Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 47
Filtrar
Mais filtros










Base de dados
Intervalo de ano de publicação
2.
Sci Rep ; 13(1): 19300, 2023 11 21.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37989781

RESUMO

We asked whether, in the first year of life, the infant brain can support the dynamic crossmodal interactions between vision and somatosensation that are required to represent peripersonal space. Infants aged 4 (n = 20, 9 female) and 8 (n = 20, 10 female) months were presented with a visual object that moved towards their body or receded away from it. This was presented in the bottom half of the screen and not fixated upon by the infants, who were instead focusing on an attention getter at the top of the screen. The visual moving object then disappeared and was followed by a vibrotactile stimulus occurring later in time and in a different location in space (on their hands). The 4-month-olds' somatosensory evoked potentials (SEPs) were enhanced when tactile stimuli were preceded by unattended approaching visual motion, demonstrating that the dynamic visual-somatosensory cortical interactions underpinning representations of the body and peripersonal space begin early in the first year of life. Within the 8-month-olds' sample, SEPs were increasingly enhanced by (unexpected) tactile stimuli following receding visual motion as age in days increased, demonstrating changes in the neural underpinnings of the representations of peripersonal space across the first year of life.


Assuntos
Percepção Espacial , Percepção do Tato , Lactente , Humanos , Feminino , Percepção Espacial/fisiologia , Tato/fisiologia , Percepção do Tato/fisiologia , Atenção/fisiologia , Encéfalo/fisiologia
4.
Sci Rep ; 13(1): 14696, 2023 09 07.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37679386

RESUMO

Human infants cannot report their experiences, limiting what we can learn about their bodily awareness. However, visual cortical responses to the body, linked to visual awareness and selective attention in adults, can be easily measured in infants and provide a promising marker of bodily awareness in early life. We presented 4- and 8-month-old infants with a flickering (7.5 Hz) video of a hand being stroked and recorded steady-state visual evoked potentials (SSVEPs). In half of the trials, the infants also received tactile stroking synchronously with visual stroking. The 8-month-old, but not the 4-month-old infants, showed a significant enhancement of SSVEP responses when they received tactile stimulation concurrent with the visually observed stroking. Follow-up experiments showed that this enhancement did not occur when the visual hand was presented in an incompatible posture with the infant's own body or when the visual stimulus was a body-irrelevant video. Our findings provide a novel insight into the development of bodily self-awareness in the first year of life.


Assuntos
Imagem Corporal , Acidente Vascular Cerebral , Adulto , Humanos , Lactente , Potenciais Evocados Visuais , Mãos , Aprendizagem , Exame Neurológico
5.
Sci Rep ; 12(1): 19281, 2022 11 11.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36369342

RESUMO

Knowledge of one's own body size is a crucial facet of body representation, both for acting on the environment and perhaps also for constraining body ownership. However, representations of body size may be somewhat plastic, particularly to allow for physical growth in childhood. Here we report a developmental investigation into the role of hand size in body representation (the sense of body ownership, perception of hand position, and perception of own-hand size). Using the rubber hand illusion paradigm, this study used different fake hand sizes (60%, 80%, 100%, 120% or 140% of typical size) in three age groups (6- to 7-year-olds, 12- to 13-year-olds, and adults; N = 229). We found no evidence that hand size constrains ownership or position: participants embodied hands which were both larger and smaller than their own, and indeed judged their own hands to have changed size following the illusion. Children and adolescents embodied the fake hands more than adults, with a greater tendency to feel their own hand had changed size. Adolescents were particularly sensitive to multisensory information. In sum, we found substantial plasticity in the representation of own-body size, with partial support for the hypothesis that children have looser representations than adults.


Assuntos
Ilusões , Percepção do Tato , Adulto , Criança , Adolescente , Humanos , Imagem Corporal , Propriocepção , Percepção Visual , Mãos
6.
Curr Biol ; 31(22): 5093-5101.e5, 2021 11 22.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34555348

RESUMO

Congenitally blind infants are not only deprived of visual input but also of visual influences on the intact senses. The important role that vision plays in the early development of multisensory spatial perception1-7 (e.g., in crossmodal calibration8-10 and in the formation of multisensory spatial representations of the body and the world1,2) raises the possibility that impairments in spatial perception are at the heart of the wide range of difficulties that visually impaired infants show across spatial,8-12 motor,13-17 and social domains.8,18,19 But investigations of early development are needed to clarify how visually impaired infants' spatial hearing and touch support their emerging ability to make sense of their body and the outside world. We compared sighted (S) and severely visually impaired (SVI) infants' responses to auditory and tactile stimuli presented on their hands. No statistically reliable differences in the direction or latency of responses to auditory stimuli emerged, but significant group differences emerged in responses to tactile and audiotactile stimuli. The visually impaired infants showed attenuated audiotactile spatial integration and interference, weighted more tactile than auditory cues when the two were presented in conflict, and showed a more limited influence of representations of the external layout of the body on tactile spatial perception.20 These findings uncover a distinct phenotype of multisensory spatial perception in early postnatal visual deprivation. Importantly, evidence of audiotactile spatial integration in visually impaired infants, albeit to a lesser degree than in sighted infants, signals the potential of multisensory rehabilitation methods in early development. VIDEO ABSTRACT.


Assuntos
Percepção Espacial , Percepção do Tato , Percepção Auditiva/fisiologia , Cegueira , Mãos/fisiologia , Humanos , Percepção Espacial/fisiologia , Tato/fisiologia , Percepção do Tato/fisiologia , Percepção Visual
7.
Cognition ; 210: 104617, 2021 05.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33556891

RESUMO

The ability to resist distracting stimuli whilst voluntarily focusing on a task is fundamental to our everyday cognitive functioning. Here, we investigated how this ability develops, and thereafter declines, across the lifespan using a single task/experiment. Young children (5-7 years), older children (10-11 years), young adults (20-27 years), and older adults (62-86 years) were presented with complex visual scenes. Endogenous (voluntary) attention was engaged by having the participants search for a visual target presented on either the left or right side of the display. The onset of the visual scenes was preceded - at stimulus onset asynchronies (SOAs) of 50, 200, or 500 ms - by a task-irrelevant sound (an exogenous crossmodal spatial distractor) delivered either on the same or opposite side as the visual target, or simultaneously on both sides (cued, uncued, or neutral trials, respectively). Age-related differences were revealed, especially in the extreme age-groups, which showed a greater impact of crossmodal spatial distractors. Young children were highly susceptible to exogenous spatial distraction at the shortest SOA (50 ms), whereas older adults were distracted at all SOAs, showing significant exogenous capture effects during the visual search task. By contrast, older children and young adults' search performance was not significantly affected by crossmodal spatial distraction. Overall, these findings present a detailed picture of the developmental trajectory of endogenous resistance to crossmodal spatial distraction from childhood to old age and demonstrate a different efficiency in coping with distraction across the four age-groups studied.


Assuntos
Atenção , Longevidade , Adolescente , Idoso , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Sinais (Psicologia) , Humanos , Estimulação Luminosa , Tempo de Reação , Adulto Jovem
8.
Child Dev ; 92(1): 351-366, 2021 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32767576

RESUMO

Adults' body representation is constrained by multisensory information and knowledge of the body such as its possible postures. This study (N = 180) tested for similar constraints in children. Using the rubber hand illusion with adults and 6- to 7-year olds, we measured proprioceptive drift (an index of hand localization) and ratings of felt hand ownership. The fake hand was either congruent or incongruent with the participant's own. Across ages, congruency of posture and visual-tactile congruency yielded greater drift toward the fake hand. Ownership ratings were higher with congruent visual-tactile information, but unaffected by posture. Posture constrains body representation similarly in children and adults, suggesting that children have sensitive, robust mechanisms for maintaining a sense of bodily self.


Assuntos
Imagem Corporal/psicologia , Desenvolvimento Infantil/fisiologia , Ilusões Ópticas/fisiologia , Estimulação Luminosa/métodos , Postura/fisiologia , Adulto , Criança , Emoções/fisiologia , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Propriocepção/fisiologia , Tato/fisiologia , Percepção do Tato/fisiologia , Adulto Jovem
9.
Child Dev ; 92(1): 21-34, 2021 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32920852

RESUMO

Two experiments examined perceptual colocation of visual and tactile stimuli in young infants. Experiment 1 compared 4- (n = 15) and 6-month-old (n = 12) infants' visual preferences for visual-tactile stimulus pairs presented across the same or different feet. The 4- and 6-month-olds showed, respectively, preferences for colocated and noncolocated conditions, demonstrating sensitivity to visual-tactile colocation on their feet. This extends previous findings of visual-tactile perceptual colocation on the hands in older infants. Control conditions excluded the possibility that both 6- (Experiment 1), and 4-month-olds (Experiment 2, n = 12) perceived colocation on the basis of an undifferentiated supramodal coding of spatial distance between stimuli. Bimodal perception of visual-tactile colocation is available by 4 months of age, that is, prior to the development of skilled reaching.


Assuntos
Desenvolvimento Infantil/fisiologia , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Percepção do Tato/fisiologia , Percepção Visual/fisiologia , Humanos , Lactente , Masculino , Estimulação Luminosa/métodos , Tato
10.
Cognition ; 197: 104127, 2020 04.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31891831

RESUMO

Tactile perception is referenced to, and modulated by, body parts and their boundaries. For example, tactile distances presented over the wrist are perceptually elongated relative to those presented within the hand or arm. This phenomenon is argued to result from a segmentation of tactile space according to body parts and their boundaries, i.e., touches presented within a body part are perceived as being more similar, and therefore closer together, whereas those that straddle a body part boundary (e.g. presented across two body parts) are perceived as more distinct and thus further apart. We tested the hypothesis that language shapes this effect by providing consolidatory labels for categories and boundaries, as it does in other perceptual domains. We examined the perceptual elongation of distance over the wrist in a group of Croatian adults (n = 37) whose first language does not differentiate between hand and arm at the wrist in common noun terms (instead, the Croatian word "ruka" encompasses the entire limb). Croatian adults, like UK adults reported in a previous study (Le Cornu Knight, Longo, & Bremner, 2014), perceived distances presented proximodistally over the wrist boundary as longer than those presented mediolaterally, whereas the reverse was found for both the hand and the arm. This pattern of results was remained when Croatian participants were split into two groups of inexperienced or proficient English-language speakers. This is striking evidence that body part boundaries consistently modulate tactile perception, despite differences in the linguistic distinctions of such body parts made by one's first language.


Assuntos
Distorção da Percepção , Percepção do Tato , Adulto , Mãos , Humanos , Idioma , Tato
11.
Biol Psychol ; 146: 107719, 2019 09.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31207259

RESUMO

Observing others being touched activates similar brain areas as those activated when one experiences a touch oneself. Event-related potential (ERP) studies have revealed that modulation of somatosensory components by observed touch occurs within 100 ms after stimulus onset, and such vicarious effects have been taken as evidence for empathy for others' tactile experiences. In previous studies body parts have been presented from a first person perspective. This raises the question of the extent to which somatosensory activation by observed touch to body parts depends on the perspective from which the body part is observed. In this study (N = 18), we examined the modulation of somatosensory ERPs by observed touch delivered to another person's hand when viewed as if from a first person versus a third person perspective. We found that vicarious touch effects primarily consist of two separable components in the early stages of somatosensory processing: an anatomical mapping for touch in first person perspective at P45, and a specular (mirror like) mapping for touch in third person perspective at P100. This is consistent with suggestions that vicarious representations exist to support predictions for one's own bodily events, but also to enable predictions of a social or interpersonal kind, at distinct temporal stages.


Assuntos
Relações Interpessoais , Córtex Somatossensorial/fisiologia , Tato/fisiologia , Adulto , Mapeamento Encefálico , Eletroencefalografia , Empatia , Potenciais Somatossensoriais Evocados/fisiologia , Feminino , Lateralidade Funcional/fisiologia , Mãos , Humanos , Masculino , Observação , Percepção do Tato , Adulto Jovem
12.
Dev Cogn Neurosci ; 35: 75-80, 2019 02.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28942240

RESUMO

The human brain recruits similar brain regions when a state is experienced (e.g., touch, pain, actions) and when that state is passively observed in other individuals. In adults, seeing other people being touched activates similar brain areas as when we experience touch ourselves. Here we show that already by four months of age, cortical responses to tactile stimulation are modulated by visual information specifying another person being touched. We recorded somatosensory evoked potentials (SEPs) in 4-month-old infants while they were presented with brief vibrotactile stimuli to the hands. At the same time that the tactile stimuli were presented the infants observed another person's hand being touched by a soft paintbrush or approached by the paintbrush which then touched the surface next to their hand. A prominent positive peak in SEPs contralateral to the site of tactile stimulation around 130 ms after the tactile stimulus onset was of a significantly larger amplitude for the "Surface" trials than for the "Hand" trials. These findings indicate that, even at four months of age, somatosensory cortex is not only involved in the personal experience of touch but can also be vicariously recruited by seeing other people being touched.


Assuntos
Córtex Somatossensorial/fisiologia , Percepção do Tato/fisiologia , Tato/fisiologia , Feminino , Humanos , Lactente , Masculino
13.
Curr Biol ; 28(22): R1294-R1295, 2018 11 19.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30458145

RESUMO

When newborns leave the enclosed spatial environment of the uterus and arrive in the outside world, they are faced with a new audiovisual environment of dynamic objects, actions and events both close to themselves and further away. One particular challenge concerns matching and making sense of the visual and auditory cues specifying object motion [1-5]. Previous research shows that adults prioritise the integration of auditory and visual information indicating looming (for example [2]) and that rhesus monkeys can integrate multisensory looming, but not receding, audiovisual stimuli [4]. Despite the clear adaptive value of correctly perceiving motion towards or away from the self - for defence against and physical interaction with moving objects - such a perceptual ability would clearly be undermined if newborns were unable to correctly match the auditory and visual cues to such motion. This multisensory perceptual skill has scarcely been studied in human ontogeny. Here we report that newborns only a few hours old are sensitive to matches between changes in visual size and in auditory intensity. This early multisensory competence demonstrates that, rather than being entirely naïve to their new audiovisual environment, newborns can make sense of the multisensory cue combinations specifying motion with respect to themselves.


Assuntos
Percepção Auditiva/fisiologia , Processamento Espacial/fisiologia , Percepção Visual/fisiologia , Estimulação Acústica/métodos , Sinais (Psicologia) , Feminino , Humanos , Recém-Nascido , Masculino , Percepção de Movimento/fisiologia , Estimulação Luminosa/métodos
14.
Cognition ; 180: 91-98, 2018 11.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30007880

RESUMO

Searching for a target while avoiding distraction is a core function of selective attention involving both voluntary and reflexive mechanisms. Here, for the first time, we investigated the development of the interplay between voluntary and reflexive mechanisms of selective attention from childhood to early adulthood. We asked 6-, 10-, and 20-year-old participants to search for a target presented in one hemifield of a complex scene, preceded by a task-irrelevant auditory cue on either the target side (valid), the opposite side (invalid), or both sides (neutral). For each scene we computed the number of salient locations (NSL) and the target saliency (TgS). All age groups showed comparable orienting effects ("valid minus neutral" trials), indicating a similar capture of spatial attention by valid cues which was independent of age. However, only adults demonstrated a suppression of the reorienting effect ("invalid minus neutral" trials), indicating late developments in the reallocation of spatial attention toward a target following auditory distraction. The searching performance of the children (both 6- and 10-year-olds), but not of the adults, was predicted by the NSL, indicating an attraction of processing resources to salient but task-irrelevant locations in childhood; conversely, only adults showed greater performance with increased TgS in valid trials, indicating late development in the use of task-related saliency. These findings highlight qualitatively different mechanisms of selective attention operating at different ages, demonstrating important developmental changes in the interplay between voluntary and reflexive mechanisms of selective attention during visual search in complex scenes.


Assuntos
Atenção/fisiologia , Percepção Auditiva/fisiologia , Cognição/fisiologia , Orientação/fisiologia , Estimulação Luminosa/métodos , Percepção Visual/fisiologia , Estimulação Acústica/métodos , Criança , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Tempo de Reação/fisiologia , Adulto Jovem
15.
J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform ; 44(1): 2-6, 2018 Jan.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29309192

RESUMO

We present the first empirical evidence that experience alters lightness perception. The role of experience in lightness perception was investigated through a cross-cultural comparison of 2 visual contrast phenomena: simultaneous lightness contrast and White's illusion. The Himba, a traditional seminomadic group known to have a local bias in perception, showed enhanced simultaneous lightness contrast but reduced White's illusion compared with groups that have a more global perceptual style: Urban-dwelling Himba and Westerners. Thus, experience of the urban environment alters lightness perception and we argue it does this by fostering the tendency to integrate information from across the visual scene. (PsycINFO Database Record


Assuntos
Sensibilidades de Contraste/fisiologia , Comparação Transcultural , Ilusões Ópticas/fisiologia , População Urbana , Adolescente , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Londres , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Namíbia , Urbanização , Adulto Jovem
16.
Dev Sci ; 21(4): e12597, 2018 Jul.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28880496

RESUMO

An ability to detect the common location of multisensory stimulation is essential for us to perceive a coherent environment, to represent the interface between the body and the external world, and to act on sensory information. Regarding the tactile environment "at hand", we need to represent somatosensory stimuli impinging on the skin surface in the same spatial reference frame as distal stimuli, such as those transduced by vision and audition. Across two experiments we investigated whether 6- (n = 14; Experiment 1) and 4-month-old (n = 14; Experiment 2) infants were sensitive to the colocation of tactile and auditory signals delivered to the hands. We recorded infants' visual preferences for spatially congruent and incongruent auditory-tactile events delivered to their hands. At 6 months, infants looked longer toward incongruent stimuli, whilst at 4 months infants looked longer toward congruent stimuli. Thus, even from 4 months of age, infants are sensitive to the colocation of simultaneously presented auditory and tactile stimuli. We conclude that 4- and 6-month-old infants can represent auditory and tactile stimuli in a common spatial frame of reference. We explain the age-wise shift in infants' preferences from congruent to incongruent in terms of an increased preference for novel crossmodal spatial relations based on the accumulation of experience. A comparison of looking preferences across the congruent and incongruent conditions with a unisensory control condition indicates that the ability to perceive auditory-tactile colocation is based on a crossmodal rather than a supramodal spatial code by 6 months of age at least.


Assuntos
Percepção Auditiva/fisiologia , Percepção do Tato/fisiologia , Adulto , Feminino , Audição/fisiologia , Humanos , Lactente , Masculino , Autoimagem , Processamento Espacial , Tato/fisiologia , Visão Ocular/fisiologia
17.
Dev Sci ; 21(3): e12557, 2018 05.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28326654

RESUMO

The present work investigates the development of bodily self-consciousness and its relation to multisensory bodily information, by measuring for the first time the development of responses to the full body illusion in childhood. We tested three age groups of children: 6- to 7-year-olds (n = 28); 8- to 9-year-olds (n = 21); 10- to 11-year-olds (n = 19), and a group of adults (n = 31). Each participant wore a head-mounted display (HMD) which displayed a view from a video camera positioned 2 metres behind their own back. Thus, they could view a virtual body from behind. We manipulated visuo-tactile synchrony by showing the participants a view of their virtual back being stroked with a stick at the same time and same place as their real back (synchronous condition), or at different times and places (asynchronous condition). After each period of stroking, we measured three aspects of bodily self-consciousness: drift in perceived self-location, self-identification with the virtual body, and touch referral to the virtual body. Results show that self-identification with the virtual body was significantly stronger in the synchronous condition than in the asynchronous condition even in the youngest group tested; however, the size of this effect increased with age. Touch referral to the virtual body was greater in the synchronous condition than in the asynchronous condition only for 10- to 11-year-olds and adults. Drift in perceived self-location was greater in the synchronous condition than in the asynchronous condition only for adults. Thus, the youngest age tested can self-identify with a virtual body, but the links between multisensory signals and bodily self-consciousness develop significantly across childhood. This suggests a long period of development of the bodily self and exciting potential for the use of virtual reality technologies with children.


Assuntos
Estado de Consciência/fisiologia , Autoimagem , Percepção do Tato/fisiologia , Adulto , Criança , Emoções , Feminino , Humanos , Ilusões/psicologia , Masculino , Tato/fisiologia , Adulto Jovem
18.
Curr Biol ; 27(8): R305-R307, 2017 04 24.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28441564

RESUMO

A new study reveals the effects of visual deprivation in early life on the development of multisensory simultaneity perception. To understand the developmental processes underlying this we need to consider the multisensory milieu of the newborn infant.


Assuntos
Percepção Auditiva , Percepção Visual , Humanos , Recém-Nascido
19.
Hum Mov Sci ; 53: 72-83, 2017 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28109545

RESUMO

When localizing touches to the hands, typically developing children and adults show a "crossed hands effect" whereby identifying which hand received a tactile stimulus is less accurate when the hands are crossed than uncrossed. This demonstrates the use of an external frame of reference for locating touches to one's own body. Given that studies indicate that developmental vision plays a role in the emergence of external representations of touch, and reliance on vision for representing the body during action is atypical in developmental coordination disorder (DCD), we investigated external spatial representations of touch in children with DCD using the "crossed hands effect". Nineteen children with DCD aged 7-11years completed a tactile localization task in which posture (uncrossed, crossed) and view (hands seen, unseen) were varied systematically. Their performance was compared to that of 35 typically developing controls (19 of a similar age and cognitive ability, and 16 of a younger age but similar fine motor ability). Like controls, the DCD group exhibited a crossed hands effect, whilst their overall tactile localization performance was weaker than their peers of similar age and cognitive ability, but in line with younger controls of similar motor ability. For children with movement difficulties, these findings indicate tactile localization impairments in relation to age expectations, but apparently typical use of an external reference frame for localizing touch.


Assuntos
Transtornos das Habilidades Motoras/fisiopatologia , Destreza Motora/fisiologia , Percepção do Tato/fisiologia , Análise de Variância , Estudos de Casos e Controles , Criança , Desenvolvimento Infantil/fisiologia , Cognição/fisiologia , Feminino , Mãos/fisiologia , Humanos , Masculino , Postura/fisiologia , Inquéritos e Questionários , Tato/fisiologia
20.
Dev Sci ; 20(6)2017 Nov.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27321051

RESUMO

Studies show that touch in adults is referenced to a representation of the body that is structured topologically according to body parts; the perceived distance between two stimuli crossing over a body part boundary is elongated relative to the perceived distance between two stimuli presented within one body part category. Here we investigate this influence of body parts on tactile space perception in children of 5, 6 and 7 years of age. We presented children with pairs of tactile stimuli on the left hand/arm, either within the hand, within the forearm, or over the wrist. With their eyes closed children were asked to adjust the distance between the thumb and forefinger of their right hand to represent the felt distance between the two tactile stimuli. Like adults, the children perceived the distance between two stimuli that cross the body part boundary to be further apart than those that were presented within the hand or arm. They also perceive tactile distance to be greater on the hand than the arm which is the first observation of Weber's illusion in young children. We propose that a topological mode of body representation is particularly advantageous during early life given that body part categories remain constant while the metric proportions of the body change substantially as the child grows.


Assuntos
Desenvolvimento Infantil/fisiologia , Percepção de Distância/fisiologia , Percepção do Tato/fisiologia , Tato/fisiologia , Extremidade Superior/inervação , Fatores Etários , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Discriminação Psicológica , Feminino , Lateralidade Funcional , Humanos , Masculino , Estimulação Física
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA
...