Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 53
Filtrar
Mais filtros










Base de dados
Intervalo de ano de publicação
1.
Contemp Clin Trials ; 130: 107236, 2023 07.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37230167

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: Stroke can lead to lasting sensorimotor deficits of the upper limb (UL) persisting into the chronic phase despite intensive rehabilitation. A major impairment of reaching after stroke is a decreased range of active elbow extension, which in turn leads to the use of compensatory movements. Retraining movement patterns relies on cognition and motor learning principles. Implicit learning may lead to better outcomes than explicit learning. Error augmentation (EA) is a feedback modality based on implicit learning resulting in improved precision and speed of UL reaching movements in people with stroke. However, accompanying changes in UL joint movement patterns have not been investigated. The objective of this study is to determine the capacity for implicit motor learning in people with chronic stroke and how this capacity is affected by post-stroke cognitive impairments. METHODS: Fifty-two subjects who have chronic stroke will practice reaching movements 3×/wk. for 9 wk. in a virtual reality environment. Participants will be randomly allocated to 1 of 2 groups to train with or without EA feedback. Outcome measures (pre-, post- and follow-up) will be: endpoint precision, speed, smoothness, and straightness and joint (UL and trunk) kinematics during a functional reaching task. The degree of cognitive impairment, lesion profile, and integrity of descending white matter tracts will be related to training outcomes. CONCLUSIONS: The results will inform us which patients can best benefit from training programs that rely on motor learning and utilize enhanced feedback. TRIAL STATUS: Ethical approval for this study was finalized in May 2022. Recruitment and data collection is actively in progress and is planned to finish in 2026. Data analysis and evaluation will occur subsequently, and the final results will be published.


Assuntos
Reabilitação do Acidente Vascular Cerebral , Acidente Vascular Cerebral , Humanos , Reabilitação do Acidente Vascular Cerebral/métodos , Retroalimentação , Recuperação de Função Fisiológica , Resultado do Tratamento , Extremidade Superior , Sobreviventes
2.
Exp Brain Res ; 238(10): 2359-2372, 2020 Oct.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32766959

RESUMO

There are contralateral and less studied ipsilateral (i), indirect cortical descending projections to motoneurons (MNs). We compared ipsilateral cortical descending influences on MNs of wrist flexors by applying transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) over the right primary motor cortex at actively maintained flexion and extension wrist positions in uni- and bimanual tasks in right-handed participants (n = 23). The iTMS response includes a short latency (~ 25 ms) motor evoked potential (iMEP), a silent period (iSP) and a long latency (~ 60 ms) facilitation called rebound (iRB). We also investigated whether the interaction between the two hands while holding an object in a bimanual task involves ipsilateral cortical descending influences. In the unimanual task, iTMS responses in the right wrist flexors were unaffected by changes in wrist position. In the bimanual task with an object, iMEPs in the right wrist flexors were larger when the ipsilateral wrist was in flexion compared to extension. Without the object, only iRB were larger when the ipsilateral wrist was extended. Thus, ipsilateral cortical descending influences on MNs were modulated only in bimanual tasks and depended on how the two hands interacted. It is concluded that the left and right cortices cooperate in bimanual tasks involving holding an object with both hands, with possible involvement of oligo- and poly-synaptic, as well as transcallosal projections to MNs. The possible involvement of spinal and transcortical stretch and cutaneous reflexes in bimanual tasks when holding an object is discussed in the context of the well-established notion that indirect, referent control underlies motor actions.


Assuntos
Córtex Motor , Punho , Eletromiografia , Potencial Evocado Motor , Lateralidade Funcional , Humanos , Movimento , Estimulação Magnética Transcraniana , Articulação do Punho
3.
J Neurophysiol ; 107(9): 2560-9, 2012 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22298827

RESUMO

Most falls in older adults occur when walking, specifically following a trip. This study investigated the short- and longer term responses of young (n = 24, 27.6 ± 4.5 yr) and older adults (n = 18, 69.1 ± 4.2 yr) to a trip during gait at comfortable speed and the role of interlimb coordination in recovery from tripping. Subjects walked on a self-paced treadmill when forward movement of their dominant leg was unexpectedly arrested for 250 ms. Recovery of center of mass (COM) movements and of double-support duration following perturbation was determined. In addition, the disruption and recovery of interlimb coordination of the arms and legs was evaluated. Although young and older subjects used similar lower limb strategies in response to the trip, older adults had less stable COM movement patterns before perturbation, had longer transient destabilization (>25%) after perturbation, required more gait cycles to recover double-support duration (older, 3.48 ± 0.7 cycles; young, 2.88 ± 0.4 cycles), and had larger phase shifts that persisted after perturbation (older, -83° to -90°; young, -39° to -42°). Older adults also had larger disruptions to interlimb coordination of the arms and legs. The timing of the initial disruption in coordination was correlated with the disturbance in gait stability only in young adults. In older adults, greater initial COM instability was related to greater longer term arm incoordination. These results suggest a relationship between interlimb coordination and gait stability, which may be associated with fall risk in older adults. Reduced coordination and gait stability suggest a need for stability-related functional training even in high-functioning older adults.


Assuntos
Envelhecimento/fisiologia , Braço/fisiologia , Marcha/fisiologia , Perna (Membro)/fisiologia , Equilíbrio Postural/fisiologia , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Adulto , Idoso , Teste de Esforço/métodos , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Adulto Jovem
4.
J Neurophysiol ; 97(6): 4069-78, 2007 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17428903

RESUMO

The vestibulospinal system likely plays an essential role in motor equivalence--the ability to reach the desired motor goal despite intentional or imposed changes in the number of body segments involved in the task. To test this hypothesis, we compared the ability of healthy subjects and patients with unilateral vestibular lesions (surgical acoustic neuroma resection 0.6 to 6.7 yr before the study) to maintain either the same hand position or the same trajectory of within arm reach movements while flexing the trunk, in the absence of vision. In randomly selected trials, the trunk motion was prevented by an electromagnetic device. Healthy subjects were able to preserve the hand position or trajectory by modifying the elbow and shoulder joint rotations in a condition-dependent way, at a minimal latency of about 60 ms after the trunk movement onset. In contrast, six of seven patients showed deficits in the compensatory angular modifications at least in one of two tasks so that 30-100% of the trunk displacement was not compensated and thus influenced the hand position or trajectory. Results suggest that vestibular influences evoked by the head motion during trunk flexion play a major role in maintaining the consistency of arm motor actions in external space despite changes in the number of body segments involved. Our findings also suggest that despite long-term plasticity in the vestibular system and related neural structures, unilateral vestibular lesion may reduce the capacity of the nervous system to achieve motor equivalence.


Assuntos
Corpo Humano , Movimento/fisiologia , Postura , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Vestíbulo do Labirinto/fisiologia , Adulto , Análise de Variância , Eletromiografia , Feminino , Lateralidade Funcional , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Análise e Desempenho de Tarefas , Extremidade Superior/inervação , Doenças Vestibulares/fisiopatologia
5.
Parkinsonism Relat Disord ; 13(7): 425-33, 2007 Oct.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17446116

RESUMO

The ability of dopamine replacement to restore rapid motor adjustments in Parkinson's disease (PD) was investigated. Medicated and non-medicated patients performed finger-to-nose movements while simultaneously bending the trunk forward, without vision. Trunk motion was blocked unexpectedly, necessitating rapid adjustments in arm trajectories. Patients exhibited irregular hand paths, plateaus in hand velocity, and prolonged movement times, which were significantly greater in perturbed trials. Medication improved kinematics but perturbation-induced disturbances persisted and did not approximate the levels of non-perturbed trials nor those of controls. Dopaminergic replenishment in PD may therefore have limited restorative benefits for rapid context-specific motor control.


Assuntos
Adaptação Fisiológica/fisiologia , Dopamina/metabolismo , Movimento/fisiologia , Doença de Parkinson/fisiopatologia , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Adaptação Fisiológica/efeitos dos fármacos , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Análise de Variância , Antiparkinsonianos/uso terapêutico , Fenômenos Biomecânicos , Técnicas de Exercício e de Movimento/métodos , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Movimento/efeitos dos fármacos , Doença de Parkinson/tratamento farmacológico , Desempenho Psicomotor/efeitos dos fármacos
6.
Exp Brain Res ; 163(4): 468-86, 2005 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15690154

RESUMO

It has been suggested that multijoint movements result from the specification of a referent configuration of the body. The activity of muscles and forces required for movements emerge depending on the difference between the actual and referent body configurations. We identified the referent arm configurations specified by the nervous system to bring the arm to the target position both in healthy individuals and in those with arm motor paresis due to stroke. From an initial position of the right arm, subjects matched a force equivalent to 30% of their maximal voluntary force in that position. The external force, produced at the handle of a double-joint manipulandum by two torque motors, pulled the hand to the left (165 degrees ) or pushed it to the right (0 degrees ). For both the initial conditions, three directions of the final force (0 degrees , +20 degrees , and -20 degrees ) with respect to the direction of the initial force were used. Subjects were instructed not to intervene when the load was unexpectedly partially or completely removed. Both groups of subjects produced similar responses to unloading of the double-joint arm system. Partial removal of the load resulted in distinct final hand positions associated with unique shoulder-elbow configurations and joint torques. The net static torque at each joint before and after unloading was represented as a function of the two joint angles describing a planar surface or invariant characteristic in 3D torque/angle coordinates. For each initial condition, the referent arm configuration was identified as the combination of elbow and shoulder angles at which the net torques at the two joints were zero. These configurations were different for different initial conditions. The identification of the referent configuration was possible for all healthy participants and for most individuals with hemiparesis suggesting that they preserved the ability to adapt their central commands-the referent arm configurations-to accommodate changes in external load conditions. Despite the preservation of the basic response patterns, individuals with stroke damage had a more restricted range of hand trajectories following unloading, an increased instability around the final endpoint position, altered patterns of elbow and shoulder muscle coactivation, and differences in the dispersion of referent configurations in elbow-shoulder joint space compared to healthy individuals. Moreover, 4 out of 12 individuals with hemiparesis were unable to specify referent configurations of the arm in a consistent way. It is suggested that problems in the specification of the referent configuration may be responsible for the inability of some individuals with stroke to produce coordinated multijoint movements. The present work adds three findings to the motor control literature concerning stroke: non-significant torque/angle relationships in some subjects, narrower range of referent arm configurations, and instability about the final position. This is the first demonstration of the feasibility of the concept of the referent configuration for the double-joint muscle-reflex system and the ability of some individuals with stroke to produce task-specific adjustments of this configuration.


Assuntos
Braço/fisiopatologia , Dano Encefálico Crônico/fisiopatologia , Infarto Cerebral/fisiopatologia , Hemiplegia/fisiopatologia , Postura/fisiologia , Acidente Vascular Cerebral/fisiopatologia , Adaptação Fisiológica/fisiologia , Adulto , Braço/inervação , Fenômenos Biomecânicos , Encéfalo/fisiologia , Dano Encefálico Crônico/etiologia , Dano Encefálico Crônico/patologia , Infarto Cerebral/patologia , Articulação do Cotovelo/fisiologia , Feminino , Hemiplegia/etiologia , Hemiplegia/patologia , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Transtornos dos Movimentos/etiologia , Transtornos dos Movimentos/patologia , Transtornos dos Movimentos/fisiopatologia , Contração Muscular/fisiologia , Músculo Esquelético/inervação , Músculo Esquelético/fisiopatologia , Amplitude de Movimento Articular/fisiologia , Articulação do Ombro/fisiologia , Acidente Vascular Cerebral/patologia , Torque , Suporte de Carga/fisiologia
7.
Exp Brain Res ; 159(1): 23-32, 2004 Nov.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15480587

RESUMO

The ability of patients with Parkinson's disease (PD) to compensate for unexpected perturbations remains relatively unexplored. To address this issue PD subjects were required to compensate at the arm for an unexpected mechanical perturbation of the trunk while performing a trunk-assisted reach. Twelve healthy and nine PD subjects (off medication) performed trunk-assisted reaching movements without vision or knowledge of results to a remembered target in the ipsilateral (T1) or contralateral (T2) workspace. On 60% of the trials trunk motion was unrestrained (free condition). On the remaining 40% of randomly selected trials trunk motion was arrested at movement onset (blocked condition). If subjects appropriately changed arm joint angles to compensate for the trunk arrest, there should be spatial and temporal invariance in the hand trajectories and in the endpoint errors across conditions. The control group successfully changed their arm configuration in a context-dependent manner which resulted in invariant hand trajectory profiles across the free and blocked conditions. More so, they initiated these changes rapidly after the trunk perturbation (group mean 70 ms). Some PD subjects were unable to maintain invariant hand paths and movement errors across conditions. Their hand velocity profiles were also more variable relative to those of the healthy subjects in the blocked-trunk trials but not in the free-trunk trials. Furthermore, the latency of compensatory changes in arm joint angles in movements toward T1 was longer in the PD group (group mean 153 ms). Finally, PD subjects' arm and trunk were desynchronized at movement onset, confirming our previous findings and consistent with PD patients' known problems in the sequential or parallel generation of different movement components. The findings that individual PD subjects were unsuccessful or delayed in producing context-dependent responses at the arm to unexpected perturbations of the trunk suggests that the basal ganglia are important nodes in the organization of adaptive behavior.


Assuntos
Adaptação Fisiológica/fisiologia , Transtornos das Habilidades Motoras/fisiopatologia , Doença de Parkinson/fisiopatologia , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Extremidade Superior/fisiologia , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Análise de Variância , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Estimulação Luminosa/métodos , Postura/fisiologia
8.
Knee Surg Sports Traumatol Arthrosc ; 12(3): 203-16, 2004 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-14689167

RESUMO

Previous studies of movement kinematics in patients with a ruptured anterior cruciate ligament (ACL) have focused on changes in angular displacement in a single joint, usually flexion/extension of the knee. In the present study, we investigated the effect of an ACL injury on the overall limb interjoint coordination. We asked healthy and chronic ACL-deficient male subjects to perform eight types of movements: forward squats, backward squats, sideways squats, squats on one leg, going up a step, going down a step, walking three steps, and stepping in place. Depending on the movement concerned, we applied principal component (PC) analysis to 3 or 4 degrees of freedom (DFs): thigh flexion/extension, knee flexion/extension, ankle flexion/extension, thigh abduction/adduction. The first three DFs were investigated in all movements. PC analysis identifies linear combinations of DFs. Movements with a fixed ratio between DFs are thus described by only one PC or synergy. PCs were computed for the entire movement as well as for the period of time when the foot was in contact with the ground. For both the control and the injured groups, two synergies (PC vectors) usually accounted for more than 95% of the DFs' angular excursions. It was possible to describe 95-99% of some movements using only one synergy. Compared to control subjects, injured subjects employed different synergies for going up a step, walking three steps, squatting sideways, and squatting forward, both in the injured and uninjured legs. Those movements may thus be more indicative of injury than other movements. Although ACL-deficiency did not increase asymmetry (angle between the PCs of the same movement performed on the right and the left sides), this result is not conclusive because of the comparatively low number of subjects who participated in the study. However, the finding that synergies in both legs of patients were different from those in control subjects for going up a step and walking three steps suggests that interjoint coordination was affected for both legs, so that the asymmetry index might have been preserved despite the injury. There was also a relationship between the asymmetry index for squatting on one leg, squatting forward, walking three steps and some of the outcomes of the knee injury and osteoarthritis outcome score (pain, symptoms, activities of daily living, sport and recreation function, and knee-related quality of life). This suggests that significant differences in the asymmetry index could be obtained if more severely-injured patients participated in this study. It is possible that subjects compensated for their mechanical deficiencies by modifying muscle activation patterns. Synergies were not only modified in injured subjects, but also rearranged: the percentage of movement explained by the first PC was different for the injured and/or uninjured legs of patients, as compared to the legs of the control group, for going up a step, going down a step, walking three steps, and squatting forward. We concluded that the analysis of interjoint coordination may be efficient in characterizing motor deficits in people with knee injuries.


Assuntos
Articulação do Tornozelo/fisiopatologia , Lesões do Ligamento Cruzado Anterior , Articulação do Joelho/fisiopatologia , Atividade Motora/fisiologia , Coxa da Perna/fisiopatologia , Adulto , Fenômenos Biomecânicos , Estudos de Casos e Controles , Humanos , Masculino , Análise de Componente Principal , Ruptura
9.
Exp Brain Res ; 153(3): 343-55, 2003 Dec.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-14504854

RESUMO

During trunk-assisted reaching to targets placed within arm's length, the influence of trunk motion on the hand trajectory is compensated for by changes in the arm configuration. The role of proprioception in this compensation was investigated by analyzing the movements of 2 deafferented and 12 healthy subjects. Subjects reached to remembered targets (placed approximately 80 degrees ipsilateral or approximately 45 degrees contralateral to the sagittal midline) with an active forward movement of the trunk produced by hip flexion. In 40% of randomly selected trials, trunk motion was mechanically blocked. No visual feedback was provided during the experiment. The hand trajectory and velocity profiles of healthy subjects remained invariant whether or not the trunk was blocked. The invariance was achieved by changes in arm interjoint coordination that, for reaches toward the ipsilateral target, started as early as 50 ms after the perturbation. Both deafferented subjects exhibited considerable, though incomplete, compensation for the effects of the perturbation. Compensation was more successful for reaches to the ipsilateral target. Both deafferented subjects showed invariance between conditions (unobstructed or blocked trunk motion) in their hand paths to the ipsilateral target, and one did to the contralateral target. For the other deafferented subject, hand paths in the two types of trials began to deviate after about 50% into the movement, because of excessive elbow extension. In movements to the ipsilateral target, when deafferented subjects compensated successfully, the changes in arm joint angles were initiated as early as 50 ms after the trunk perturbation, similar to healthy subjects. Although the deafferented subjects showed less than ideal compensatory control, they compensated to a remarkably large extent given their complete loss of proprioception. The presence of partial compensation in the absence of vision and proprioception points to the likelihood that not only proprioception but also vestibulospinal pathways help mediate this compensation.


Assuntos
Denervação/efeitos adversos , Movimento/fisiologia , Propriocepção/fisiologia , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Distúrbios Somatossensoriais/fisiopatologia , Adaptação Fisiológica/fisiologia , Vias Aferentes/lesões , Vias Aferentes/fisiopatologia , Idoso , Braço/inervação , Braço/fisiologia , Vias Eferentes/fisiologia , Retroalimentação/fisiologia , Feminino , Humanos , Articulações/fisiologia , Masculino , Mecanorreceptores/fisiologia , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Valores de Referência , Núcleos Vestibulares/fisiologia
10.
Exp Brain Res ; 151(3): 289-300, 2003 Aug.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12819841

RESUMO

A technique is described that characterizes the dynamics of the interjoint coordination of arm reaching movements in healthy subjects (n=10) and in patients who had sustained a left-sided cerebrovascular accident (n=18). All participants were right-handed. Data from the affected right arm of patients with stroke were compared with those from the right arm of healthy subjects. Seated subjects made 25 pointing movements in a single session. Movements were made from an initial target located ipsilaterally to the right arm beside the body, to a final target located in front of the subject in the contralateral arm workspace. Kinematic data from the finger, wrist, elbow, both shoulders and sternum were recorded in three dimensions at 200 Hz with an optical tracking system. Analysis of interjoint coordination was based on the patterns of temporal delay between rotations at two adjacent joints (shoulder and elbow). The data were reduced to a single graph (Temporal Coordination or TC index) integrating the essential temporal characteristics of joint movement (the angular displacements, velocities and timing). TC segments, duration and amplitude, were analysed. The analysis was sensitive to the differences in interjoint coordination between healthy subjects and patients with arm motor deficits. In patients, the temporal coordination between elbow and shoulder movements was disrupted from the middle to the end of the reach. More specifically, in mid-reach, all patients had difficulty coordinating elbow flexion with shoulder horizontal adduction. In addition, patients with severe arm hemiparesis had difficulty changing elbow movement direction from flexion to extension and in coordinating this change with shoulder movement. At the end of the reach, patients with severe hemiparesis had deficits in the execution of elbow extension while all patients had impaired coordination of elbow extension and shoulder horizontal adduction. In addition, active ranges of joint motions were significantly decreased in the stroke compared to the healthy subjects. Finally, TC analysis revealed significant relationships between specific aspects of disrupted interjoint coordination and the level of motor impairment, suggesting that it may be a useful tool in the identification of specific movement coordination deficits in neurological impaired populations that can be targeted in treatment for arm motor recovery.


Assuntos
Articulação do Cotovelo/fisiologia , Movimento/fisiologia , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Articulação do Ombro/fisiologia , Acidente Vascular Cerebral/fisiopatologia , Adulto , Idoso , Análise de Variância , Braço/fisiologia , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Estatísticas não Paramétricas
11.
Exp Brain Res ; 138(3): 288-303, 2001 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11460767

RESUMO

Movements of different body segments may be combined in different ways to achieve the same motor goal. How this is accomplished by the nervous system was investigated by having subjects make fast pointing movements with the arm in combination with a forward bending of the trunk that was unexpectedly blocked in some trials. Subjects moved their hand above the surface of a table without vision from an initial position near the midline of the chest to remembered targets placed within the reach of the arm in either the ipsi- or contralateral workspace. In experiment 1, subjects were instructed to make fast arm movements to the target without corrections whether or not the trunk was arrested. Only minor changes were found in the hand trajectory and velocity profile in response to the trunk arrest, and these changes were seen only late in the movement. In contrast, the patterns of the interjoint coordination substantially changed in response to the trunk arrest, suggesting the presence of compensatory arm-trunk coordination minimizing the deflections from the hand trajectory regardless of whether the trunk is recruited or mechanically blocked. Changes in the arm interjoint coordination in response to the trunk arrest could be detected kinematically at a minimal latency of 50 ms. This finding suggests a rapid reflex compensatory mechanism driven by vestibular and/or proprioceptive afferent signals. In experiment 2, subjects were required, as soon as they perceived the trunk arrest, to change the hand motion to the same direction as that of the trunk. Under this instruction, subjects were able to initiate corrections only after the hand approached or reached the final position. Thus, centrally mediated compensatory corrections triggered in response to the trunk arrest were likely to occur too late to maintain the observed invariant hand trajectory in experiment 1. In experiment 3, subjects produced similar pointing movements, but to a target that moved together with the trunk. In these body-oriented pointing movements, the hand trajectories from trials in which the trunk was moving or arrested were substantially different. The same trajectories represented in a relative frame of reference moving with the trunk were virtually identical. We conclude that hand trajectory invariance can be produced in an external spatial (experiment 1) or an internal trunk-centered (experiment 3) frame of reference. The invariance in the external frame of reference is accomplished by active compensatory changes in the arm joint angles nullifying the influence of the trunk motion on the hand trajectory. We suggest that to make a transition to the internal frame of reference, control systems suppress this compensation. One of the hypotheses opened to further experimental testing is that the integration of additional (trunk) degrees of freedom into movement is based on afferent (proprioceptive, vestibular) signals stemming from the trunk motion and transmitted to the arm muscles.


Assuntos
Abdome/inervação , Variação Genética/fisiologia , Mãos/inervação , Movimento/fisiologia , Orientação/fisiologia , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Tórax/inervação , Abdome/fisiologia , Adulto , Fenômenos Biomecânicos , Feminino , Mãos/fisiologia , Força da Mão/fisiologia , Humanos , Cinestesia/fisiologia , Masculino , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Estimulação Luminosa , Tórax/fisiologia
12.
Exp Brain Res ; 137(3-4): 411-23, 2001 Apr.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11355386

RESUMO

It is known that proprioceptive feedback induces muscle activation when the facilitation of appropriate motoneurons exceeds their threshold. In the suprathreshold range, the muscle-reflex system produces torques depending on the position and velocity of the joint segment(s) that the muscle spans. The static component of the torque-position relationship is referred to as the invariant characteristic (IC). According to the equilibrium-point (EP) hypothesis, control systems produce movements by changing the activation thresholds and thus shifting the IC of the appropriate muscles in joint space. This control process upsets the balance between muscle and external torques at the initial limb configuration and, to regain the balance, the limb is forced to establish a new configuration or, if the movement is prevented, a new level of static torques. Taken together, the joint angles and the muscle torques generated at an equilibrium configuration define a single variable called the EP. Thus by shifting the IC, control systems reset the EP. Muscle activation and movement emerge following the EP resetting because of the natural physical tendency of the system to reach equilibrium. Empirical and simulation studies support the notion that the control IC shifts and the resulting EP shifts underlying fast point-to-point arm movements are gradual rather than step-like. However, controversies exist about the duration of these shifts. Some studies suggest that the IC shifts cease with the movement offset. Other studies propose that the IC shifts end early in comparison to the movement duration (approximately, at peak velocity). The purpose of this study was to evaluate the duration of the IC shifts underlying fast point-to-point arm movements. Subjects made fast (hand peak velocity about 1.3 m/s) planar arm movements toward different targets while grasping a handle. Hand forces applied to the handle and shoulder/elbow torques were, respectively, measured from a force sensor placed on the handle, or computed with equations of motion. In some trials, an electromagnetic brake prevented movements. In such movements, the hand force and joint torques reached a steady state after a time that was much smaller than the movement duration in unobstructed movements and was approximately equal to the time to peak velocity (mean difference < 80 ms). In an additional experiment, subjects were instructed to rapidly initiate corrections of the pushing force in response to movement arrest. They were able to initiate such corrections only when the joint torques and the pushing force had practically reached a steady state. The latency of correction onset was, however, smaller than the duration of unobstructed movements. We concluded that during the time at which the steady state torques were reached, the control pattern of IC shifts remained the same despite the movement block. Thereby the duration of these shifts did not exceed the time of reaching the steady state torques. Our findings are consistent with the hypothesis that, in unobstructed movements, the IC shifts and resulting shifts in the EP end approximately at peak velocity. In other words, during the latter part of the movement, the control signals responsible for the equilibrium shift remained constant, and the movement was driven by the arm inertial, viscous and elastic forces produced by the muscle-reflex system. Fast movements may thus be completed without continuous control guidance. As a consequence, central corrections and sequential commands may be issued rapidly, without waiting for the end of kinematic responses to each command, which may be important for many motor behaviours including typing, piano playing and speech. Our study also illustrates that the timing of the control signals may be substantially different from that of the resulting motor output and that the same control pattern may produce different motor outputs depending on external conditions.


Assuntos
Braço/fisiologia , Movimento/fisiologia , Equilíbrio Postural/fisiologia , Propriocepção/fisiologia , Adulto , Algoritmos , Braço/inervação , Retroalimentação/fisiologia , Humanos , Contração Isométrica/fisiologia , Contração Isotônica/fisiologia , Modelos Neurológicos , Músculo Esquelético/inervação , Músculo Esquelético/fisiologia
13.
Exp Brain Res ; 133(3): 279-92, 2000 Aug.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10958518

RESUMO

The role of the basal ganglia in the coordination of different body segments and utilization of motor synergies was investigated by analyzing reaching movements to remembered three-dimensional (3D) targets in patients with Parkinson's disease (PD). Arm movements were produced alone or in combination with a forward bending of the trunk, with or without visual feedback. Movements in PD patients were more temporally segmented, as evidenced by irregular changes in tangential velocity profiles. In addition, the relative timing in the onsets and offsets of fingertip and trunk motions were substantially different in PD patients than in control subjects. While the control subjects synchronized both onsets and offsets, the PD patients had large mean intervals between the onsets and offsets of the fingertip and trunk motions. Moreover, PD patients showed substantially larger trial-to-trial variability in these intervals. The degree of synchronization in PD patients gradually increased during the movement under the influence of visual feedback. The mean and variability of the intersegmental intervals decreased as the fingertip approached the target. This improvement in timing occurred even though the separate variability in the timing of arm and trunk motions was not reduced by vision. In combined movements, even without vision, the PD patients were able to achieve normal accuracy, suggesting they were able to use the same movement synergies as normals to control the multiple degrees of freedom involved in the movements and to compensate for the added trunk movement. However, they were unable to recruit these synergies in the stereotyped manner characteristic of healthy subjects. These results suggest that the basal ganglia are involved in the temporal coordination of movement of different body segments and that related timing abnormalities may be partly compensated by vision. Abnormal intersegmental timing may be a highly sensitive indicator of a deficient ability to assemble complex movements in patients with basal-ganglia dysfunction. This abnormality may be apparent even when the overall movement goal of reaching a target is preserved and normal movement synergies appear to be largely intact.


Assuntos
Ataxia/fisiopatologia , Dedos/fisiologia , Movimento/fisiologia , Doença de Parkinson/fisiopatologia , Postura/fisiologia , Visão Ocular/fisiologia , Idoso , Análise de Variância , Braço/fisiologia , Fenômenos Biomecânicos , Estudos de Casos e Controles , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Desempenho Psicomotor
14.
Exp Brain Res ; 131(3): 336-49, 2000 Apr.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10789948

RESUMO

Previous studies addressing the problem of the control of multiple degrees of freedom have examined the influence of trunk movement on pointing movements within the arm's reach. Such movements may be controlled by two functionally independent units of coordination (synergies): one involving only arm joints and producing the hand trajectory to the target (the transport synergy), and the other coordinating trunk and arm movements leaving the hand trajectory unchanged (the compensatory synergy). The question of whether or not this functional subdivision depends on visual feedback was addressed in the present study. We also tested whether or not the motor effects of different synergies are summated as independent components, a control strategy called "superposition." Finally, we investigated whether or not the relationship between different degrees of freedom within each synergy could be considered linear resulting in proportional changes in different joint angles. Seated subjects produced fast, uncorrected arm movements to an ipsi- or a contralateral target in the direction of +/-45 degrees to the sagittal midline of the trunk. Targets could be reached using the arm alone (control trials) or by combining the arm motion with a forward or backward trunk motion produced by hip flexion or extension (test trials), with and without visual feedback. The shape of the hand trajectory, its direction and tangential velocity, movement precision, joint angles and the sequence of the trunk and hand recruitment and de-recruitment were measured. In both visual conditions, the direction of the hand trajectory observed in control trials was generally preserved in test trials. In terms of sequencing, even in the absence of vision, the trunk movement was initiated before the onset of and outlasted the hand shift, indicating that the potential influence of the trunk on the hand movement was compensated by rotations in the elbow and shoulder joint. The analysis of other variables also implied that the effects of trunk recruitment on the hand trajectory were minor compared to those which could be observed if these effects were not compensated by appropriate changes in the arm joint angles. It was concluded that an arm-trunk compensatory synergy is present in pointing movements regardless of visual feedback. Principal component analysis showed that the relationship between elbow, shoulder and hip joint angles in individual arm and combined arm-trunk movements cannot be considered linear, implying that this relationship is adjusted according to the changing arm geometry. The changes in each arm joint angle (elbow, shoulder) elicited by a forward trunk bending in one block of trials were compared with those elicited by a backward bending in another block, whereas the hand moved to the same target in both blocks. These changes were opposite but of similar magnitude. As a result, for each moment of movement, the mean joint angle obtained by averaging across two directions of trunk motion was practically identical to that in control trials in which the trunk was motionless. It is concluded that the transport and arm-trunk compensatory synergies are combined as independent units, according to the principle of superposition. This principle may simplify the control of the coordination of a redundant number of degrees of freedom.


Assuntos
Braço/inervação , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Percepção Visual , Adulto , Retroalimentação , Lateralidade Funcional , Humanos , Articulações/fisiologia , Atividade Motora/fisiologia , Reflexo Vestíbulo-Ocular
15.
Neurosci Lett ; 283(1): 65-8, 2000 Mar 31.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10729635

RESUMO

It is suggested that the nervous system may specify a referent configuration (R) of the body determined by the set of the threshold joint angles at which all skeletal muscles may be silent. At the same time, electromyographic (EMG) activity and forces are generated to resist deflections of the body from this configuration. The R configuration may thus be considered an internal geometric image with which the actual body configuration (Q) is compared. Thereby the difference between the R and Q is a major factor determining the recruitment and gradation of the activity of each skeletal muscle. Control systems may produce movements by changing the R configuration according to task demands. The referent hypothesis predicts that when the R and Q configurations match each other, a global minimum in the EMG activity of all muscles involved should occur, an event most likely observed in movements with reversal in direction. To test the validity of the R hypothesis for head movements, three-dimensional kinematics and EMG activity of 14 functionally diverse neck muscles were analysed in monkeys during head rotations to and from fruit targets placed beyond the oculomotor range. Despite the functional and anatomical diversity of the neck muscles, the activity of all muscles was minimised at a reversal point of the movement trajectory, as predicted by the R hypothesis. This study thus illustrates the notion that a change in the internal geometric image of a biomechanical system may underlie movement production.


Assuntos
Movimentos da Cabeça/fisiologia , Músculos do Pescoço/anatomia & histologia , Músculos do Pescoço/fisiologia , Animais , Eletromiografia , Haplorrinos , Modelos Neurológicos
16.
Ann Chir ; 53(8): 742-50, 1999.
Artigo em Francês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10584386

RESUMO

INTRODUCTION: The lateral bending test is used for the preoperative evaluation of scoliotic patients in order to determine the type of spinal curvatures as well as to assess spine flexibility and possible corrections. However, very few biomechanical studies have been dedicated to the analysis of lateral bending. In this article, a biomechanical model of the human trunk has been used in order to evaluate the possibility of simulating lateral bending tests. METHODS: This model includes elements representing the osseo-ligamentous structures of the spine, rib cage and pelvis, as well as 160 muscle fascicles represented by bilinear cable elements. For 4 scoliotic patients (right thoracic and left lumbar curvatures), 3D upright standing and bending reconstructions were generated from calibrated x-rays and used to calculate the displacements of the vertebrae T1 and L5. These displacements were applied to the model in standing position in order to simulate lateral bending. The resulting geometry of the deformed model was compared to the reconstructed geometry in lateral bending for the other vertebral levels (T2 to L4). RESULTS: The model allows the reproduction of the thoracic Cobb angle modifications with an accuracy superior to 2 degrees, as well as the vertebral rotations in the frontal plane (agreement greater than 85%). The positions of the vertebral body centroids following the simulations showed an agreement of over 77% with reconstructed positions. The direction of the axial angulation for the thoracic and lumbar apical vertebrae is correctly reproduced by the model. The axial rotation for these vertebrae does not result in a common pattern for the 4 patients, which is consistent with the diversity of published data concerning the direction of this coupling. CONCLUSIONS: This study shows the feasibility of simulating lateral bending tests using a 3D biomechanical model integrating muscles. The effect of muscle forces on trunk stiffness and intersegmental mobility can also be assessed using this approach. Future developments should enable the evaluation of the biomechanical properties of scoliotic deformities, thus providing a useful tool for preoperative surgical planning.


Assuntos
Simulação por Computador , Análise de Elementos Finitos , Intensificação de Imagem Radiográfica/métodos , Amplitude de Movimento Articular , Escoliose/diagnóstico por imagem , Escoliose/fisiopatologia , Fenômenos Biomecânicos , Estudos de Viabilidade , Humanos , Cuidados Pré-Operatórios , Reprodutibilidade dos Testes , Rotação , Escoliose/classificação , Escoliose/cirurgia
17.
Exp Brain Res ; 126(1): 55-67, 1999 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10333007

RESUMO

Previous studies have shown that in neurologically normal subjects the addition of trunk motion during a reaching task does not affect the trajectory of the arm endpoint. Typically, the trunk begins to move before the onset and continues to move after the offset of the arm endpoint displacement. This observation shows that the potential contribution of the trunk to the motion of the arm endpoint toward a target is neutralized by appropriate compensatory movements of the shoulder and elbow. We tested the hypothesis that cortical and subcortical brain lesions may disrupt the timing of trunk and arm endpoint motion in hemiparetic subjects. Eight hemiparetic and six age-matched healthy subjects were seated on a stool with the right (dominant) arm in front of them on a table. The tip of the index finger (the arm endpoint) was initially at a distance of 20 cm from the midline of the chest. Wrist, elbow, and upper body positions as well as the coordinates of the arm endpoint were recorded with a three-dimensional motion analysis system (Optotrak) by infrared light-emitting diodes placed on the tip of the finger, the styloid process of the ulna, the lateral epicondyle of the humerus, the acromion processes bilaterally, and the sternal notch. In response to a preparatory signal, subjects lifted their arm 1-2 cm above the table and in response to a "go" signal moved their endpoint as fast as possible from a near to a far target located at a distance of 35 cm and at a 45 degrees angle to the right or left of the sagittal midline of the trunk. After a pause (200-500 ms) they moved the endpoint back to the near target. Pointing movements were made without trunk motion (control trials) or with a sagittal motion of the trunk produced by means of a hip flexion or extension (test trials). In one set of test trials, subjects were required to move the trunk forward while moving the arm to the target ("in-phase movements"). In the other set, subjects were required to move the trunk backward when the arm moved to the far target ("out-of-phase movements"). Compared with healthy subjects, movements in hemiparetic subjects were segmented, slower, and characterized by a greater variability and by deflection of the trajectory from a straight line. In addition, there was a moderate increase in the errors in movement direction and extent. These deficits were similar in magnitude whether or not the trunk was involved. Although hemiparetic subjects were able to compensate the influence of the trunk motion on the movement of the arm endpoint, they accomplished this by making more segmented movements than healthy subjects. In addition, they were unable to stabilize the sequence of trunk and arm endpoint movements in a set of trials. It is concluded that recruitment and sequencing of different degrees of freedom may be impaired in this population of patients. This inability may partly be responsible for other deficits observed in hemiparetic subjects, including an increase in movement segmentation and duration. The lack of stereotypic movement sequencing may imply that these subjects had deficits in learning associated with short-term memory.


Assuntos
Hemiplegia/fisiopatologia , Desempenho Psicomotor , Adulto , Idoso , Braço/inervação , Transtornos Cerebrovasculares/fisiopatologia , Articulação do Cotovelo , Feminino , Dedos/inervação , Lateralidade Funcional , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Atividade Motora , Movimento , Valores de Referência , Articulação do Punho
18.
J Electromyogr Kinesiol ; 8(6): 383-90, 1998 Dec.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9840893

RESUMO

It has been suggested that the coordination of the activity of multiple muscles results from the comparison of the actual configuration of the body with a referent configuration specified by the nervous system so that the recruitment and gradation of the activity of each skeletal muscle depend on the difference between these two configurations. Active movements may be produced by the modification of the referent configuration. The hypothesis predicts the existence of a global minimum in electromyographic (EMG) activity of multiple muscles during movements involving reversals in direction. This prediction was tested in five subjects by analysing movements resembling the act of reaching for an object placed beyond one's reach from a sitting position. In such movements, initially sitting subjects raise their body to a semi-standing position and then return to sitting. Consistent with the hypothesis is the observation of a global minimum in the surface EMG activity of 16 muscles of the arm, trunk and leg at a specific phase of the movement. When the minimum occurred, EMG activity of each muscle did not exceed 2-7% of its maximal activity during the movement. As predicted, global EMG minima occurred at the phase corresponding to the reversal in movement direction, that is, during the transition from raising to lowering of the body. The global EMG minimum may represent the point at which temporal matching occurs between the actual and the referent body configurations. This study implies a specific link between motor behavior and the geometric shape of the body modified by the brain according to the desired action.


Assuntos
Movimento/fisiologia , Músculo Esquelético/fisiologia , Músculos Abdominais/fisiologia , Adulto , Braço/fisiologia , Eletromiografia , Eletrofisiologia , Feminino , Humanos , Cooperação Internacional , Perna (Membro)/fisiologia , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Sociedades Médicas
19.
Brain Res ; 802(1-2): 274-80, 1998 Aug 17.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9748626

RESUMO

This study examined the influence of trunk recruitment on the kinematic characteristics of pointing movements. The distribution of final positions of the hand, the extent and direction of the hand trajectory was basically preserved when trunk movement was combined with arm pointing. These effects were observed during pointing not only with but also without vision. The results imply that two functionally independent units of coordination are used in pointing regardless of visual feedback-one producing arm movement to the target (the reaching synergy) and the other coordinating trunk and arm movements diminishing the influence of the trunk on the arm endpoint trajectory (the compensatory synergy).


Assuntos
Braço/fisiologia , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Tórax/fisiologia , Visão Ocular/fisiologia , Adaptação Fisiológica/fisiologia , Retroalimentação , Humanos , Recrutamento Neurofisiológico/fisiologia
20.
Motor Control ; 2(3): 189-205, 1998 Jul.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9644289

RESUMO

The lambda model of the equilibrium-point hypothesis (Feldman & Levin, 1995) is an approach to motor control which, like physics, is based on a logical system coordinating empirical data. The model has gone through an interesting period. On one hand, several nontrivial predictions of the model have been successfully verified in recent studies. In addition, the explanatory and predictive capacity of the model has been enhanced by its extension to multimuscle and multijoint systems. On the other hand, claims have recently appeared suggesting that the model should be abandoned. The present paper focuses on these claims and concludes that they are unfounded. Much of the experimental data that have been used to reject the model are actually consistent with it.


Assuntos
Movimento/fisiologia , Equilíbrio Postural , Eletromiografia/métodos , Humanos , Músculo Esquelético/inervação
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA
...