Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 7 de 7
Filtrar
Mais filtros










Base de dados
Intervalo de ano de publicação
1.
Genes Brain Behav ; 19(7): e12679, 2020 09.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32488937

RESUMO

The imprinted genes Grb10 and Nesp influence impulsive behavior on a delay discounting task in an opposite manner. A recently developed theory suggests that this pattern of behavior may be representative of predicted effects of imprinted genes on tolerance to risk. Here we examine whether mice lacking paternal expression of Grb10 show abnormal behavior across a number of measures indicative of risk-taking. Although Grb10+/p mice show no difference from wild type (WT) littermates in their willingness to explore a novel environment, their behavior on an explicit test of risk-taking, namely the Predator Odor Risk-Taking task, is indicative of an increased willingness to take risks. Follow-up tests suggest that this risk-taking is not simply because of a general decrease in fear, or a general increase in motivation for a food reward, but reflects a change in the trade-off between cost and reward. These data, coupled with previous work on the impulsive behavior of Grb10+/p mice in the delayed reinforcement task, and taken together with our work on mice lacking maternal Nesp, suggest that maternally and paternally expressed imprinted genes oppositely influence risk-taking behavior as predicted.


Assuntos
Proteína Adaptadora GRB10/genética , Impressão Genômica , Assunção de Riscos , Animais , Medo , Feminino , Masculino , Camundongos , Motivação
2.
Genetics ; 209(1): 233-239, 2018 05.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29563147

RESUMO

Imprinted genes are expressed from one parental allele only as a consequence of epigenetic events that take place in the mammalian germ line and are thought to have evolved through intragenomic conflict between parental alleles. We demonstrate, for the first time, oppositional effects of imprinted genes on brain and behavior. Specifically, we show that mice lacking paternal Grb10 make fewer impulsive choices, with no dissociable effects on a separate measure of impulsive action. Taken together with previous work showing that mice lacking maternal Nesp55 make more impulsive choices, this suggests that impulsive choice behavior is a substrate for the action of genomic imprinting. Moreover, the contrasting effect of these two genes suggests that impulsive choices are subject to intragenomic conflict and that maternal and paternal interests pull this behavior in opposite directions. Finally, these data may also indicate that an imbalance in expression of imprinted genes contributes to pathological conditions such as gambling and drug addiction, where impulsive behavior becomes maladaptive.


Assuntos
Comportamento Animal , Proteína Adaptadora GRB10/genética , Expressão Gênica , Impressão Genômica , Análise de Variância , Animais , Imunofluorescência , Proteína Adaptadora GRB10/metabolismo , Imuno-Histoquímica , Comportamento Impulsivo , Masculino , Camundongos
3.
Nat Genet ; 48(12): 1473-1480, 2016 12.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27776119

RESUMO

Pregnancy is a state of high metabolic demand. Fasting diverts metabolism to fatty acid oxidation, and the fasted response occurs much more rapidly in pregnant women than in non-pregnant women. The product of the imprinted DLK1 gene (delta-like homolog 1) is an endocrine signaling molecule that reaches a high concentration in the maternal circulation during late pregnancy. By using mouse models with deleted Dlk1, we show that the fetus is the source of maternal circulating DLK1. In the absence of fetally derived DLK1, the maternal fasting response is impaired. Furthermore, we found that maternal circulating DLK1 levels predict embryonic mass in mice and can differentiate healthy small-for-gestational-age (SGA) infants from pathologically small infants in a human cohort. Therefore, measurement of DLK1 concentration in maternal blood may be a valuable method for diagnosing human disorders associated with impaired DLK1 expression and to predict poor intrauterine growth and complications of pregnancy.


Assuntos
Adaptação Fisiológica , Biomarcadores/sangue , Retardo do Crescimento Fetal/diagnóstico , Feto/metabolismo , Recém-Nascido Pequeno para a Idade Gestacional/sangue , Peptídeos e Proteínas de Sinalização Intercelular/sangue , Complicações na Gravidez/diagnóstico , Animais , Proteínas de Ligação ao Cálcio , Estudos de Casos e Controles , Estudos de Coortes , Feminino , Retardo do Crescimento Fetal/sangue , Idade Gestacional , Humanos , Recém-Nascido , Camundongos , Gravidez , Complicações na Gravidez/sangue
4.
Mamm Genome ; 25(1-2): 87-93, 2014 Feb.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23974804

RESUMO

Imprinted genes are defined by their parent-of-origin-specific monoallelic expression. Although the epigenetic mechanisms regulating imprinted gene expression have been widely studied, their functional importance is still unclear. Imprinted genes are associated with a number of physiologies, including placental function and foetal growth, energy homeostasis, and brain and behaviour. This review focuses on genomic imprinting in the brain and on two imprinted genes in particular, Nesp and paternal Grb10, which, when manipulated in animals, have been shown to influence adult behaviour. These two genes are of particular interest as they are expressed in discrete and overlapping neural regions, recognised as key "imprinting hot spots" in the brain. Furthermore, these two genes do not appear to influence placental function and/or maternal provisioning of offspring. Consequently, by understanding their behavioural function we may begin to shed light on the evolutionary significance of imprinted genes in the adult brain, independent of the recognised role in maternal care. In addition, we discuss the potential future directions of research investigating the function of these two genes and the behavioural role of imprinted genes more generally.


Assuntos
Encéfalo/fisiologia , Expressão Gênica , Genética Comportamental , Impressão Genômica , Adaptação Biológica , Adulto , Animais , Conflito Psicológico , Proteína Adaptadora GRB10/genética , Proteína Adaptadora GRB10/metabolismo , Subunidades alfa Gs de Proteínas de Ligação ao GTP/genética , Subunidades alfa Gs de Proteínas de Ligação ao GTP/metabolismo , Humanos
5.
Eur J Neurosci ; 39(4): 520-30, 2014 Feb.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24283296

RESUMO

Assessing risk is an essential part of human behaviour and may be disrupted in a number of psychiatric conditions. Currently, in many animal experimental designs the basis of the potential 'risk' is loss or attenuation of reward, which fail to capture 'real-life' risky situations where there is a trade-off between a separate cost and reward. The development of rodent tasks where two separate and conflicting factors are traded against each other has begun to address this discrepancy. Here, we discuss the merits of these risk-taking tasks and describe the development of a novel test for mice - the 'predator-odour risk-taking' task. This paradigm encapsulates a naturalistic approach to measuring risk-taking behaviour where mice have to balance the benefit of gaining a food reward with the cost of exposure to a predator odour using a range of different odours (rat, cat and fox). We show that the 'predator-odour risk-taking' task was sensitive to the trade-off between cost and benefit by demonstrating reduced motivation to collect food reward in the presence of these different predator odours in two strains of mice and, also, if the value of the food reward was reduced. The 'predator-odour risk-taking' task therefore provides a strong platform for the investigation of the genetic substrates of risk-taking behaviour using mouse models, and adds a further dimension to other recently developed rodent tests.


Assuntos
Comportamento Animal , Neurociências/métodos , Recompensa , Assunção de Riscos , Animais , Gatos , Medo , Locomoção , Masculino , Camundongos , Camundongos Endogâmicos C57BL , Odorantes , Reflexo de Sobressalto
6.
Curr Protoc Mouse Biol ; 4(2): 35-45, 2014 Jun 16.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25723917

RESUMO

Impulsive behavior is a key constituent of many psychiatric illnesses, with maladaptive response control being a feature of disorders such as ADHD, schizophrenia, mania, and addiction. In order to understand the neurological underpinnings of impulsivity, a number of behavioral tasks have been developed for use with animal models. Data from studies with rats and other animals have led to the idea of the existence of dissociable components of impulsivity, which in turn informs studies of human disorders and potentially the development of specific therapies. Increasingly, mouse models are being used to investigate the known genetic contribution to psychiatric disorders in which abnormal response control leads to altered impulsive behaviors. In order to maximize the potential of these mouse models, it is important that researchers take into account the non-unitary nature of response control and impulsivity. In this article, we briefly review the tasks available to behavioral neuroscientists and how these can be used in order to tease apart the contribution of a specific genetic lesion into the discrete aspects of impulsive behavior.


Assuntos
Comportamento Impulsivo , Camundongos/fisiologia , Modelos Animais , Animais , Humanos , Camundongos/genética
7.
Nat Commun ; 4: 2311, 2013.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23921428

RESUMO

Imprinted, maternally silenced insulin-like growth factor-2 is expressed in both the foetus and placenta and has been shown to have roles in foetal and placental development in animal models. Here we compared mice engineered to be null for the placenta-specific P0 transcript (insulin-like growth factor-2-P0 KO) to mice with disruptions of all four insulin-like growth factor-2 transcripts, and therefore null for insulin-like growth factor-2 in both placenta and foetus (insulin-like growth factor-2-total KO). Both models lead to intrauterine growth restriction but dissociate between a situation where there is an imbalance between foetal demand and placental supply of nutrients (the insulin-like growth factor-2-P0 KO) and one where demand and supply is more balanced (the insulin-like growth factor-2-total KO). Increased reactivity to anxiety-provoking stimuli is manifested later in life only in those animals where there is a mismatch between placental supply and foetal demand for nutrients during gestation. Our findings further distinguish placental dysfunction from intrauterine growth restriction and reveal a role for the placenta in long-term programming of emotional behaviour.


Assuntos
Envelhecimento/patologia , Ansiedade/patologia , Fator de Crescimento Insulin-Like II/deficiência , Placenta/metabolismo , Estimulação Acústica , Animais , Animais Recém-Nascidos , Ansiedade/genética , Comportamento Animal , Peso ao Nascer , Peso Corporal , Modelos Animais de Doenças , Comportamento Exploratório , Feminino , Regulação da Expressão Gênica no Desenvolvimento , Impressão Genômica/genética , Hipocampo/metabolismo , Hipocampo/patologia , Fator de Crescimento Insulin-Like II/genética , Fator de Crescimento Insulin-Like II/metabolismo , Aprendizagem em Labirinto , Camundongos , Camundongos Knockout , Gravidez
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA
...