Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 777
Filtrar
Más filtros

Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
Annu Rev Neurosci ; 47(1): 255-276, 2024 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38663429

RESUMEN

The zebrafish visual system has become a paradigmatic preparation for behavioral and systems neuroscience. Around 40 types of retinal ganglion cells (RGCs) serve as matched filters for stimulus features, including light, optic flow, prey, and objects on a collision course. RGCs distribute their signals via axon collaterals to 12 retinorecipient areas in forebrain and midbrain. The major visuomotor hub, the optic tectum, harbors nine RGC input layers that combine information on multiple features. The retinotopic map in the tectum is locally adapted to visual scene statistics and visual subfield-specific behavioral demands. Tectal projections to premotor centers are topographically organized according to behavioral commands. The known connectivity in more than 20 processing streams allows us to dissect the cellular basis of elementary perceptual and cognitive functions. Visually evoked responses, such as prey capture or loom avoidance, are controlled by dedicated multistation pathways that-at least in the larva-resemble labeled lines. This architecture serves the neuronal code's purpose of driving adaptive behavior.


Asunto(s)
Células Ganglionares de la Retina , Colículos Superiores , Vías Visuales , Pez Cebra , Animales , Vías Visuales/fisiología , Pez Cebra/fisiología , Células Ganglionares de la Retina/fisiología , Colículos Superiores/fisiología , Percepción Visual/fisiología
2.
Annu Rev Neurosci ; 47(1): 345-368, 2024 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38684081

RESUMEN

The activity patterns of grid cells form distinctively regular triangular lattices over the explored spatial environment and are largely invariant to visual stimuli, animal movement, and environment geometry. These neurons present numerous fascinating challenges to the curious (neuro)scientist: What are the circuit mechanisms responsible for creating spatially periodic activity patterns from the monotonic input-output responses of single neurons? How and why does the brain encode a local, nonperiodic variable-the allocentric position of the animal-with a periodic, nonlocal code? And, are grid cells truly specialized for spatial computations? Otherwise, what is their role in general cognition more broadly? We review efforts in uncovering the mechanisms and functional properties of grid cells, highlighting recent progress in the experimental validation of mechanistic grid cell models, and discuss the coding properties and functional advantages of the grid code as suggested by continuous attractor network models of grid cells.


Asunto(s)
Cognición , Células de Red , Modelos Neurológicos , Animales , Cognición/fisiología , Células de Red/fisiología , Humanos , Percepción Espacial/fisiología , Red Nerviosa/fisiología , Encéfalo/fisiología , Neuronas/fisiología
3.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 121(23): e2312851121, 2024 Jun 04.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38771864

RESUMEN

The way goal-oriented birds adjust their travel direction and route in response to wind significantly affects their travel costs. This is expected to be particularly pronounced in pelagic seabirds, which utilize a wind-dependent flight style called dynamic soaring. Dynamic soaring seabirds in situations without a definite goal, e.g. searching for prey, are known to preferentially fly with crosswinds or quartering-tailwinds to increase the speed and search area, and reduce travel costs. However, little is known about their reaction to wind when heading to a definite goal, such as homing. Homing tracks of wandering albatrosses (Diomedea exulans) vary from beelines to zigzags, which are similar to those of sailboats. Here, given that both albatrosses and sailboats travel slower in headwinds and tailwinds, we tested whether the time-minimizing strategies used by yacht racers can be compared to the locomotion patterns of wandering albatrosses. We predicted that when the goal is located upwind or downwind, albatrosses should deviate their travel directions from the goal on the mesoscale and increase the number of turns on the macroscale. Both hypotheses were supported by track data from albatrosses and racing yachts in the Southern Ocean confirming that albatrosses qualitatively employ the same strategy as yacht racers. Nevertheless, albatrosses did not strictly minimize their travel time, likely making their flight robust against wind fluctuations to reduce flight costs. Our study provides empirical evidence of tacking in albatrosses and demonstrates that man-made movement strategies provide a new perspective on the laws underlying wildlife movement.


Asunto(s)
Aves , Vuelo Animal , Viento , Animales , Vuelo Animal/fisiología , Aves/fisiología , Orientación/fisiología , Fenómenos de Retorno al Lugar Habitual/fisiología , Orientación Espacial/fisiología , Migración Animal/fisiología
4.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 121(5): e2310735121, 2024 Jan 30.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38252838

RESUMEN

Animals navigate their environment by manipulating their movements and adjusting their trajectory which requires a sophisticated integration of sensory data with their current motor status. Here, we utilize the nematode Caenorhabditis elegans to explore the neural mechanisms of processing the sensory and motor information for navigation. We developed a microfluidic device which allows animals to freely move their heads while receiving temporal NaCl stimuli. We found that C. elegans regulates neck bending direction in response to temporal NaCl concentration changes in a way which is consistent with a C. elegans' navigational strategy which regulates traveling direction toward preferred NaCl concentrations. Our analysis also revealed that the activity of a neck motor neuron is significantly correlated with neck bending and activated by the decrease in NaCl concentration in a phase-dependent manner. By combining the analysis of behavioral and neural response to NaCl stimuli and optogenetic perturbation experiments, we revealed that NaCl decrease during ventral bending activates the neck motor neuron which counteracts ipsilateral bending. Simulations further suggest that this phase-dependent response of neck motor neurons can facilitate curving toward preferred salt concentrations.


Asunto(s)
Fenómenos Fisiológicos del Sistema Nervioso , Cloruro de Sodio , Animales , Caenorhabditis elegans , Cloruro de Sodio Dietético , Neuronas Motoras
5.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 121(30): e2402509121, 2024 Jul 23.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39008670

RESUMEN

Insects rely on path integration (vector-based navigation) and landmark guidance to perform sophisticated navigational feats, rivaling those seen in mammals. Bees in particular exhibit complex navigation behaviors including creating optimal routes and novel shortcuts between locations, an ability historically indicative of the presence of a cognitive map. A mammalian cognitive map has been widely accepted. However, in insects, the existence of a centralized cognitive map is highly contentious. Using a controlled laboratory assay that condenses foraging behaviors to short distances in walking bumblebees, we reveal that vectors learned during path integration can be transferred to long-term memory, that multiple such vectors can be stored in parallel, and that these vectors can be recalled at a familiar location and used for homeward navigation. These findings demonstrate that bees meet the two fundamental requirements of a vector-based analog of a decentralized cognitive map: Home vectors need to be stored in long-term memory and need to be recalled from remembered locations. Thus, our data demonstrate that bees possess the foundational elements for a vector-based map. By utilizing this relatively simple strategy for spatial organization, insects may achieve high-level navigation behaviors seen in vertebrates with the limited number of neurons in their brains, circumventing the computational requirements associated with the cognitive maps of mammals.


Asunto(s)
Encéfalo , Navegación Espacial , Animales , Abejas/fisiología , Encéfalo/fisiología , Navegación Espacial/fisiología , Memoria/fisiología , Memoria a Largo Plazo/fisiología , Cognición/fisiología
6.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 121(12): e2315758121, 2024 Mar 19.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38489383

RESUMEN

Grid cells in the entorhinal cortex (EC) encode an individual's location in space, integrating both environmental and multisensory bodily cues. Notably, body-derived signals are also primary signals for the sense of self. While studies have demonstrated that continuous application of visuo-tactile bodily stimuli can induce perceptual shifts in self-location, it remains unexplored whether these illusory changes suffice to trigger grid cell-like representation (GCLR) within the EC, and how this compares to GCLR during conventional virtual navigation. To address this, we systematically induced illusory drifts in self-location toward controlled directions using visuo-tactile bodily stimulation, while maintaining the subjects' visual viewpoint fixed (absent conventional virtual navigation). Subsequently, we evaluated the corresponding GCLR in the EC through functional MRI analysis. Our results reveal that illusory changes in perceived self-location (independent of changes in environmental navigation cues) can indeed evoke entorhinal GCLR, correlating in strength with the magnitude of perceived self-location, and characterized by similar grid orientation as during conventional virtual navigation in the same virtual room. These data demonstrate that the same grid-like representation is recruited when navigating based on environmental, mainly visual cues, or when experiencing illusory forward drifts in self-location, driven by perceptual multisensory bodily cues.


Asunto(s)
Células de Red , Ilusiones , Navegación Espacial , Humanos , Corteza Entorrinal/fisiología , Células de Red/fisiología , Estado de Conciencia , Ilusiones/fisiología , Tacto , Navegación Espacial/fisiología
7.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 121(25): e2321614121, 2024 Jun 18.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38857401

RESUMEN

The medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) is a key brain structure for higher cognitive functions such as decision-making and goal-directed behavior, many of which require awareness of spatial variables including one's current position within the surrounding environment. Although previous studies have reported spatially tuned activities in mPFC during memory-related trajectory, the spatial tuning of mPFC network during freely foraging behavior remains elusive. Here, we reveal geometric border or border-proximal representations from the neural activity of mPFC ensembles during naturally exploring behavior, with both allocentric and egocentric boundary responses. Unlike most of classical border cells in the medial entorhinal cortex (MEC) discharging along a single wall, a large majority of border cells in mPFC fire particularly along four walls. mPFC border cells generate new firing fields to external insert, and remain stable under darkness, across distinct shapes, and in novel environments. In contrast to hippocampal theta entrainment during spatial working memory tasks, mPFC border cells rarely exhibited theta rhythmicity during spontaneous locomotion behavior. These findings reveal spatially modulated activity in mPFC, supporting local computation for cognitive functions involving spatial context and contributing to a broad spatial tuning property of cortical circuits.


Asunto(s)
Corteza Prefrontal , Ritmo Teta , Corteza Prefrontal/fisiología , Corteza Prefrontal/citología , Animales , Ritmo Teta/fisiología , Masculino , Ratones , Corteza Entorrinal/fisiología , Neuronas/fisiología , Hipocampo/fisiología , Memoria Espacial/fisiología , Ratones Endogámicos C57BL , Memoria a Corto Plazo/fisiología
8.
J Neurosci ; 44(24)2024 Jun 12.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38641405

RESUMEN

Structural differences along the hippocampal long axis are believed to underlie meaningful functional differences. Yet, recent data-driven parcellations of the hippocampus subdivide the hippocampus into a 10-cluster map with anterior-medial, anterior-lateral, and posteroanterior-lateral, middle, and posterior components. We tested whether task and experience could modulate this clustering using a spatial learning experiment where male and female participants were trained to virtually navigate a novel neighborhood in a Google Street View-like environment. Participants were scanned while navigating routes early in training and after a 2 week training period. Using the 10-cluster map as the ideal template, we found that participants who eventually learn the neighborhood well have hippocampal cluster maps consistent with the ideal-even on their second day of learning-and their cluster mappings do not deviate over the 2 week training period. However, participants who eventually learn the neighborhood poorly begin with hippocampal cluster maps inconsistent with the ideal template, though their cluster mappings may become more stereotypical after the 2 week training. Interestingly this improvement seems to be route specific: after some early improvement, when a new route is navigated, participants' hippocampal maps revert back to less stereotypical organization. We conclude that hippocampal clustering is not dependent solely on anatomical structure and instead is driven by a combination of anatomy, task, and, importantly, experience. Nonetheless, while hippocampal clustering can change with experience, efficient navigation depends on functional hippocampal activity clustering in a stereotypical manner, highlighting optimal divisions of processing along the hippocampal anterior-posterior and medial-lateral axes.


Asunto(s)
Hipocampo , Navegación Espacial , Realidad Virtual , Hipocampo/fisiología , Masculino , Humanos , Femenino , Navegación Espacial/fisiología , Adulto , Adulto Joven , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética/métodos , Aprendizaje Espacial/fisiología , Análisis por Conglomerados
9.
Cereb Cortex ; 34(1)2024 01 14.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38100330

RESUMEN

There is disagreement regarding the major components of the brain network supporting spatial cognition. To address this issue, we applied a lesion mapping approach to the clinical phenomenon of topographical disorientation. Topographical disorientation is the inability to maintain accurate knowledge about the physical environment and use it for navigation. A review of published topographical disorientation cases identified 65 different lesion sites. Our lesion mapping analysis yielded a topographical disorientation brain map encompassing the classic regions of the navigation network: medial parietal, medial temporal, and temporo-parietal cortices. We also identified a ventromedial region of the prefrontal cortex, which has been absent from prior descriptions of this network. Moreover, we revealed that the regions mapped are correlated with the Default Mode Network sub-network C. Taken together, this study provides causal evidence for the distribution of the spatial cognitive system, demarking the major components and identifying novel regions.


Asunto(s)
Orientación Espacial , Orientación , Humanos , Encéfalo/patología , Mapeo Encefálico , Confusión/etiología , Confusión/patología , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética
10.
Cereb Cortex ; 34(3)2024 03 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38494889

RESUMEN

A recent neuroimaging study in adults found that the occipital place area (OPA)-a cortical region involved in "visually guided navigation" (i.e. moving about the immediately visible environment, avoiding boundaries, and obstacles)-represents visual information about walking, not crawling, suggesting that OPA is late developing, emerging only when children are walking, not beforehand. But when precisely does this "walking selectivity" in OPA emerge-when children first begin to walk in early childhood, or perhaps counterintuitively, much later in childhood, around 8 years of age, when children are adult-like walking? To directly test these two hypotheses, using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) in two groups of children, 5- and 8-year-olds, we measured the responses in OPA to first-person perspective videos through scenes from a "walking" perspective, as well as three control perspectives ("crawling," "flying," and "scrambled"). We found that the OPA in 8-year-olds-like adults-exhibited walking selectivity (i.e. responding significantly more to the walking videos than to any of the others, and no significant differences across the crawling, flying, and scrambled videos), while the OPA in 5-year-olds exhibited no walking selectively. These findings reveal that OPA undergoes protracted development, with walking selectivity only emerging around 8 years of age.


Asunto(s)
Mapeo Encefálico , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Niño , Preescolar , Humanos , Mapeo Encefálico/métodos , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética/métodos , Neuroimagen , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Caminata
11.
Cereb Cortex ; 34(7)2024 Jul 03.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39016432

RESUMEN

Sound is an important navigational cue for mammals. During spatial navigation, hippocampal place cells encode spatial representations of the environment based on visual information, but to what extent audiospatial information can enable reliable place cell mapping is largely unknown. We assessed this by recording from CA1 place cells in the dark, under circumstances where reliable visual, tactile, or olfactory information was unavailable. Male rats were exposed to auditory cues of different frequencies that were delivered from local or distal spatial locations. We observed that distal, but not local cue presentation, enables and supports stable place fields, regardless of the sound frequency used. Our data suggest that a context dependency exists regarding the relevance of auditory information for place field mapping: whereas locally available auditory cues do not serve as a salient spatial basis for the anchoring of place fields, auditory cue localization supports spatial representations by place cells when available in the form of distal information. Furthermore, our results demonstrate that CA1 neurons can effectively use auditory stimuli to generate place fields, and that hippocampal pyramidal neurons are not solely dependent on visual cues for the generation of place field representations based on allocentric reference frames.


Asunto(s)
Estimulación Acústica , Señales (Psicología) , Células de Lugar , Ratas Long-Evans , Percepción Espacial , Animales , Masculino , Células de Lugar/fisiología , Percepción Espacial/fisiología , Región CA1 Hipocampal/fisiología , Región CA1 Hipocampal/citología , Ratas , Percepción Auditiva/fisiología , Potenciales de Acción/fisiología , Navegación Espacial/fisiología
12.
Cell Mol Life Sci ; 81(1): 147, 2024 Mar 19.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38502309

RESUMEN

GABAergic interneurons are poised with the capacity to shape circuit output via inhibitory gating. How early in the development of medial vestibular nucleus (MVN) are GABAergic neurons recruited for feedforward shaping of outputs to higher centers for spatial navigation? The role of early GABAergic transmission in assembling vestibular circuits for spatial navigation was explored by neonatal perturbation. Immunohistochemistry and confocal imaging were utilized to reveal the expression of parvalbumin (PV)-expressing MVN neurons and their perineuronal nets. Whole-cell patch-clamp recording, coupled with optogenetics, was conducted in vitro to examine the synaptic function of MVN circuitry. Chemogenetic targeting strategy was also employed in vivo to manipulate neuronal activity during navigational tests. We found in rats a neonatal critical period before postnatal day (P) 8 in which competitive antagonization of GABAergic transmission in the MVN retarded maturation of inhibitory neurotransmission, as evidenced by deranged developmental trajectory for excitation/inhibition ratio and an extended period of critical period-like plasticity in GABAergic transmission. Despite increased number of PV-expressing GABAergic interneurons in the MVN, optogenetic-coupled patch-clamp recording indicated null-recruitment of these neurons in tuning outputs along the ascending vestibular pathway. Such perturbation not only offset output dynamics of ascending MVN output neurons, but was further accompanied by impaired vestibular-dependent navigation in adulthood. The same perturbations were however non-consequential when applied after P8. Results highlight neonatal GABAergic transmission as key to establishing feedforward output dynamics to higher brain centers for spatial cognition and navigation.


Asunto(s)
Navegación Espacial , Ratas , Animales , Interneuronas , Transmisión Sináptica , Núcleos Vestibulares/metabolismo , Neuronas GABAérgicas
13.
Neurobiol Dis ; 190: 106378, 2024 Jan.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38103701

RESUMEN

Spatial navigation critically underlies hippocampal-entorhinal circuit function that is early affected in Alzheimer's disease (AD). There is growing evidence that AD pathophysiology dynamically interacts with the sleep/wake cycle impairing hippocampal memory. To elucidate sleep-dependent consolidation in a cohort of symptomatic AD patients (n = 12, 71.25 ± 2.16 years), we tested hippocampal place learning by means of a virtual reality task and verbal memory by a word-pair association task before and after a night of sleep. Our results show an impaired overnight memory retention in AD compared with controls in the verbal task, together with a significant reduction of sleep spindle activity (i.e., lower amplitude of fast sleep spindles, p = 0.016) and increased duration of the slow oscillation (SO; p = 0.019). Higher spindle density, faster down-to-upstate transitions within SOs, and the time delay between SOs and nested spindles predicted better memory performance in healthy controls but not in AD patients. Our results show that mnemonic processing and memory consolidation in AD is slightly impaired as reflected by dysfunctional oscillatory dynamics and spindle-SO coupling during NonREM sleep. In this translational study based on experimental paradigms in animals and extending previous work in healthy aging and preclinical disease stages, our results in symptomatic AD further deepen the understanding of the memory decline within a bidirectional relationship of sleep and AD pathology.


Asunto(s)
Enfermedad de Alzheimer , Consolidación de la Memoria , Humanos , Consolidación de la Memoria/fisiología , Polisomnografía , Sueño/fisiología , Memoria/fisiología , Trastornos de la Memoria/etiología
14.
Hippocampus ; 34(7): 310-326, 2024 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38721743

RESUMEN

Classic research has shown a division in the neuroanatomical structures that support flexible (e.g., short-cutting) and habitual (e.g., familiar route following) navigational behavior, with hippocampal-caudate systems associated with the former and putamen systems with the latter. There is, however, disagreement about whether the neural structures involved in navigation process particular forms of spatial information, such as associations between constellations of cues forming a cognitive map, versus single landmark-action associations, or alternatively, perform particular reinforcement learning algorithms that allow the use of different spatial strategies, so-called model-based (flexible) or model-free (habitual) forms of learning. We sought to test these theories by asking participants (N = 24) to navigate within a virtual environment through a previously learned, 9-junction route with distinctive landmarks at each junction while undergoing functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). In a series of probe trials, we distinguished knowledge of individual landmark-action associations along the route versus knowledge of the correct sequence of landmark-action associations, either by having absent landmarks, or "out-of-sequence" landmarks. Under a map-based perspective, sequence knowledge would not require hippocampal systems, because there are no constellations of cues available for cognitive map formation. Within a learning-based model, however, responding based on knowledge of sequence would require hippocampal systems because prior context has to be utilized. We found that hippocampal-caudate systems were more active in probes requiring sequence knowledge, supporting the learning-based model. However, we also found greater putamen activation in probes where navigation based purely on sequence memory could be planned, supporting models of putamen function that emphasize its role in action sequencing.


Asunto(s)
Hipocampo , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Navegación Espacial , Humanos , Navegación Espacial/fisiología , Hipocampo/fisiología , Hipocampo/diagnóstico por imagen , Masculino , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética/métodos , Femenino , Adulto Joven , Adulto , Cuerpo Estriado/fisiología , Cuerpo Estriado/diagnóstico por imagen , Mapeo Encefálico/métodos , Realidad Virtual , Señales (Psicología)
15.
Hippocampus ; 34(4): 204-216, 2024 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38214182

RESUMEN

Developmental topographical disorientation (DTD) refers to the lifelong inability to orient by means of cognitive maps in familiar surroundings despite otherwise well-preserved general cognitive functions, and the absence of any acquired brain injury or neurological condition. While reduced functional connectivity between the hippocampus and other brain regions has been reported in DTD individuals, no structural differences in gray matter tissue for the whole brain neither for the hippocampus were detected. Considering that the human hippocampus is the main structure associated with cognitive map-based navigation, here, we investigated differences in morphological and morphometric hippocampal features between individuals affected by DTD (N = 20) and healthy controls (N = 238). Specifically, we focused on a developmental anomaly of the hippocampus that is characterized by the incomplete infolding of hippocampal subfields during fetal development, giving the hippocampus a more round or pyramidal shape, called incomplete hippocampal inversion (IHI). We rated IHI according to standard criteria and extracted hippocampal subfield volumes after FreeSurfer's automatic segmentation. We observed similar IHI prevalence in the group of individuals with DTD with respect to the control population. Neither differences in whole hippocampal nor major hippocampal subfield volumes have been observed between groups. However, when assessing the IHI independent criteria, we observed that the hippocampus in the DTD group is more medially positioned comparing to the control group. In addition, we observed bigger hippocampal fissure volume for the DTD comparing to the control group. Both of these findings were stronger for the right hippocampus comparing to the left. Our results provide new insights regarding the hippocampal morphology of individuals affected by DTD, highlighting the role of structural anomalies during early prenatal development in line with the developmental nature of the spatial disorientation deficit.


Asunto(s)
Confusión , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Humanos , Encéfalo , Hipocampo/diagnóstico por imagen , Lóbulo Temporal
16.
Hippocampus ; 34(4): 168-196, 2024 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38178693

RESUMEN

Head direction (HD) cells, which fire persistently when an animal's head is pointed in a particular direction, are widely thought to underlie an animal's sense of spatial orientation and have been identified in several limbic brain regions. Robust HD cell firing is observed throughout the thalamo-parahippocampal system, although recent studies report that parahippocampal HD cells exhibit distinct firing properties, including conjunctive aspects with other spatial parameters, which suggest they play a specialized role in spatial processing. Few studies, however, have quantified these apparent differences. Here, we performed a comparative assessment of HD cell firing characteristics across the anterior dorsal thalamus (ADN), postsubiculum (PoS), parasubiculum (PaS), medial entorhinal (MEC), and postrhinal (POR) cortices. We report that HD cells with a high degree of directional specificity were observed in all five brain regions, but ADN HD cells display greater sharpness and stability in their preferred directions, and greater anticipation of future headings compared to parahippocampal regions. Additional analysis indicated that POR HD cells were more coarsely modulated by other spatial parameters compared to PoS, PaS, and MEC. Finally, our analyses indicated that the sharpness of HD tuning decreased as a function of laminar position and conjunctive coding within the PoS, PaS, and MEC, with cells in the superficial layers along with conjunctive firing properties showing less robust directional tuning. The results are discussed in relation to theories of functional organization of HD cell tuning in thalamo-parahippocampal circuitry.


Asunto(s)
Núcleos Talámicos Anteriores , Giro Parahipocampal , Animales , Giro Parahipocampal/fisiología , Corteza Cerebral , Percepción Espacial , Cabeza/fisiología
17.
Kidney Int ; 2024 Jul 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38959996

RESUMEN

Patient navigators enable adult patients to circumnavigate complex health systems, improving access to health care and outcomes. Here, we aimed to evaluate the effects of a patient navigation program in children with chronic kidney disease (CKD). In this multi-center, randomized controlled trial, we randomly assigned children (aged 0-16 years) with CKD stages 1-5 (including children on dialysis or with kidney transplants), from low socioeconomic status backgrounds, and/or residing in remote areas, to receive patient navigation at randomization (immediate) or at six months (waitlist). The primary outcome was self-rated health (SRH) of participating children at six months, using intention to treat analysis. Secondary outcomes included caregivers' SRH and satisfaction with health care, children's quality of life, hospitalizations, and missed school days. Repeated measures of the primary outcome from baseline to six months were analyzed using cumulative logit mixed effects models. Semi-structured interviews were thematically evaluated. Of 398 screened children, 162 were randomized (80 immediate and 82 waitlist); mean age (standard deviation) of 8.8 (4.8) years with 64.8% male. SRH was not significantly different between the immediate and wait-listed groups at six months. There were also no differences across all secondary outcomes between the two groups. Caregivers' perspectives were reflected in seven themes: easing mental strain, facilitating care coordination, strengthening capacity to provide care, reinforcing care collaborations, alleviating family tensions, inability to build rapport and unnecessary support. Thus, in children with CKD, self-rated health may not improve in response to a navigator program, but caregivers gained skills related to providing and accessing care.

18.
Cancer ; 130(9): 1549-1567, 2024 May 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38306297

RESUMEN

PLAIN LANGUAGE SUMMARY: Cancer patient navigators work in diverse settings ranging from community-based programs to comprehensive cancer centers to improve outcomes in underserved populations by eliminating barriers to timely cancer prevention, early detection, diagnosis, treatment, and survivorship in a culturally appropriate and competent manner. This article clarifies the roles and responsibilities of Entry, Intermediate, and Advanced level cancer patient navigators. The competencies described in this article apply to patient navigators, nurse navigators, and social work navigators. This article provides a resource for administrators to create job descriptions for navigators with specific levels of expertise and for patient navigators to advance their oncology careers and attain a higher level of expertise.


Asunto(s)
Neoplasias , Navegación de Pacientes , Humanos , Atención a la Salud , Neoplasias/diagnóstico , Neoplasias/terapia , Recursos Humanos
19.
Eur J Neurosci ; 59(10): 2522-2534, 2024 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38650479

RESUMEN

Dopamine neurons signal the salience of environmental stimuli and influence learning, although it is less clear if these neurons also determine the salience of memories. Ventral tegmental area (VTA) dopamine neurons increase their firing in the presence of new objects and reduce it upon repeated, inconsequential exposures, marking the shift from novelty to familiarity. This study investigates how dopamine neuron activity during repeated familiar object exposure affects an animal's preference for new objects in a subsequent novel object recognition (NOR) test. We hypothesize that a single familiarization session will not sufficiently lower dopamine activity, such that the memory of a familiar object remains salient, leading to equal exploration of familiar and novel objects and weaker NOR discrimination. In contrast, multiple familiarization sessions likely suppress dopamine activity more effectively, reducing the salience of the familiar object and enhancing subsequent novelty discrimination. Our experiments in mice indicated that multiple familiarization sessions reduce VTA dopamine neuron activation, as measured by c-Fos expression, and enhance novelty discrimination compared with a single familiarization session. Dopamine neurons that show responsiveness to novelty were primarily located in the paranigral nucleus of the VTA and expressed vesicular glutamate transporter 2 transcripts, marking them as dopamine-glutamate neurons. Chemogenetic inhibition of dopamine neurons during a single session paralleled the effects of multiple sessions, improving NOR. These findings suggest that a critical role of dopamine neurons during the transition from novelty to familiarity is to modulate the salience of an object's memory.


Asunto(s)
Neuronas Dopaminérgicas , Ratones Endogámicos C57BL , Reconocimiento en Psicología , Área Tegmental Ventral , Animales , Reconocimiento en Psicología/fisiología , Neuronas Dopaminérgicas/fisiología , Neuronas Dopaminérgicas/metabolismo , Área Tegmental Ventral/fisiología , Ratones , Masculino , Proteínas Proto-Oncogénicas c-fos/metabolismo , Proteína 2 de Transporte Vesicular de Glutamato/metabolismo , Proteína 2 de Transporte Vesicular de Glutamato/genética
20.
J Hepatol ; 80(5): 702-713, 2024 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38242324

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND & AIMS: Direct-acting antivirals (DAAs) are highly effective for treating HCV infection even among people who inject drugs (PWID). Yet, little is known about patients' adherence patterns and their association with sustained virologic response (SVR) rates. We aimed to summarize various adherence patterns and determine their associations with SVR. METHODS: Electronic blister packs were used to measure daily adherence to once-a-day sofosbuvir/velpatasvir during the 12-week treatment period among active PWIDs. Blister pack data were available for 496 participants who initiated DAAs for whom SVR status was known. Adherence was summarized in multiple patterns, such as total adherent days, consecutive missed days, and early discontinuations. Thresholds for adherence patterns associated with >90% SVR rates were also determined. RESULTS: The overall SVR rate was 92.7%, with a median adherence rate of 75%. All adherence patterns indicating greater adherence were significantly associated with achieving SVR. Participant groups with ≥50% (>42/84) adherent days or <26 consecutive missed days achieved an SVR rate of >90%. Greater total adherent days during 9-12 weeks and no early discontinuation were significantly associated with higher SVR rates only in those with <50% adherence. Participants with first month discontinuation and ≥2 weeks of treatment interruption had low SVR rates, 25% and 85%, respectively. However, greater adherent days were significantly associated with SVR (adjusted odds ratio 1.10; 95% CI 1.04-1.16; p <0.001) even among participants with ≥14 consecutive missed days. CONCLUSIONS: High SVR rates can be achieved in the PWID population despite suboptimal adherence. Encouraging patients to take as much medication as possible, with <2 weeks consecutive missed days and without early discontinuation, was found to be important for achieving SVR. IMPACT AND IMPLICATIONS: People who inject drugs can be cured of HCV in >90% of cases, even with relatively low adherence to direct-acting antivirals, but early discontinuations and long treatment interruptions can significantly reduce the likelihood of achieving cure. Clinicians should encourage people who inject drugs who are living with HCV to adhere daily to direct-acting antivirals as consistently as possible, but if any days are interrupted, to continue and complete treatment. These results from the HERO study are important for patients living with HCV, clinicians, experts writing clinical guidelines, and payers. CLINICAL TRIAL NUMBER: NCT02824640.


Asunto(s)
Consumidores de Drogas , Hepatitis C Crónica , Hepatitis C , Abuso de Sustancias por Vía Intravenosa , Humanos , Antivirales/uso terapéutico , Hepacivirus , Hepatitis C/epidemiología , Hepatitis C Crónica/tratamiento farmacológico , Abuso de Sustancias por Vía Intravenosa/complicaciones , Abuso de Sustancias por Vía Intravenosa/tratamiento farmacológico , Abuso de Sustancias por Vía Intravenosa/epidemiología , Respuesta Virológica Sostenida , Cumplimiento y Adherencia al Tratamiento
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA