Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 19 de 19
Filtrar
Más filtros











Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
Br J Dev Psychol ; 2024 Sep 21.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39305180

RESUMEN

This paper examined British children's (8- to 10-year-olds) and adolescents' (13- to 15-year-olds, N = 340; Female N = 171, 50.3%) expectations, evaluations and reasoning about a bystander peer who challenges the social exclusion of an immigrant or non-immigrant peer by a peer group of non-immigrant students. Participants read a hypothetical scenario in which a peer was excluded from an afterschool club by the peer group. The scenarios were either intergroup or intragroup contexts. Participants' expectations of a peer bystander challenging the social exclusion by the peer group, their perception of how the peer group would evaluate the challenger, and their reasoning around their expectations were measured. Adolescents were less likely to expect a peer bystander to challenge exclusion compared to children. Participants' perceptions of how the group would evaluate the challenger were significantly lower in intergroup compared to intragroup contexts. In intergroup contexts, adolescents with low expectations of challenging favoured group dynamics and group repercussions reasoning over moral reasoning, while children did not use group repercussions reasoning.

2.
Int J Behav Dev ; 47(1): 9-20, 2023 Jan.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37064763

RESUMEN

The present study examined age-related differences in bystander reactions within the context of peer exclusion of national ingroup (British) and immigrant outgroup (Australian or Turkish) peers. The immigrant peers were from nations that varied in terms of their perceived intergroup status in Britain. Participants were British children (n = 110, 8-11 years) and adolescents (n = 193, 13-16 years) who were presented with one of three scenarios in which either a British national, Australian immigrant or Turkish immigrant peer was excluded by a British peer group. Participants indicated their bystander responses. Perceived similarity and bystander self-efficacy were examined as possible correlates of bystander reactions. Findings revealed that children were more likely to directly challenge the social exclusion when the excluded peer was British or Australian compared to when they were Turkish. In contrast, adolescents did not differentiate in their response - they were equally likely to directly challenge the exclusion regardless of the excluded peer's nationality. Importantly, when the excluded peer was Turkish, moderated mediation analysis showed that, with age, there was higher bystander self-efficacy for challenging the exclusions. In turn, higher bystander self-efficacy was related to higher direct challenging. These novel findings demonstrate the importance of intergroup relations, perceived similarity and bystander self-efficacy in the emergence of age-related differences in bystander reactions to the exclusion of immigrant peers [219 words].

3.
Front Psychol ; 13: 833589, 2022.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36110281

RESUMEN

We examined developmental changes in British children's (8- to 10-year-olds) and adolescents' (13- to 15-year-olds, N = 340; Female N = 171, 50.3%) indirect bystander reactions (i.e., judgments about whether to get help and from whom when witnessing social exclusion) and their social-moral reasoning regarding their reactions to social exclusion. We also explored, for the first time, how the group membership of the excluder and victim affect participants' reactions. Participants read a hypothetical scenario in which they witnessed a peer being excluded from a school club by another peer. We manipulated the group membership of the victim (either British or an immigrant) and the group membership of the excluder (either British or an immigrant). Participants' likelihood of indirect bystander reactions decreased from childhood into adolescence. Children were more likely to get help from a teacher or an adult than getting help from a friend, whereas adolescents were more likely to get help from a friend than getting help from a teacher or an adult. For both indirect bystander reactions, children justified their likelihood of responding by referring to their trust in their teachers and friends. Adolescents were more likely to refer to group loyalty and dynamics, and psychological reasons. The findings support and extend the Social Reasoning Developmental (SRD) approach by showing the importance of group processes with age in shaping children's judgments about how to respond indirectly by asking for help from others, when they are bystanders in a situation that involves exclusion. The findings have practical implications for combating social exclusion and promoting prosocial bystander behavior in schools.

4.
Front Psychol ; 13: 837276, 2022.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36017427

RESUMEN

The present study examined British children's and adolescents' individual and perceived group evaluations of a challenger when a member of one's own group excludes a British national or an immigrant newcomer to the school (Turkish or Australian) from participating in a group activity. Participants included British children (n = 110, Mage in years = 9.69, SD = 1.07, 44 girls, aged 8-11) and adolescents (n = 193, Mage in years = 14.16, SD = 0.92, 104 girls, aged 13-16), who were inducted into their group and heard hypothetical scenarios in which a member of their own group expressed a desire to exclude the newcomer from joining their activity. Subsequently, participants heard that another member of the ingroup challenged the exclusionary act by stating that they should be inclusive. Children's and adolescents' individual evaluations of the bystander who challenged the social exclusion of an immigrant peer were more positive than their perceived group evaluations, recognizing that groups are often exclusionary. Only adolescents but not children differed in their individual and perceived group evaluations in the social exclusion of British peers. When the newcomer was an immigrant peer, adolescents were more likely to evaluate the challenger positively in both their individual and perceived group evaluations compared to children. Further, children, compared to adolescents, were more likely to reason about social and group norms to justify their evaluations only when the excluded peer was an immigrant but not when the excluded peer was British. Adolescents were more likely to reason about fairness, rights, and equality. The findings indicate that exclusionary group norms surrounding immigrants begin in childhood. Interventions that focus on changing group norms to be more inclusive could be effective in reducing prejudicial attitudes toward immigrants in childhood.

5.
J Exp Child Psychol ; 214: 105290, 2022 02.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34563891

RESUMEN

We examined minority-status (non-Cypriot immigrant) and majority-status (Cypriot national) preadolescents' bystander reactions to, and reasoning about, intergroup social exclusion (N = 367; Mage = 11.7 years; 50% Cypriot). Participants read one of three contexts where victim group identity was either non-Cypriot or Cypriot or a context where identity was not mentioned (i.e., control). Cypriot participants reported higher prosocial bystander responses when Cypriot victims were excluded compared with when non-Cypriot victims were excluded. Non-Cypriot participants reported equally high prosocial bystander responses for Cypriot and non-Cypriot victims, and both were higher than those for the control condition. When choosing to challenge social exclusion, non-Cypriot and Cypriot participants employed moral reasoning, focusing on concerns of welfare and equality. When choosing not to challenge the exclusion, Cypriot bystanders referenced personal choice (e.g., "I would not say anything; it is not my problem") more when victim identity was salient. Non-Cypriot bystanders referenced personal choice only when not challenging exclusion in the control context. Cypriot participants with high levels of intergroup contact reported higher helping intentions toward non-Cypriot victims. These findings support and extend social reasoning developmental theory and highlight practical implications for tackling intergroup social exclusion in schools and maintaining positive intergroup relations.


Asunto(s)
Grupo Paritario , Aislamiento Social , Niño , Humanos , Intención , Grupos Minoritarios , Principios Morales
6.
Dev Psychol ; 57(8): 1342-1349, 2021 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34591576

RESUMEN

This study examined prosocial bystander behavior in an online ball-throwing game (Cyberball), toward the exclusion of immigrants and nonimmigrant peers within intergroup and intragroup contexts. Participants were British children (8- to 10-year-olds) and adolescents (13- to 15-year-olds, N = 292; female N = 144). They were an ethnically diverse low-to-middle SES sample from a South Asian, White, Black, or mixed ethnic background. Participants played the game and witnessed a victim being excluded by peers. The victim's and excluders' group membership and status were highlighted in a prototypical (i.e., majority status peers excluding a minority status victim) or nonprototypical (i.e., minority status peers excluding a majority status victim) intergroup context. In intragroup contexts exclusion involved peers from the same group (i.e., majority status peers excluding a majority status victim or minority status peers excluding a minority status victim). Prosocial bystander behavior and "verbal" reactions to the exclusion were measured. Adolescents showed more prosocial bystander behavior than children when it was an intergroup context but not when it was an intragroup context. Only adolescents showed more prosocial bystander behavior when the intergroup context was prototypical compared to nonprototypical. Verbal reactions were related to prosocial bystander behavior and, with age, individuals increasingly verbally challenged the exclusion and the motivation behind it. The findings support the Social Reasoning Developmental (SRD) approach to social exclusion by showing that from late childhood into midadolescence bystander behavior is increasingly related to group membership and group status of the excluders and victim. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).


Asunto(s)
Grupo Paritario , Distancia Psicológica , Adolescente , Niño , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Motivación , Conducta Social , Aislamiento Social
7.
Ann N Y Acad Sci ; 1505(1): 118-141, 2021 12.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34176148

RESUMEN

Spatial prioritization is a critical step in conservation planning, a process designed to ensure that limited resources are applied in ways that deliver the highest possible returns for biodiversity and human wellbeing. In practice, many spatial prioritizations fall short of their potential by focusing on places rather than actions, and by using data of snapshots of assets or threats rather than estimated impacts. We introduce spatial action mapping as an approach that overcomes these shortfalls. This approach produces a spatially explicit view of where and how much a given conservation action is likely to contribute to achieving stated conservation goals. Through seven case examples, we demonstrate simple to complex versions of how this method can be applied across local to global scales to inform decisions about a wide range of conservation actions and benefits. Spatial action mapping can support major improvements in efficient use of conservation resources and will reach its full potential as the quality of environmental, social, and economic datasets converge and conservation impact evaluations improve.


Asunto(s)
Biodiversidad , Conservación de los Recursos Naturales/métodos , Ecosistema , Mapeo Geográfico , Análisis Espacial , Conservación de los Recursos Naturales/estadística & datos numéricos , Humanos
8.
Br J Nurs ; 28(1): 11-15, 2019 Jan 10.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30620651

RESUMEN

A programme to standardise catheterisation practice was introduced in Sherwood Forest Hospitals NHS Trust in 2016, with the aim of reducing the incidence of catheter-associated urinary tract infections (CAUTIs). The initiative involved the use of a catheterisation pack (Bard®Tray). Within the first year following its introduction, the CAUTI rate had been reduced from 13.3% to 2.1% (between July 2016 and June 2017), which is a reduction of more than 80%. Standardisation has also brought cost savings for the Trust of about £33 000 a year. The Trust has maintained its standardisation approach and CAUTI rates remain at around 2%.


Asunto(s)
Infecciones Relacionadas con Catéteres/prevención & control , Infección Hospitalaria/prevención & control , Práctica Clínica Basada en la Evidencia , Cateterismo Urinario/enfermería , Infecciones Urinarias/prevención & control , Infecciones Relacionadas con Catéteres/epidemiología , Infección Hospitalaria/epidemiología , Hospitales , Humanos , Investigación en Evaluación de Enfermería , Medicina Estatal , Reino Unido , Cateterismo Urinario/efectos adversos , Infecciones Urinarias/epidemiología
9.
Child Dev ; 87(5): 1379-91, 2016 09.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27684393

RESUMEN

Adolescents' evaluations of discriminatory race-based humor and their expectations about peer responses to discrimination were investigated in 8th- (Mage  = 13.80) and 10th-grade (Mage  = 16.11) primarily European-American participants (N = 256). Older adolescents judged race-based humor as more acceptable than did younger adolescents and were less likely to expect peer intervention. Participants who rejected discrimination were more likely to reference welfare/rights and prejudice and to anticipate that peers would intervene. Showing awareness of group processes, adolescents who rejected race-based humor believed that peers who intervened would be more likely to be excluded. They also disapproved of exclusion more than did participants who supported race-based humor. Results expose the complexity of situations involving subtle discrimination. Implications for bullying interventions are discussed.


Asunto(s)
Conducta del Adolescente/psicología , Procesos de Grupo , Grupo Paritario , Distancia Psicológica , Racismo/psicología , Conducta Social , Percepción Social , Ingenio y Humor como Asunto/psicología , Adolescente , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino
10.
Br J Dev Psychol ; 33(4): 419-33, 2015 Nov.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26058823

RESUMEN

A developmental intergroup approach was taken to examine the development of prosocial bystander intentions among children and adolescents. Participants as bystanders (N = 260) aged 8-10 and 13-15 years were presented with scenarios of direct aggression between individuals from different social groups (i.e., intergroup verbal aggression). These situations involved either an ingroup aggressor and an outgroup victim or an outgroup aggressor and an ingroup victim. This study focussed on the role of intergroup factors (group membership, ingroup identification, group norms, and social-moral reasoning) in the development of prosocial bystander intentions. Findings showed that prosocial bystander intentions declined with age. This effect was partially mediated by the ingroup norm to intervene and perceived severity of the verbal aggression. However, a moderated mediation analysis showed that only when the victim was an ingroup member and the aggressor an outgroup member did participants become more likely with age to report prosocial bystander intentions due to increased ingroup identification. Results also showed that younger children focussed on moral concerns and adolescents focussed more on psychological concerns when reasoning about their bystander intention. These novel findings help explain the developmental decline in prosocial bystander intentions from middle childhood into early adolescence when observing direct intergroup aggression.


Asunto(s)
Agresión/fisiología , Desarrollo Infantil/fisiología , Intención , Principios Morales , Conducta Social , Percepción Social , Pensamiento/fisiología , Conducta Verbal/fisiología , Adolescente , Niño , Femenino , Procesos de Grupo , Humanos , Masculino
11.
Br J Dev Psychol ; 32(3): 257-61, 2014 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24954093

RESUMEN

Abrams, Rutland, Palmer, Ferrell, and Pelletier (2014) showed that better second-order mental state understanding facilitates 6-7-year-olds' ability to link a partially disloyal child's atypicality to inclusive or exclusive reactions by in-group or outgroup members. This finding is interpreted in terms of predictions from the developmental subjective group dynamics model. We respond to thoughtful commentaries by Rhodes and Chalik, Patterson, and Rakoczy. Children face a significant developmental challenge in becoming able to recognize and interpret social atypicality in intergroup contexts. Researching that ability to contextualize judgements raises new questions about the nature of peer inclusion and exclusion, about children's social cognition, and about the way that social cognitive development and social experience combine. Rather than individual-focused cognition taking priority over category-based cognition, we argue the two become more systematically integrated during development. We note that loyalty is but one example of typicality, and we also consider the role of more advanced perspective taking among older children, and the role of multiple classification skill among younger children, as well as potential implications for intervention to reduce peer victimization and prejudice.


Asunto(s)
Desarrollo Infantil/fisiología , Procesos de Grupo , Percepción Social , Teoría de la Mente/fisiología , Pensamiento/fisiología , Niño , Humanos , Grupo Paritario
12.
Br J Dev Psychol ; 32(3): 233-47, 2014 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24471452

RESUMEN

Children aged 6-7 years judged a loyal and a partially disloyal member of a school in terms of how typical they are within the school group and their likely acceptance by peers from the same school and a different school. Second-order mental-state understanding (SOMSU) predicted whether children thought atypical members would be included differently in the two groups. Counterfactual reasoning ability, multiple classification ability, and working memory ability did not predict children's judgements of group members. Moreover, as predicted by the developmental subjective group dynamics model, only children with higher levels of SOMSU and who discerned differences in the typicality of normative and deviant ingroup members inferred that peers would differently include atypical individuals from the same and different groups.


Asunto(s)
Cognición/fisiología , Procesos de Grupo , Juicio/fisiología , Percepción Social , Teoría de la Mente/fisiología , Niño , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Memoria a Corto Plazo/fisiología , Grupo Paritario
13.
Dev Psychol ; 50(1): 258-70, 2014 Jan.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23544855

RESUMEN

Research with adults has demonstrated a "black sheep effect" (BSE) whereby, relative to evaluations of normative group members, ingroup deviants are derogated more than outgroup deviants. The developmental subjective group dynamics (DSGD) model holds that the BSE should develop during middle childhood when children apply wider social norms. Three hundred and thirty-eight children who were between 5 and 12 years old judged a normative (socially desirable) and a deviant (socially undesirable) member from an ingroup or an outgroup school. Results confirmed a developmental increase in the BSE, the first time this has been demonstrated. Children's own evaluations of group members were mediated by their expectations about ingroup peers' evaluations. In line with DSGD and social domain theories, with age, children's explanations of peer evaluations for ingroup deviance focused relatively more on loyalty. Practical and theoretical implications for peer inclusion and exclusion are discussed.


Asunto(s)
Relaciones Interpersonales , Grupo Paritario , Identificación Social , Percepción Social , Niño , Femenino , Humanos , Estudios Longitudinales , Masculino , Valores de Referencia , Autoimagen , Conducta Social
14.
An. psicol ; 27(3): 698-707, oct.-dic. 2011. tab, graf
Artículo en Inglés | IBECS | ID: ibc-94308

RESUMEN

Este estudio examinó el desarrollo de las actitudes hacia estímulos que representan a figuras de peso bajo utilizando dos medidas diferentes. En primer lugar, los niños (de 5-11 años) atribuyeron rasgos positivos y negativos a imágenes de personas de peso bajo, peso promedio y sobrepeso. En segundo lugar, se examinó la preferencia por las mismas imágenes en tres contextos diferentes (Total N=151). La tarea de atribución de rasgos mostró que el sesgo hacia el peso anuló el sesgo de género entre las niñas de todas las edades; las chicas prefirieron imágenes de peso bajo significativamente más que las de peso promedio o sobrepeso. En la tarea de preferencias, cuando tenían que elegir a un “amigo” o “a quién invitar a casa”, tanto los niños como las niñas de 5 a 7 años prefirieron figuras de peso bajo (AU)


This study examined developmental attitudes towards underweight stimuli using two different measures. Children aged 5 - 11 firstly attributed positive and negative traits to images of underweight, average-weight and overweight stimuli. A second measure investigated picture preference of the same stimuli for 3 different contexts (Total N=151). The trait attribution task revealed that weight bias overrode gender bias amongst female participants of all ages; girls significantly preferred underweight images to average-weight and overweight images. In the picture preference task, preferences for the underweight stimuli were given by both male and female 5 to 7 year olds, when selecting which stimuli they would have as a "friend‟ or to "take home to play‟ (AU)


Asunto(s)
Humanos , Masculino , Femenino , Niño , Peso Corporal/etnología , Delgadez/diagnóstico , Delgadez/patología , Pesos y Medidas Corporales/psicología , Actitud/etnología , Niño , Estilo de Vida Saludable/etnología , Delgadez/prevención & control , Delgadez/psicología , Pesos y Medidas Corporales/métodos , Pesos y Medidas Corporales/estadística & datos numéricos , Pesos y Medidas Corporales/normas
15.
Prev Chronic Dis ; 2 Spec no: A17, 2005 Nov.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16263050

RESUMEN

The Alabama Department of Public Health (ADPH) is collaborating with the National Cancer Institute to develop detailed profiles of underserved Alabama communities most at risk for cancer. These profiles will be combined with geocoded data to create a pilot project, Cancer Prevention for Alabama's Underserved Populations: A Focused Approach. The project's objectives are to provide the ADPH's cancer prevention programs with a more accurate and cost-effective means of planning, implementing, and evaluating its prevention activities in an outcomes-oriented and population-appropriate manner. The project links geocoded data from the Alabama Statewide Cancer Registry with profiles generated by the National Cancer Institute's cancer profiling system, Consumer Health Profiles. These profiles have been successfully applied to market-focused cancer prevention messages across the United States. The ADPH and the National Cancer Institute will evaluate the efficacy of using geocoded data and lifestyle segmentation information in strategy development and program implementation. Alabama is the first state in the nation not only to link geocoded cancer registry data with lifestyle segmentation data but also to use the National Cancer Institute's profiles and methodology in combination with actual state data.


Asunto(s)
Sistemas de Información Geográfica , Accesibilidad a los Servicios de Salud , Neoplasias/prevención & control , Mercadeo Social , Alabama , Promoción de la Salud , Humanos , Sistema de Registros , Planes Estatales de Salud , Estados Unidos
16.
Am J Psychiatry ; 162(3): 552-9, 2005 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15741473

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVE: The authors investigated outcome at discharge and at follow-up assessments for adults abused as children who completed a 6-week inpatient program for traumatic stress recovery. METHOD: Participants were assessed at admission, discharge, and 3, 6, and 12 months postdischarge on measures of global symptom severity, posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD), and disrupted beliefs. Two wait-list comparison groups were also assessed at two points in time for comparison with the discharge and 3-month postdischarge assessments of the treatment group. RESULTS: Relative to admission, the mean scores on all outcome measures for the treatment group were improved at discharge and at 6 and 12 months. Relative to a wait list group, the treatment group was significantly improved at discharge. After 3 months, the scores for the treatment group were not different from those of a wait list group because of deterioration in the treatment group. Age, source of income, and number of axis II disorders were associated with differing patterns of PTSD symptom change over time. Between 32% and 45%, depending on outcome measure, met stringent criteria for clinically significant change at 12 months postdischarge. CONCLUSIONS: At discharge from a specialized inpatient treatment program, adults with a history of abuse during childhood showed improvement relative to a wait list group. Scores tended to deteriorate in the 3 months following discharge but rebounded to discharge levels by 12 months postdischarge. Although many abused adults benefited from specialized inpatient treatment, a substantial proportion did not show clinically significant change 1 year later.


Asunto(s)
Maltrato a los Niños/psicología , Hospitalización , Trastornos por Estrés Postraumático/terapia , Comunidad Terapéutica , Adolescente , Adulto , Anciano , Niño , Maltrato a los Niños/estadística & datos numéricos , Abuso Sexual Infantil/psicología , Abuso Sexual Infantil/estadística & datos numéricos , Comorbilidad , Femenino , Estudios de Seguimiento , Hospitales Psiquiátricos/estadística & datos numéricos , Humanos , Acontecimientos que Cambian la Vida , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Ontario/epidemiología , Trastornos de la Personalidad/diagnóstico , Trastornos de la Personalidad/epidemiología , Escalas de Valoración Psiquiátrica , Proyectos de Investigación , Índice de Severidad de la Enfermedad , Trastornos por Estrés Postraumático/diagnóstico , Trastornos por Estrés Postraumático/epidemiología , Resultado del Tratamiento , Listas de Espera
17.
Am J Orthopsychiatry ; 75(1): 152-7, 2005 Jan.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15709858

RESUMEN

Mental health service (MHS) providers confront questions of informed consent for evaluation and treatment of children in state custody who are placed in residential or foster care programs, where legal responsibility is shared between state and parent. There are ethical issues encountered by MHS providers who work with this growing population of children in placement. Matters of informed consent and access to information about treatment influence relationships with the parents, legal guardians, Child Protective Service workers, and the child. These specific concerns are addressed: informed consent, the right to be informed, and the rights of parents or foster carers to participate in a child's treatment. Recommendations for resolving dilemmas faced by MHS providers are discussed.


Asunto(s)
Custodia del Niño/ética , Servicios de Salud del Niño/ética , Cuidados en el Hogar de Adopción/ética , Consentimiento Informado/ética , Servicios de Salud Mental/ética , Instituciones Residenciales/ética , Adulto , Niño , Defensa del Niño , Servicios de Salud del Niño/estadística & datos numéricos , Confidencialidad , Guías como Asunto , Humanos , Servicios de Salud Mental/estadística & datos numéricos , Padres , Relaciones Profesional-Familia , Estados Unidos
18.
Am J Orthopsychiatry ; 74(2): 112-21, 2004 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15113240

RESUMEN

Thirty adults were interviewed about their experiences in a 6-week program for recovery from posttraumatic stress. The most valued aspects were their relationships with staff and the staff's expertise in treating the long-term effects of abuse. They valued the structured group treatment that allowed them to process emotions with the goal of healing. A few were upset by process groups, whereas others had difficulty with the sudden loss of support at discharge from the program.


Asunto(s)
Maltrato a los Niños/psicología , Convalecencia , Aprendizaje , Trastornos por Estrés Postraumático/psicología , Trastornos por Estrés Postraumático/terapia , Sobrevivientes/psicología , Adulto , Niño , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Relaciones Profesional-Paciente , Refuerzo en Psicología , Apoyo Social , Encuestas y Cuestionarios
19.
Can J Commun Ment Health ; (4 Suppl): 25-38, 2002.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-14664104

RESUMEN

This article discusses the effects of family breakdown--and the resulting issues of custody and access--on children. It explores trends in child custody and access and issues surrounding those trends, particularly in terms of the benefits and limitations of joint custody. It then identifies the contextual problems of family breakups (including the relative poverty experienced by mother-led families, the unreliability of financial support from fathers, the complications for children which result from step-parent figures, including serial partners of their parents, the implications of interparental conflict, the need to fully address suspicions and/or allegations of abuse by one parent against another, and the problem of Parental Alienation Syndrome). Typical responses of children to family breakup are then considered, including feelings of loss, guilt, and responsibility, and a sense of divided loyalties--particularly in light of intense or violent parental conflict. And finally, recommendations are made to minimize the detrimental effects of family breakup on children: (a) allowing children choice and flexibility, (b) exploring the benefits of mediation for families, (c) promoting parental co-operation, and (d) encouraging an ongoing relationship with the nonresidential parent.


Asunto(s)
Custodia del Niño , Divorcio/psicología , Relaciones Padres-Hijo , Padres , Adulto , Niño , Defensa del Niño , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Factores Socioeconómicos
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA