Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 10 de 10
Filtrar
Mais filtros










Base de dados
Intervalo de ano de publicação
1.
Conscious Cogn ; 53: 211-221, 2017 08.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28688964

RESUMO

A study investigated how the ability to suppress thoughts in the laboratory was affected by type of thought suppressed (positive, negative, neutral), participants' age and working memory capacity (WMC). Linked variables (Use of thought suppression, social desirability, and mindfulness) were measured to assess whether they modified susceptibility to thought intrusion. Younger, middle aged and older adults suppressed three different valenced thoughts in a counterbalanced order for 5-min per thought. Participants then completed a WMC task and questionnaire measures of the linked variables. Valence had no effect on intrusions. WMC was positively related to intrusions; higher WMC corresponded to greater intrusions. Age was negatively related to intrusions; with increasing age intrusions decreased. Hierarchical regression showed only age and backward digit span (WMC) significantly predicted intrusions. The relationship between WMC and intrusions was not moderated by age. WMC and age both independently predict level of intrusion, and no synergistic effect was found.


Assuntos
Envelhecimento/fisiologia , Atenção/fisiologia , Memória de Curto Prazo/fisiologia , Atenção Plena , Pensamento/fisiologia , Adolescente , Adulto , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Adulto Jovem
2.
Addict Behav ; 44: 65-70, 2015 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25648574

RESUMO

Research to understand how individuals cope with intrusive negative or threatening thoughts suggests a variety of different cognitive strategies aimed at thought control. In this review, two of these strategies--thought suppression and repressive coping--are discussed in the context of addictive behaviour. Thought suppression involves conscious, volitional attempts to expel a thought from awareness, whereas repressive coping, which involves the avoidance of thoughts without the corresponding conscious intention, appears to be a far more automated process. Whilst there has been an emerging body of research exploring the role of thought suppression in addictive behaviour, there remains a dearth of research which has considered the role of repressive coping in the development of, and recovery from, addiction. Based on a review of the literature, and a discussion of the supposed mechanisms which underpin these strategies for exercising mental control, a conceptual model is proposed which posits a potential common mechanism. This model makes a number of predictions which require exploration in future research to fully understand the cognitive strategies utilised by individuals to control intrusive thoughts related to their addictive behaviour.


Assuntos
Adaptação Psicológica , Comportamento Aditivo/psicologia , Repressão Psicológica , Humanos , Atenção Plena , Psicometria , Pensamento
3.
Psychopharmacology (Berl) ; 219(1): 205-11, 2012 Jan.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21735073

RESUMO

RATIONALE: Suppressing smoking thoughts has been shown to result in elevated smoking. However, the effect of suppressing smoking thoughts on desire to smoke and withdrawal symptoms has not been investigated. OBJECTIVES: We examined the effects of suppressing smoking thoughts on the subsequent desire to smoke and on tobacco withdrawal symptoms, relative to groups that were either thinking about anything they wished or actively thinking about smoking. METHODS: A randomised experimental study compared the effects of three manipulations (suppressed smoking thoughts, expressed smoking thoughts and thoughts of anything they wished) on desire and withdrawal immediately after the manipulation and 5 and 10 min after. RESULTS: Suppressing smoking thoughts did not result in elevated subsequent desire to smoke, relative to the other manipulations. Suppressing smoking thoughts resulted in a significant elevation in hunger ratings, relative to the other manipulations, at all measurement times. There were no significant effects for the other withdrawal symptoms. Self-reported greater use of thought suppression in everyday life was significantly associated with greater desire to smoke at baseline and was associated with lower mindfulness scores. CONCLUSIONS: Laboratory-instructed suppression of smoking thoughts is associated with increased reports of hunger but did not lead to increases in other withdrawal symptoms or elevated desire to smoke. Reports of everyday use of thought suppression are associated with elevated desire to smoke at baseline. Further investigations need to assess the effect of suppressing smoking cravings, instead of general smoking thoughts, on desire to smoke and tobacco withdrawal.


Assuntos
Repressão Psicológica , Abandono do Hábito de Fumar/métodos , Abandono do Hábito de Fumar/psicologia , Fumar/psicologia , Síndrome de Abstinência a Substâncias/psicologia , Pensamento , Adolescente , Adulto , Idoso , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Fumar/terapia , Síndrome de Abstinência a Substâncias/terapia , Adulto Jovem
5.
Psychol Sci ; 21(9): 1225-30, 2010 Sep.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20660892

RESUMO

Thought suppression is a method frequently employed by individuals who are trying to control their thoughts and behaviors. Although this strategy is known to actually increase unwanted thoughts, it is unclear whether thought suppression also results in behavioral rebound. The study presented in this article investigated the effects of suppressing thoughts of smoking in everyday life on the number of cigarettes subsequently smoked. Study participants recorded their daily cigarette intake and stress levels over a 3-week period. In Week 1 and Week 3, participants monitored intake and stress. During Week 2, in addition to monitoring intake and stress, participants in the experimental groups either suppressed or expressed smoking thoughts, whereas the control group continued monitoring. Our results showed a clear behavioral rebound: The suppression group smoked significantly more in Week 3 than the expression or control group did. Moreover, the tendency to suppress thoughts (measured by the White Bear Suppression Inventory) was positively related to the number of attempts to quit smoking. The implications of our findings for smoking cessation are discussed.


Assuntos
Fumar/psicologia , Pensamento , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Repressão Psicológica , Abandono do Hábito de Fumar/psicologia , Estresse Psicológico/psicologia
6.
Psychol Aging ; 25(2): 391-404, 2010 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20545423

RESUMO

In two studies, the special status of flashbulb memories was investigated by contrasting the effects of age on the phenomenology and consistency of flashbulb memories of September 11, over a 2-year delay period, with those of a mundane staged control event: participants learning that they had not won a small prize. Flashbulb memories produced no significant age effects for either phenomenological characteristics or test-retest consistency, as predicted by Mather's (2004) emotional compensation hypothesis. By contrast, the control event resulted in significant age effects for phenomenological characteristics (e.g., specificity and the amount of detail recalled) but not for test-retest consistency. Furthermore, in both age groups, memories of September 11 were significantly more vivid, detailed, and consistent than control memories even though the test-retest interval was twice as long for flashbulb memories. In addition, correlations between consistency scores and ratings of rehearsal were positive for control memories but negative for flashbulb memories. The theoretical implications of these findings for research on cognitive aging and flashbulb memories are discussed.


Assuntos
Envelhecimento/psicologia , Nível de Alerta , Acontecimentos que Mudam a Vida , Rememoração Mental , Ataques Terroristas de 11 de Setembro , Adulto , Idoso , Associação , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Retenção Psicológica , Adulto Jovem
7.
Appetite ; 54(3): 499-503, 2010 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20152872

RESUMO

Recent research has shown that suppressing food related thoughts can cause a subsequent increase in consumption relative to groups not suppressing, or thinking about food. The present study examined whether the effects of thought suppression on subsequent eating behaviour would interact with participants restrained eating status. One hundred and sixteen female participants were split into three groups. One third suppressed thoughts of chocolate, one third thought about chocolate and the final third thought about anything they wished. Following this, participants took part in a task where they rated two brands of chocolate on several taste characteristics. Participants were unaware that the dependent variable was the amount of chocolate consumed and not taste preference. Participants also completed measures of dietary restraint, craving, guilt and thought suppression. Results indicated that restrained eaters in the suppression condition consumed significantly more chocolate than restrained eaters in the expression or control condition. Participants low on restraint ate statistically equivalent amounts in all three groups. In addition, participants reporting frequent use of thought suppression (assessed by the White Bear Suppression Inventory) reported greater chocolate cravings.


Assuntos
Comportamento Alimentar/psicologia , Pensamento/fisiologia , Atitude , Cacau , Feminino , Preferências Alimentares/psicologia , Culpa , Humanos , Análise de Regressão , Adulto Jovem
8.
Appetite ; 50(2-3): 415-21, 2008.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18006187

RESUMO

One hundred and thirty-four non-dieting participants spent 5 min thinking aloud under three different conditions. Participants either suppressed or expressed thoughts of eating chocolate, or verbalised with no further instructions. After thinking aloud, all participants took part in a taste preference task where they tried two brands of chocolate and answered questions about their preference. Unbeknownst to participants the variable of interest was the amount of chocolate eaten, not their preference. Results indicated an interaction between condition (suppression vs. expression vs. control) and gender. Both male and female participants showed a behavioural rebound effect, consuming significantly more chocolate after suppression than participants in the verbalise only control group. However, in the expression group, a clear difference between males and females was manifested, while females ate a similar amount of chocolate in the expression and verbalise only control groups, males ate the most chocolate in the expression group and this was significantly greater than the amount eaten after suppression or the verbalise only control group.


Assuntos
Cacau , Ingestão de Alimentos/psicologia , Comportamento Alimentar/psicologia , Controles Informais da Sociedade , Pensamento , Adulto , Comportamento , Feminino , Preferências Alimentares , Humanos , Masculino
9.
Aging Ment Health ; 11(4): 394-404, 2007 Jul.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17612803

RESUMO

The present study was conducted to test the hypothesis that the increased psychological well-being and positivity effect in old age can be related to a high prevalence of repressive coping in healthy older adults. Both older (mean age 73) and younger (mean age 20) adults completed a range of indices measuring psychopathology and repressive coping. Results showed that older adults scored lower than younger adults on almost all indices of psychopathology, and were more likely to be classed as repressive copers than younger adults (41% versus 11%, respectively). Furthermore, when the repressive copers and borderline repressors were removed from both samples, age effects on several, but not all, measures of psychopathology disappeared, indicating that even older non-repressors showed better mental health than young non-repressors. Possible mechanisms of increased positivity, in terms of repressive coping and reductions in intrusive thoughts and rumination in old age, are discussed.


Assuntos
Adaptação Psicológica , Satisfação Pessoal , Psicopatologia , Repressão Psicológica , Adolescente , Adulto , Fatores Etários , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Inquéritos e Questionários , Reino Unido
10.
Conscious Cogn ; 12(4): 684-94, 2003 Dec.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-14656510

RESUMO

Participants were asked to carry out a series of simple tasks while following mental control instructions. In advance of each task, they either suppressed thoughts of their intention to perform the task, concentrated on such thoughts, or monitored their thoughts without trying to change them. Suppression resulted in reduced reports of intentionality as compared to monitoring, and as compared to concentration. There was a weak trend for suppression to enhance reported intentionality for a repetition of the action carried out after suppression instructions had been discontinued.


Assuntos
Conscientização , Estado de Consciência , Ego , Inibição Psicológica , Controle Interno-Externo , Desempenho Psicomotor , Pensamento , Adulto , Atenção , Feminino , Humanos , Intenção , Cinestesia , Masculino , Teoria da Construção Pessoal , Resolução de Problemas
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA
...