Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 20 de 42
Filter
1.
Cerebellum ; 17(3): 300-307, 2018 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29248983

ABSTRACT

The vestibulo-ocular reflex maintains gaze stabilization during angular or linear head accelerations, allowing adequate dynamic visual acuity. In case of bilateral vestibular hypofunction, patients use saccades to compensate for the reduced vestibulo-ocular reflex function, with covert saccades occurring even during the head displacement. In this study, we questioned whether covert saccades help maintain dynamic visual acuity, and evaluated which characteristic of these saccades are the most relevant to improve visual function. We prospectively included 18 patients with chronic bilateral vestibular hypofunction. Subjects underwent evaluation of dynamic visual acuity in the horizontal plane as well as video recording of their head and eye positions during horizontal head impulse tests in both directions (36 ears tested). Frequency, latency, consistency of covert saccade initiation, and gain of covert saccades as well as residual vestibulo-ocular reflex gain were calculated. We found no correlation between residual vestibulo-ocular reflex gain and dynamic visual acuity. Dynamic visual acuity performance was however positively correlated with the frequency and gain of covert saccades and negatively correlated with covert saccade latency. There was no correlation between consistency of covert saccade initiation and dynamic visual acuity. Even though gaze stabilization in space during covert saccades might be of very short duration, these refixation saccades seem to improve vision in patients with bilateral vestibular hypofunction during angular head impulses. These findings emphasize the need for specific rehabilitation technics that favor the triggering of covert saccades. The physiological origin of covert saccades is discussed.


Subject(s)
Reflex, Vestibulo-Ocular , Saccades , Vestibular Diseases/physiopathology , Adult , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Eye Movement Measurements , Female , Head Impulse Test , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Prospective Studies , Visual Acuity , Young Adult
2.
Ann Phys Rehabil Med ; 58(2): 40-53, 2015 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25543183

ABSTRACT

Spatial neglect (SN) is commonly associated with poor functional outcome. Adaptation to a rightward optical deviation of vision has been shown to benefit to SN rehabilitation. The neurophysiological foundations and the optimal modalities of prism adaptation (PA) therapy however remain to be validated. This study is aimed at exploring the long-term sensory-motor, cognitive and functional effects produced by weekly PA sessions over a period of four weeks. A double-blind, monocentric randomized and controlled trial (RCT) was carried out. Twenty patients with left SN secondary to stroke were included, 10 in the "prism" group and 10 in the "control" group. The sensory-motor effects of PA were evaluated by measurement of manual and visual straight-ahead, and also by precision of pointing without visual feedback before and after each PA session. The functional independence measure (FIM) was evaluated before and at 1, 3 and 6 months after PA, while SN severity was assessed using the Behavioural Inattention Test (BIT) before and 6 months after PA. Before the intervention, only manual straight-ahead pointing constituted a reproducible sensory-motor measurement. During prism exposure, a questionnaire showed that not a single patient were aware of the direct effects of optical deviation on pointing movement performance. The sensory-motor after-effects produced by the PA produced a more rapid reduction of the rightward manual straight-ahead, which was secondarily followed by visual straight-ahead. These sensory-motor effects helped to clarify the action mechanisms of PA on SN. At the conclusion of the 6-month follow-up, the two groups showed similar improvement, indicating that a weekly PA session over 4 weeks was not sufficient to produce long-term functional benefit. This improvement was correlated with the evolution of visual straight-ahead, which can be proposed as a marker for patients outcome.


Subject(s)
Adaptation, Physiological/physiology , Perceptual Disorders/rehabilitation , Psychomotor Performance , Space Perception , Visual Perception , Adult , Aged , Attention/physiology , Double-Blind Method , Feedback, Sensory , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Perceptual Disorders/etiology , Perceptual Disorders/physiopathology , Stroke/complications , Treatment Outcome
3.
Exp Brain Res ; 231(1): 1-11, 2013 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23963603

ABSTRACT

The cerebellum is critically involved in the adaptation mechanisms that maintain the accuracy of goal-directed acts such as saccadic eye movements. Two categories of saccades, each relying on different adaptation mechanisms, are defined: reactive (externally triggered) saccades and voluntary (internally triggered) saccades. The contribution of the medio-posterior part of the cerebellum to reactive saccades adaptation has been clearly demonstrated, but the evidence that other parts of the cerebellum are also involved is limited. Moreover, the cerebellar substrates of voluntary saccades adaptation have only been marginally investigated. Here, we addressed these two questions by investigating the adaptive capabilities of patients with cerebellar or pre-cerebellar stroke. We recruited three groups of patients presenting focal lesions located, respectively, in the supero-anterior cerebellum, the infero-posterior cerebellum and the lateral medulla (leading to a Wallenberg syndrome including motor dysfunctions similar to those resulting from lesion of the medio-posterior cerebellum). Adaptations of reactive saccades and of voluntary saccades were tested during separate sessions in all patients and in a group of healthy participants. The functional lesion of the medio-posterior cerebellum in Wallenberg syndrome strongly impaired the adaptation of both reactive and voluntary saccades. In contrast, patients with lesion in the supero-anterior part of the cerebellum presented a specific adaptation deficit of voluntary saccades. Finally, patients with an infero-posterior cerebellar lesion showed mild adaptation deficits. We conclude that the medio-posterior cerebellum is critical for the adaptation of both saccade categories, whereas the supero-anterior cerebellum is specifically involved in the adaptation of voluntary saccades.


Subject(s)
Adaptation, Physiological/physiology , Cerebellar Diseases/psychology , Saccades/physiology , Adult , Cerebellar Diseases/physiopathology , Data Interpretation, Statistical , Female , Functional Laterality/physiology , Humans , Lateral Medullary Syndrome/physiopathology , Lateral Medullary Syndrome/psychology , Male , Medulla Oblongata/physiopathology , Middle Aged , Neurologic Examination , Photic Stimulation , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Spinocerebellar Ataxias/physiopathology , Spinocerebellar Ataxias/psychology , Stroke/physiopathology , Stroke/psychology
4.
J Fr Ophtalmol ; 35(4): 242-50, 2012 Apr.
Article in French | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22018708

ABSTRACT

INTRODUCTION: Voluntary or reactive saccades predominate in rapid eye movements. Their goal is to preserve an active and optimal visual perception of the environment. Saccades cannot be guided once launched. Oculomotor plasticity, or saccadic adaptation, is still partially unknown, in particular the role played by the basal ganglia. New neuro-ophthalmological rehabilitation techniques require understanding the neurophysiological basis and demonstrating the neuronal structures involved in this plasticity. OBJECTIVES: This study assessed the reactive saccade adaptation in patients with idiopathic Parkinson disease, as a model of basal ganglia dysfunction. We predicted that saccadic adaptation would be preserved in this pathology. PATIENTS AND METHODS: Five patients with mild idiopathic hemi-Parkinson disease were included, as well as four age-matched controls. Reactive saccade adaptation was studied using the double-step target paradigm, in patients with OFF-Dopa treatment and in controls. RESULTS: Group analysis demonstrated that patients had a lower level of adaptation than the controls (p<0.05). Individually, two patients did not adapt for bilateral saccades and one for ipsilateral (compared to Parkinson motor clinical syndrome) saccades. Two additional patients adapted on both sides but with a deficit in contralateral saccades when compared to the control group. DISCUSSION: These preliminary results suggest basal ganglia involvement in reactive saccadic adaptation, which remains to be clarified.


Subject(s)
Neuronal Plasticity/physiology , Parkinson Disease/physiopathology , Saccades/physiology , Adaptation, Physiological/physiology , Aged , Female , Humans , Individuality , Male , Middle Aged , Models, Biological , Parkinson Disease/psychology , Reaction Time , Visual Perception/physiology
5.
Ann N Y Acad Sci ; 1233: 327-34, 2011 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21951012

ABSTRACT

Abnormal eye movements in multiple sclerosis (MS) are often persistent and known to be associated with general disability. However, there is no precise knowledge concerning their incidence and resulting visual handicap. The aim of our study was to describe the persistent ocular motor manifestations in MS and relate them to visual functions tested with visual acuity and with a vision-related questionnaire. We selected 24 MS patients complaining of persistent visual disability associated with ocular motor manifestations without any anterior visual pathway deficit. Internuclear ophthalmoplegia was the most frequently observed symptom, followed by gaze-evoked nystagmus, saccadic hypermetria, and then pendular nystagmus. Pendular nystagmus, saccadic hypermetria, and the association of internuclear ophthalmoplegia and gaze-evoked nystagmus were associated with decreased visual acuity and visual functional scores. There was a correlation between the number of abnormal eye movements and visual functions. This study demonstrates that ocular motor dysfunction in MS induces specific visual dysfunction and handicap.


Subject(s)
Multiple Sclerosis/complications , Multiple Sclerosis/physiopathology , Ocular Motility Disorders/etiology , Ocular Motility Disorders/physiopathology , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Nystagmus, Pathologic/etiology , Nystagmus, Pathologic/physiopathology , Surveys and Questionnaires , Visual Acuity
6.
Neurology ; 76(19): 1650-7, 2011 May 10.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21555732

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVE: Acquired pendular nystagmus occurs mainly in multiple sclerosis (MS) and focal brainstem lesions. In the later case, it is part of the syndrome of oculopalatal tremor. Even though pathophysiology of acquired pendular nystagmus has been clearly characterized experimentally in both etiologies, there is a persisting ambiguity in clinical literature, which leads one to consider both clinical conditions as a common entity. The objective of our work was to compare in a prospective study clinical features, eye movement recording, and functional consequences of acquired pendular nystagmus in 14 patients with oculopalatal tremor and 20 patients with MS. METHODS: Besides complete neurologic evaluation, evaluation of visual function, 3-dimensional eye movement recording, and functional scores of the Visual Function Questionnaire were recorded. RESULTS: One patient with oculopalatal tremor and 15 patients with MS disclosed signs of optic neuropathy. The nystagmus in the oculopalatal group showed significant larger mean amplitude (8 deg vs 1 deg), higher mean peak velocity (16 deg/s vs 6 deg/s), lower mean frequency (1-3 Hz vs 4-6 Hz), and larger asymmetry and irregularity of ocular oscillations compared to the MS group. The vision-specific health-related quality of life was more deteriorated in the oculopalatal tremor group than in the MS group. CONCLUSIONS: This study emphasizes the need to consider acquired pendular nystagmus in MS and oculopalatal tremor as 2 different clinical entities. This is of particular importance regarding the future evaluation of potential specific effects of pharmacologic agents.


Subject(s)
Eye Movements/physiology , Multiple Sclerosis/complications , Myoclonus/complications , Nystagmus, Pathologic/diagnosis , Nystagmus, Pathologic/etiology , Adult , Diagnostic Techniques, Ophthalmological , Female , Fourier Analysis , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Multiple Sclerosis/psychology , Myoclonus/psychology , Nystagmus, Pathologic/psychology , Prospective Studies , Quality of Life , Retrospective Studies , Statistics as Topic , Vision, Ocular/physiology
7.
Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci ; 366(1564): 572-85, 2011 Feb 27.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21242144

ABSTRACT

We review evidence showing a right-hemispheric dominance for visuo-spatial processing and representation in humans. Accordingly, visual disorganization symptoms (intuitively related to remapping impairments) are observed in both neglect and constructional apraxia. More specifically, we review findings from the intervening saccade paradigm in humans--and present additional original data--which suggest a specific role of the asymmetrical network at the temporo-parietal junction (TPJ) in the right hemisphere in visual remapping: following damage to the right dorsal posterior parietal cortex (PPC) as well as part of the corpus callosum connecting the PPC to the frontal lobes, patient OK in a double-step saccadic task exhibited an impairment when the second saccade had to be directed rightward. This singular and lateralized deficit cannot result solely from the patient's cortical lesion and, therefore, we propose that it is due to his callosal lesion that may specifically interrupt the interhemispheric transfer of information necessary to execute accurate rightward saccades towards a remapped target location. This suggests a specialized right-hemispheric network for visuo-spatial remapping that subsequently transfers target location information to downstream planning regions, which are symmetrically organized.


Subject(s)
Parietal Lobe/physiology , Saccades/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Agnosia/physiopathology , Animals , Apraxias/physiopathology , Haplorhini , Humans , Parietal Lobe/physiopathology , Space Perception/physiology
8.
Neurosci Biobehav Rev ; 34(8): 1103-20, 2010 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20026351

ABSTRACT

Sensory-motor adaptation mechanisms play a pivotal role in maintaining the performance of goal-directed movements. The saccadic system, used to explore the visual environment through fast and accurate shifts of the eyes (saccades), is a valuable model for studying adaptation mechanisms. Significant progresses have been recently made in identifying the properties and neural substrates of saccadic adaptation elicited by the double-step target paradigm. Behavioural data collected in healthy and brain-damaged subjects, and neurophysiological data from non human primates, will be reviewed in an attempt to build a coherent picture of saccadic adaptation mechanisms. Emphasis will further be put on the contextual factors of saccadic adaptation, and on the link between adaptive changes of oculomotor commands and visual perception. It will be shown that saccadic adaptation relies on multiple mechanisms according to experimental contexts, time-scales, saccade categories, and direction of adaptive changes of saccade amplitude (shortening versus lengthening). Taking into account this complexity will be a key toward a comprehensive understanding of the physiopathology of saccadic adaptation and toward the development of possible rehabilitation procedures.


Subject(s)
Adaptation, Physiological/physiology , Feedback, Sensory/physiology , Saccades/physiology , Animals , Humans , Models, Biological , Neuronal Plasticity/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology
9.
Cerebellum ; 7(4): 595-601, 2008.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19009327

ABSTRACT

Sensory-motor adaptation processes are critically involved in maintaining accurate motor behavior throughout life. Yet their underlying neural substrates and task-dependency bases are still poorly understood. We address these issues here by studying adaptation of saccadic eye movements, a well-established model of sensory-motor plasticity. The cerebellum plays a major role in saccadic adaptation but it has not yet been investigated whether this role can account for the known specificity of adaptation to the saccade type (e.g., reactive versus voluntary). Two patients with focal lesions in different parts of the cerebellum were tested using the double-step target paradigm. Each patient was submitted to two separate sessions: one for reactive saccades (RS) triggered by the sudden appearance of a visual target and the second for scanning voluntary saccades (SVS) performed when exploring a more complex scene. We found that a medial cerebellar lesion impaired adaptation of reactive-but not of voluntary-saccades, whereas a lateral lesion affected adaptation of scanning voluntary saccades, but not of reactive saccades. These findings provide the first evidence of an involvement of the lateral cerebellum in saccadic adaptation, and extend the demonstrated role of the cerebellum in RS adaptation to adaptation of SVS. The double dissociation of adaptive abilities is also consistent with our previous hypothesis of the involvement in saccadic adaptation of partially separated cerebellar areas specific to the reactive or voluntary task (Alahyane et al. Brain Res 1135:107-121 (2007)).


Subject(s)
Acclimatization/physiology , Cerebellum/physiology , Cerebellum/physiopathology , Fixation, Ocular/physiology , Motor Activity/physiology , Neurons/physiology , Saccades/physiology , Stroke/physiopathology , Adult , Ataxia/etiology , Ataxia/physiopathology , Cerebral Infarction/physiopathology , Functional Laterality , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Neuronal Plasticity/physiology , Visual Perception
10.
Rev Neurol (Paris) ; 163(4): 421-39, 2007 Apr.
Article in French | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17452944

ABSTRACT

Oscillopsia is an illusion of an unstable visual world. It is associated with poor visual acuity and is a disabling and stressful symptom reported by numerous patients with neurological disorders. The goal of this paper is to review the physiology of the systems subserving stable vision, the various pathophysiological mechanisms of oscillopsia and the different treatments available. Visual stability is conditioned by two factors. First, images of the seen world projected onto the retina have to be stable, a sine qua non condition for foveal discriminative function. Vestibulo-ocular and optokinetic reflexes act to stabilize the retinal images during head displacements; ocular fixation tends to limit the occurrence of micro ocular movements during gazing; a specific system also acts to maintain the eyes stable during eccentric gaze. Second, although we voluntary move our gaze (body, head and eye displacements), the visual world is normally perceived as stable, a phenomenon known as space constancy. Indeed, complex cognitive processes compensate for the two sensory consequences of gaze displacement, namely an oppositely-directed retinal drift and a change in the relationship between retinal and spatial (or subject-centered) coordinates of the visual scene. In patients, oscillopsia most often results from abnormal eye movements which cause excessive motion of images on the retina, such as nystagmus or saccadic intrusions or from an impaired vestibulo-ocular reflex. Understanding the exact mechanisms of impaired eye stability may lead to the different treatment options that have been documented in recent years. Oscillopsia could also result from an impairment of spatial constancy mechanisms that in normal condition compensate for gaze displacements, but clinical data in this case are scarce. However, we suggest that some visuo-perceptive deficits consecutive to temporo-parietal lesions resemble oscillopsia and could result from a deficit in elaborating spatial constancy.


Subject(s)
Ocular Motility Disorders/physiopathology , Ocular Motility Disorders/therapy , Adaptation, Ocular/physiology , Fixation, Ocular/physiology , Humans , Nervous System Diseases/complications , Nystagmus, Pathologic/physiopathology , Ocular Motility Disorders/etiology , Reflex, Vestibulo-Ocular/physiology , Saccades/physiology
11.
Neurology ; 65(1): 150-2, 2005 Jul 12.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16009906

ABSTRACT

In a patient with damage of the left cerebellar cortex (SCA territory), the authors tested four combinations of exposure to optical shift (leftward prisms, right hand; rightward prisms, right hand; leftward prisms, left (ataxic) hand; rightward prisms, left (ataxic) hand). He adapted to rightward but not leftward prisms, independent of which hand was used during exposure. This suggests a role of anterior cerebellar cortex in the computation or compensation of ipsidirectional visual error.


Subject(s)
Cerebellar Diseases/physiopathology , Cerebellum/physiopathology , Perceptual Disorders/physiopathology , Stroke/physiopathology , Adaptation, Physiological/physiology , Adult , Arm/innervation , Arm/physiopathology , Basilar Artery/pathology , Basilar Artery/physiopathology , Cerebellar Ataxia/diagnosis , Cerebellar Ataxia/etiology , Cerebellar Ataxia/physiopathology , Cerebellar Diseases/diagnosis , Cerebellar Diseases/etiology , Cerebellum/blood supply , Cerebellum/pathology , Functional Laterality/physiology , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Neural Pathways/pathology , Neural Pathways/physiopathology , Perceptual Disorders/diagnosis , Perceptual Disorders/etiology , Photic Stimulation , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Space Perception/physiology , Stroke/diagnosis
12.
Eur J Neurosci ; 14(8): 1331-44, 2001 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11703462

ABSTRACT

Several studies have suggested that the pattern of neuronal activity in the superior colliculus (SC) interacts with the well-known topographical coding of saccades (motor map). To further describe this interaction, we recorded gaze saccades evoked by electrical microstimulation of SC deeper layers in the head-unrestrained cat and systematically varied the collicular locus (25 sites) and parameters (intensity, frequency) of the stimulation. Long stimulation trains were used to avoid saccade truncation. We found that the direction and amplitude of evoked gaze shifts were related to the stimulation locus, describing a gaze shift map. For 18 out of 20 sites the amplitude, but not the direction, also strongly depended on stimulation strength. Indeed, gaze amplitude continuously increased when raising current intensity up to several times the threshold value T (the largest intensity tested was 6 x T), whereas varying pulse frequency from 150 to 750 pulses per second (p.p.s.) revealed an optimal frequency range (300 and 500 p.p.s.) eliciting the largest gaze shifts. Moreover, the intensity effect on amplitude increased in an orderly fashion with the rostro-caudal stimulation locus. Gaze shift amplitude was not related to the number of delivered stimulation pulses. Concerning movement initiation, increasing either intensity or frequency led to an exponential decrease in gaze latency until minimal values near 30 ms were reached, but the number of pulses delivered during the corresponding latency period remained constant within a 300-500 p.p.s. frequency range. These findings indicate that the pattern of collicular discharge evoked by electrical stimulation strongly interacts with the gaze shift map and provide evidence for a summation of collicular activities by downstream premotor neurons.


Subject(s)
Efferent Pathways/physiology , Motion Perception/physiology , Neurons/physiology , Orientation/physiology , Saccades/physiology , Superior Colliculi/physiology , Animals , Brain Mapping , Cats , Electric Stimulation , Photic Stimulation , Reaction Time/physiology
13.
Eur J Neurosci ; 14(8): 1345-59, 2001 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11703463

ABSTRACT

The medioposterior cerebellum [vermian lobules VI and VII and caudal fastigial nucleus (cFN)] is known to play a major role in the control of saccadic gaze shifts toward a visual target. To determine the relative contribution of the cFN efferent pathways to the brainstem reticular formation and to the superior colliculus (SC), we recorded in the head-unrestrained cat the effects of cFN unilateral inactivation on gaze shifts evoked by electrical microstimulation of the deeper SC layers. Gaze shifts evoked after muscimol injection still exhibited the typical qualitative features of normal saccadic gaze shifts. Nevertheless, consistent modifications in amplitude and latency were observed. For ipsiversive movements (evoked by the SC contralateral to the inactivated cFN), these changes depended on the locus of stimulation on the motor map: for the anterior 2/3 of the SC, amplitude increased and latency tended to decrease; for the posterior 1/3 of the SC, amplitude decreased and latency increased. For the contraversive direction, amplitude moderately decreased and latency tended to increase for all but the caudal-most stimulated SC site. These modifications of SC-evoked gaze shifts during cFN inactivation differed from the ipsiversive hypermetria/contraversive hypometria pattern observed for visually triggered gaze shifts recorded during the same recording sessions. We conclude that (i) the topographical organization of gaze shift amplitude in the deeper SC layers is influenced by the cerebellum and is either severely distorted or demonstrates an amplitude reduction during inactivation of the contralateral or ipsilateral cFN, respectively; (ii) gaze shifts evoked by SC microstimulation and visually triggered gaze shifts either rely on distinct cerebellar-dependent control processes or differ by the location of the caudal-most active SC population. We present a functional scheme providing several predictions regarding the modulatory influence of the cerebellum on SC neuronal activities and on the topographical organization of fastigial-SC projections.


Subject(s)
Cerebellar Nuclei/physiology , GABA Agonists/pharmacology , Muscimol/pharmacology , Neural Pathways/physiology , Neurons/physiology , Saccades/physiology , Superior Colliculi/physiology , Acceleration , Animals , Brain Mapping , Cats , Cerebellar Nuclei/cytology , Cerebellar Nuclei/drug effects , Electric Stimulation , Functional Laterality/physiology , Motion Perception/physiology , Neural Pathways/cytology , Neural Pathways/drug effects , Neurons/drug effects , Orientation/physiology , Photic Stimulation , Reaction Time/drug effects , Reaction Time/physiology , Saccades/drug effects , Superior Colliculi/cytology , Superior Colliculi/drug effects
14.
Vision Res ; 41(25-26): 3283-94, 2001.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11718773

ABSTRACT

During the course of previous recordings of visually-triggered gaze shifts in the head-unrestrained cat, we occasionally observed small head movements which preceded the initiation of the saccadic eye/head gaze shift toward a visual target. These early head movements (EHMs) were directed toward the target and occurred with a probability varying between animals from 0.4% to 16.4% (mean=5.2%, n=11 animals). The amplitude of EHM ranged from 0.4 degrees to 8.3 degrees (mean=1.9 degrees ), their latency from 66 to 270 ms (median=133 ms) and the delay from EHM onset to gaze shift onset averaged 183+/-108 ms (n=240). Their occurrence did not depend on visual target eccentricity in the studied range (7-35 degrees ), but influenced the metrics and dynamics of the ensuing gaze shifts (gain and velocity reduced). We also found in the two tested cats that low intensity microstimulation of the superior colliculus deeper layers elicited a head movement preceding the gaze shift. Altogether, these results suggest that the presentation of a visual target can elicit a head movement without triggering a saccadic eye/head gaze shift. The visuomotor pathways triggering these early head movements can involve the deep superior colliculus.


Subject(s)
Electric Stimulation/methods , Head Movements/physiology , Photic Stimulation/methods , Superior Colliculi/physiology , Animals , Cats , Linear Models , Saccades/physiology , Visual Pathways/physiology
15.
Exp Brain Res ; 132(4): 457-63, 2000 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10912826

ABSTRACT

It is known that the medio-posterior cerebellar lobules VI/VII of the vermis and caudal part of the fastigial nucleus (cFN) are involved in the control of saccadic displacements of the visual axis in space (gaze). We have recently shown in the head-unrestrained cat that inactivation of the cFN severely impairs the accuracy of orienting gaze shifts toward visual targets by altering the amplitude of both eye and head components. In the present paper, we report additional data that indicate that the deficits induced by cFN inactivation are not restricted to saccadic gaze shifts but extend to the forward reaching movement of the whole body toward a visual target. Indeed, the path followed by the animal walking toward a visible food target was systematically curved toward the inactivated side. This deficit could largely be accounted for by an angular bias in the heading direction used by the animal to reach the target. These data suggest that pharmacological inactivation of the cFN leads to a general deficit in spatial orientation.


Subject(s)
Cerebellar Nuclei/physiology , Locomotion/physiology , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Video Recording , Animals , Cats , Cerebellar Nuclei/injuries , GABA Agonists , Muscimol
16.
Exp Brain Res ; 132(2): 243-59, 2000 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10853949

ABSTRACT

It is known that the saccadic system shows adaptive changes when the command sent to the extraocular muscles is inappropriate. Despite an abundance of supportive psychophysical investigations, the neurophysiological substrate of this process is still debated. The present study addresses this issue using H2(15)O positron emission tomography (PET). We contrasted three conditions in which healthy human subjects were required to perform saccadic eye movements toward peripheral visual targets. Two conditions involved a modification of the target location during the course of the initial saccade, when there is suppression of visual perception. In the RAND condition, intra-saccadic target displacement was random from trial-to-trial, precluding any systematic modification of the primary saccade amplitude. In the ADAPT condition, intra-saccadic target displacement was uniform, causing adaptive modification of the primary saccade amplitude. In the third condition (stationary, STAT), the target remained at the same location during the entire trial. Difference images reflecting regional cerebral-blood-flow changes attributable to the process of saccadic adaptation (ADAPT minus RAND; ADAPT minus STAT) showed a selective activation in the oculomotor cerebellar vermis (OCV; lobules VI and VII). This finding is consistent with neurophysiological studies in monkeys. Additional analyses indicated that the cerebellar activation was not related to kinematic factors, and that the absence of significant activation within the frontal eye fields (FEF) or the superior colliculus (SC) did not represent a false negative inference. Besides the contribution of the OCV to saccadic adaptation, we also observed, in the RAND condition, that the saccade amplitude was significantly larger when the previous trial involved a forward jump than when the previous trial involved a backward jump. This observation indicates that saccade accuracy is constantly monitored on a trial-to-trial basis. Behavioral measurements and PET observations (RAND minus STAT) suggest that this single-trial control of saccade amplitude may be functionally distinct from the process of saccadic adaptation.


Subject(s)
Adaptation, Physiological/physiology , Saccades/physiology , Tomography, Emission-Computed , Adult , Biomechanical Phenomena , Cerebellum/diagnostic imaging , Cerebellum/physiology , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Superior Colliculi/diagnostic imaging , Superior Colliculi/physiology
17.
Exp Brain Res ; 124(3): 351-62, 1999 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9989441

ABSTRACT

We investigated whether and how adaptive changes in saccadic amplitudes (short-term saccadic adaptation) modify hand movements when subjects are involved in a pointing task to visual targets without vision of the hand. An experiment consisted of the pre-adaptation test of hand pointing (placing the finger tip on a LED position), a period of adaptation, and a post-adaptation test of hand pointing. In a basic task (transfer paradigm A), the pre- and post-adaptation trials were performed without accompanying eye and head movements: in the double-step gaze adaptation task, subjects had to fixate a single, suddenly displaced visual target by moving eyes and head in a natural way. Two experimental sessions were run with the visual target jumping during the saccades, either backwards (from 30 to 20 degrees, gaze saccade shortening) or onwards (30 to 40 degrees, gaze saccade lengthening). Following gaze-shortening adaptation (level of adaptation 79+/-10%, mean and s.d.), we found a statistically significant shift (t-test, error level P<0.05) in the final hand-movement points, possibly due to adaptation transfer, representing 15.2% of the respective gaze adaptation. After gaze-lengthening adaptation (level of adaptation 92+/-17%). a non-significant shift occurred in the opposite direction to that expected from adaptation transfer. The applied computations were also performed on some data of an earlier transfer paradigm (B, three target displacements at a time) with gain shortening. They revealed a significant transfer relative to the amount of adaptation of 18.5< or = 17.5% (P<0.05). In the coupling paradigm (C), we studied the influence of gaze saccade adaptation of hand-pointing movements with concomitant orienting gaze shifts. The adaptation levels achieved were 59+/-20% (shortening) and 61+/-27% (lengthening). Shifts in the final fingertip positions were congruent with internal coupling between gaze and hand, representing 53% of the respective gaze-amplitude changes in the shortening session and 6% in the lengthening session. With an adaptation transfer of less than 20% (paradigm A and B), we concluded that saccadic adaptation does not "automatically" produce a functionally meaningful change in the skeleto-motor system controlling hand-pointing movements. In tasks with concomitant gaze saccades (coupling paradigm C), the modification of hand pointing by the adapted gaze comes out more clearly, but only in the shortening session.


Subject(s)
Adaptation, Physiological/physiology , Fixation, Ocular/physiology , Hand/physiology , Movement/physiology , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Saccades/physiology , Electrooculography , Humans , Time Factors
18.
Neurosci Biobehav Rev ; 22(6): 761-88, 1998 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9809311

ABSTRACT

The nature of the neural mechanisms involved in movement planning still remains widely unknown. We review in the present paper the state of our knowledge of the mechanisms whereby a visual input is transformed into a motor command. For the sake of generality, we consider the main problems that the nervous system has to solve to generate a movement, that is: target localization, definition of the initial state of the motor apparatus, and hand trajectory formation. For each of these problems three questions are addressed. First, what are the main results presented in the literature? Second, are these results compatible with each other? Third, which factors may account for the existence of incompatibilities between experimental observations or between theoritical models? This approach allows the explanation of some of the contradictions existing within the movement-generation literature. It also suggests that the search for general theories may be in vain, the central nervous system being able to use different strategies both in encoding the target location with respect to the body and in planning hand displacement. In our view, this conclusion may advance the field by both opening new lines of research and bringing some sterile controversies to an end.


Subject(s)
Hand/physiology , Movement/physiology , Ocular Physiological Phenomena , Animals , Humans , Psychomotor Performance/physiology
19.
J Neurophysiol ; 80(3): 1180-96, 1998 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9744931

ABSTRACT

The implication of the caudal part of the fastigial nucleus (cFN) in the control of saccadic shifts of the visual axis is now well established. In contrast a possible involvement of the rostral part of the fastigial nuceus (rFN) remains unknown. In the current study we investigated in the head-unrestrained cat the contribution of the rFN to the control of visually triggered saccadic gaze shifts by measuring the deficits after unilateral muscimol injection in the rFN. A typical gaze dysmetria was observed: gaze saccades directed toward the inactivated side were hypermetric, whereas those with an opposite direction were hypometric. For both movement directions, gaze dysmetria was proportional to target retinal eccentricity and could be described as a modified gain in the translation of visual signals into eye and head motor commands. Correction saccades were triggered when the target remained visible and reduced the gaze fixation error to 2.7 +/- 1.3 degrees (mean +/- SD) on average. The hypermetria of ipsiversive gaze shifts resulted predominantly from a hypermetric response of the eyes, whereas the hypometria of contraversive gaze shifts resulted from hypometric responses of both eye and head. However, even in this latter case, the eye saccade was more affected than the motion of the head. As a consequence, for both directions of gaze shift the relative contributions of the eye and head to the overall gaze displacement were altered by muscimol injection. This was revealed by a decreased contribution of the head for ipsiversive gaze shifts and an increased head contribution for contraversive movements. These modifications were associated with slight changes in the delay between eye and head movement onsets. Inactivation of the rFN also affected the initiation of eye and head movements. Indeed, the latency of ipsiversive gaze and head movements decreased to 88 and 92% of normal, respectively, whereas the latency of contraversive ones increased to 149 and 145%. The deficits induced by rFN inactivation were then compared with those obtained after muscimol injection in the cFN of the same animals. Several deficits differed according to the site of injection within the fastigial nucleus (tonic orbital eye rotation, hypermetria of ipsiversive gaze shifts and fixation offset, relationship between dysmetria and latency of contraversive gaze shifts, postural deficit). In conclusion, the present study demonstrates that the rFN is involved in the initiation and the control of combined eye-head gaze shifts. In addition our findings support a functional distinction between the rFN and cFN for the control of orienting gaze shifts. This distinction is discussed with respect to the segregated fastigiofugal projections arising from the rFN and cFN.


Subject(s)
Cerebellar Nuclei/physiology , Head/physiology , Saccades/physiology , Animals , Behavior, Animal/drug effects , Cats , Fixation, Ocular/drug effects , Fixation, Ocular/physiology , Functional Laterality/physiology , GABA Agonists/pharmacology , Muscimol/pharmacology , Neurons/drug effects , Neurons/physiology , Nystagmus, Pathologic/physiopathology , Pursuit, Smooth/drug effects , Pursuit, Smooth/physiology , Reaction Time/physiology , Restraint, Physical
20.
J Neurophysiol ; 80(3): 1552-7, 1998 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9744959

ABSTRACT

Muscimol injection in the caudal part of the fastigial nucleus (cFN) leads, in the head-unrestrained cat, to a characteristic dysmetria of saccadic gaze shifts toward visual targets. The goal of the current study was to test whether this pharmacological cFN inactivation impaired the ability to compensate for unexpected perturbations in gaze position during the latency period of the saccadic response. Such perturbations consisted of moving gaze away from the target by a transient electrical microstimulation in the deep layers of the superior colliculus simultaneously with extinction of the visual target. After injection of muscimol in the cFN, targets located in the contralesional hemifield elicited gaze shifts that fell short of the target in both "perturbed" and "unperturbed" trials. The amplitude of the compensatory contraversive gaze shifts in perturbed trials coincided with the predicted amplitude of unperturbed responses starting from the same position. Targets located in the opposite hemifield elicited hypermetric gaze shifts in both trial types, and the error of compensatory responses was not statistically different from that of unperturbed gaze shifts. These results indicate that inactivation of the cFN does not interfere with the ability of the head-unrestrained cat to compensate for ipsiversive or contraversive perturbations in gaze position. Thus the gaze-related feedback signals that are used to compute a reference signal of desired gaze displacement are not impaired by cFN inactivation.


Subject(s)
Cerebellar Nuclei/physiology , Fixation, Ocular/physiology , Animals , Cats , Cerebellar Nuclei/drug effects , GABA Agonists/pharmacology , Head/physiology , Motor Cortex/physiology , Muscimol/pharmacology , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Restraint, Physical , Visual Fields/physiology
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...