/metabolismo , Memoria a Corto Plazo/fisiología , Mutación/genética , Estimulación Luminosa , Torque
8.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 105(12): 4910-5, 2008 Mar 25.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18353989

RESUMEN

Whether motion vision uses color contrast is a controversial issue that has been investigated in several species, from insects to humans. We used Drosophila to answer this question, monitoring the optomotor response to moving color stimuli in WT and genetic variants. In the fly eye, a motion channel (outer photoreceptors R1-R6) and a color channel (inner photoreceptors R7 and R8) have been distinguished. With moving bars of alternating colors and high color contrast, a brightness ratio of the two colors can be found, at which the optomotor response is largely missing (point of equiluminance). Under these conditions, mutant flies lacking functional rhodopsin in R1-R6 cells do not respond at all. Furthermore, genetically eliminating the function of photoreceptors R7 and R8 neither alters the strength of the optomotor response nor shifts the point of equiluminance. We conclude that the color channel (R7/R8) does not contribute to motion detection as monitored by the optomotor response.


Asunto(s)
Drosophila melanogaster/fisiología , Movimiento (Física) , Visión Ocular , Animales , Ceguera , Color , Sensibilidad de Contraste , Drosophila melanogaster/citología , Mutación/genética , Células Fotorreceptoras de Invertebrados/citología
9.
Elife ; 92020 02 18.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32066523

RESUMEN

Experience alters brain structure, but the underlying mechanism remained unknown. Structural plasticity reveals that brain function is encoded in generative changes to cells that compete with destructive processes driving neurodegeneration. At an adult critical period, experience increases fiber number and brain size in Drosophila. Here, we asked if Toll receptors are involved. Tolls demarcate a map of brain anatomical domains. Focusing on Toll-2, loss of function caused apoptosis, neurite atrophy and impaired behaviour. Toll-2 gain of function and neuronal activity at the critical period increased cell number. Toll-2 induced cycling of adult progenitor cells via a novel pathway, that antagonized MyD88-dependent quiescence, and engaged Weckle and Yorkie downstream. Constant knock-down of multiple Tolls synergistically reduced brain size. Conditional over-expression of Toll-2 and wek at the adult critical period increased brain size. Through their topographic distribution, Toll receptors regulate neuronal number and brain size, modulating structural plasticity in the adult brain.


Everything that you experience leaves its mark on your brain. When you learn something new, the neurons involved in the learning episode grow new projections and form new connections. Your brain may even produce new neurons. Physical exercise can induce similar changes, as can taking antidepressants. By contrast, stress, depression, ageing and disease can have the opposite effect, triggering neurons to break down and even die. The ability of the brain to change in response to experience is known as structural plasticity, and it is in a tug-of-war with processes that drive neurodegeneration. Structural plasticity occurs in other species too: for example, it was described in the fruit fly more than a quarter of a century ago. Yet, the molecular mechanisms underlying structural plasticity remain unclear. Li et al. now show that, in fruit flies, this plasticity involves Toll receptors, a family of proteins present in the brain but best known for their role in the immune system. Fruit flies have nine different Toll receptors, the most abundant being Toll-2. When activated, these proteins can trigger a series of molecular events in a cell. Li et al. show that increasing the amount of Toll-2 in the fly brain makes the brain produce new neurons. Activating neurons in a brain region has the same effect, and this increase in neuron number also depends on Toll-2. By contrast, reducing the amount of Toll-2 causes neurons to lose their projections and connections, and to die, and impairs fly behaviour. Li et al. also show that each Toll receptor has a unique distribution across the fly brain. Different types of experiences activate different brain regions, and therefore different Toll receptors. These go on to trigger a common molecular cascade, but they modulate it such as to result in distinct outcomes. By working together in different combinations, Toll receptors can promote either the death or survival of neurons, and they can also drive specific brain cells to remain dormant or to produce new neurons. By revealing how experience changes the brain, Li et al. provide clues to the way neurons work and form; these findings may also help to find new treatments for disorders that change brain structure, such as certain psychiatric conditions. Toll-like receptors in humans could thus represent a promising new target for drug discovery.


Asunto(s)
Encéfalo/fisiología , Drosophila melanogaster/fisiología , Plasticidad Neuronal/fisiología , Receptores Toll-Like/metabolismo , Animales , Encéfalo/citología , Encéfalo/metabolismo , Neuronas/fisiología
10.
Dev Cell ; 50(6): 780-792.e7, 2019 09 23.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31447264

RESUMEN

Size trade-offs of visual versus olfactory organs is a pervasive feature of animal evolution. This could result from genetic or functional constraints. We demonstrate that head sensory organ size trade-offs in Drosophila are genetically encoded and arise through differential subdivision of the head primordium into visual versus non-visual fields. We discover that changes in the temporal regulation of the highly conserved eyeless/Pax6 gene expression during development is a conserved mechanism for sensory trade-offs within and between Drosophila species. We identify a natural single nucleotide polymorphism in the cis-regulatory region of eyeless in a binding site of its repressor Cut that is sufficient to alter its temporal regulation and eye size. Because eyeless/Pax6 is a conserved regulator of head sensory placode subdivision, we propose that its temporal regulation is key to define the relative size of head sensory organs.


Asunto(s)
Evolución Biológica , Proteínas de Unión al ADN/metabolismo , Proteínas de Drosophila/metabolismo , Drosophila melanogaster/genética , Regulación del Desarrollo de la Expresión Génica , Órganos de los Sentidos/metabolismo , Animales , Sitios de Unión , Proteínas de Unión al ADN/genética , Proteínas de Drosophila/genética , Elementos de Facilitación Genéticos/genética , Ojo/anatomía & histología , Ojo/metabolismo , Femenino , Geografía , Cabeza , Nucleótidos/genética , Tamaño de los Órganos/genética , Polimorfismo de Nucleótido Simple/genética , Factores de Tiempo
11.
PLoS One ; 11(2): e0148208, 2016.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26848852

RESUMEN

A visual stimulus at a particular location of the visual field may elicit a behavior while at the same time equally salient stimuli in other parts do not. This property of visual systems is known as selective visual attention (SVA). The animal is said to have a focus of attention (FoA) which it has shifted to a particular location. Visual attention normally involves an attention span at the location to which the FoA has been shifted. Here the attention span is measured in Drosophila. The fly is tethered and hence has its eyes fixed in space. It can shift its FoA internally. This shift is revealed using two simultaneous test stimuli with characteristic responses at their particular locations. In tethered flight a wild type fly keeps its FoA at a certain location for up to 4s. Flies with a mutation in the radish gene, that has been suggested to be involved in attention-like mechanisms, display a reduced attention span of only 1s.


Asunto(s)
Atención/fisiología , Drosophila melanogaster/fisiología , Percepción Visual , Animales , Conducta de Elección , Proteínas de Drosophila/genética , Drosophila melanogaster/genética , Femenino , Mutación , Fosfoproteínas/genética , Estimulación Luminosa , Probabilidad
12.
PLoS One ; 11(11): e0167066, 2016.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27875580

RESUMEN

Like other animals flies develop a state of learned helplessness in response to unescapable aversive events. To show this, two flies, one 'master', one 'yoked', are each confined to a dark, small chamber and exposed to the same sequence of mild electric shocks. Both receive these shocks when the master fly stops walking for more than a second. Behavior in the two animals is differently affected by the shocks. Yoked flies are transiently impaired in place learning and take longer than master flies to exit from the chamber towards light. After the treatment they walk more slowly and take fewer and shorter walking bouts. The low activity is attributed to the fly's experience that its escape response, an innate behavior to terminate the electric shocks, does not help anymore. Earlier studies using heat pulses instead of electric shocks had shown similar effects. This parallel supports the interpretation that it is the uncontrollability that induces the state.


Asunto(s)
Conducta Animal , Reacción de Fuga , Aprendizaje , Estrés Fisiológico , Caminata , Animales , Drosophila melanogaster
13.
PLoS One ; 11(8): e0161412, 2016.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27571359

RESUMEN

Visual environments may simultaneously comprise stimuli of different significance. Often such stimuli require incompatible responses. Selective visual attention allows an animal to respond exclusively to the stimuli at a certain location in the visual field. In the process of establishing its focus of attention the animal can be influenced by external cues. Here we characterize the behavioral properties and neural mechanism of cueing in the fly Drosophila melanogaster. A cue can be attractive, repulsive or ineffective depending upon (e.g.) its visual properties and location in the visual field. Dopamine signaling in the brain is required to maintain the effect of cueing once the cue has disappeared. Raising or lowering dopamine at the synapse abolishes this after-effect. Specifically, dopamine is necessary and sufficient in the αß-lobes of the mushroom bodies. Evidence is provided for an involvement of the αßposterior Kenyon cells.


Asunto(s)
Dopamina/metabolismo , Drosophila melanogaster/fisiología , Cuerpos Pedunculados/fisiología , Animales , Encéfalo/fisiología , Señales (Psicología)
14.
Curr Biol ; 25(12): 1619-24, 2015 Jun 15.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26028434

RESUMEN

The circadian clock enables organisms to anticipate daily environmental cycles and drives corresponding changes in behavior [1, 2]. Such endogenous oscillators also enable animals to display time-specific memory [1, 3-5]. For instance, mice and honeybees associate the location of a stimulus (like food or mate) with a certain time of day (time-place learning) [6, 7]. However, the mechanism underlying time-related learning and memory is not known. In the present study, we investigate time-specific odor learning. We use a genetically tractable animal, the fly Drosophila melanogaster. Starved flies are trained in the morning and afternoon to associate distinct odors with sucrose reward. The training is repeated the next day, and their time-dependent odor preference is tested on the third day. Our results indicate that Drosophila can express appetitive memory at the relevant time of day if the two conditioning events are separated by more than 4 hr. Flies can form time-odor associations in constant darkness (DD) as well as in a daily light-dark (LD) cycle, but not when kept under constant light (LL) conditions. Circadian clock mutants, period(01) (per(01)) and clock(AR) (clk(AR)), learned to associate sucrose reward with a certain odor but were unable to form time-odor associations. Our findings show that flies can utilize temporal information as an additional cue in appetitive learning. Time-odor learning in flies depends on a per- and clk-dependent endogenous mechanism that is independent of environmental light cues.


Asunto(s)
Ritmo Circadiano , Drosophila melanogaster/fisiología , Animales , Proteínas CLOCK/genética , Proteínas CLOCK/metabolismo , Aprendizaje , Memoria , Proteínas Circadianas Period/genética , Proteínas Circadianas Period/metabolismo
15.
Angew Chem Int Ed Engl ; 37(7): 975-979, 1998 Apr 20.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29711472

RESUMEN

A color change from purple to green takes place on addition of tetrathiafulvalene (TTF) to the macrobicyclic receptor 14+ , which is composed of a cyclobis(paraquat-p-phenylene) tetracation that shares one of its paraphenylene rings with a 1,5-naphthoparaphenylene-[36]crown-10 macrocycle. The TTF molecule forces the macrobicycle to turn inside out (see schematic drawing below) and displaces the self-complexed 1,5-dioxynaphthalene ring system from the center of the tetracationic cyclophane.

16.
Curr Biol ; 23(9): 799-803, 2013 May 06.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23602474

RESUMEN

In a wide range of animals, uncontrollable stressful events can induce a condition called "learned helplessness." In mammals it is associated with low general activity, poor learning, disorders of sleep and feeding, ulcers, and reduced immune status, as well as with increased serotonin in parts of the brain. It is considered an animal model of depression in humans. Here we investigate learned helplessness in Drosophila, showing that this behavioral state consists of a cognitive and a modulatory, possibly mood-like, component. A fly, getting heated as soon as it stops walking, reliably resumes walking to escape the heat. If, in contrast, the fly is not in control of the heat, it learns that its behavior has no effect and quits responding. In this state, the fly walks slowly and takes longer and more frequent rests, as if it were "depressed." This downregulation of walking behavior is more pronounced in females than in males. Learned helplessness in Drosophila is an example of how, in a certain situation, behavior is organized according to its expected consequences.


Asunto(s)
Drosophila melanogaster/fisiología , Animales , Cognición , Femenino , Desamparo Adquirido , Masculino , Motivación , Caracteres Sexuales , Estrés Fisiológico , Caminata
17.
Science ; 305(5686): 1020-2, 2004 Aug 13.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15310908

RESUMEN

Vision relies on constancy mechanisms. Yet, these are little understood, because they are difficult to investigate in freely moving organisms. One such mechanism, translation invariance, enables organisms to recognize visual patterns independent of the region of their visual field where they had originally seen them. Tethered flies (Drosophila melanogaster) in a flight simulator can recognize visual patterns. Because their eyes are fixed in space and patterns can be displayed in defined parts of their visual field, they can be tested for translation invariance. Here, we show that flies recognize patterns at retinal positions where the patterns had not been presented before.


Asunto(s)
Drosophila melanogaster/fisiología , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos , Animales , Percepción de Color , Condicionamiento Psicológico , Señales (Psicología) , Femenino , Vuelo Animal , Aprendizaje , Orientación , Retina/fisiología , Percepción del Tamaño
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA