Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 57
Filtrar
1.
Br J Ophthalmol ; 89(11): 1438-41, 2005 Nov.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16234448

RESUMO

AIMS: To establish the range of normal distance stereoacuity in young children using the Frisby Davis distance stereo test (FD2). METHODS: Children passing preschool vision screening assessments underwent measurement of distance stereoacuity with the FD2 using a standard testing protocol. RESULTS: 59 visually normal children aged between 36 months and 68 months were recruited to this study. All 59 were able to understand the test requirements and were examined with the FD2 stereo test. Four (6.8%) had no measurable stereoacuity; 13 (24%) had stereoacuity measurable only at a 3 metre testing distance (mean 92.3 seconds of arc; SD 52.6). These children were significantly younger than the remaining 42 (76%) who demonstrated a stereoacuity response at a 6 metre testing distance (mean 29.6 seconds of arc; SD 13.1, p=0.008). CONCLUSION: The FD2 stereo test enables the measurement of distance stereoacuity in young children. There appears to be a maturational effect with distance stereoacuity improving between 36 months and 68 months. The data on age related normal values will provide a baseline from which to compare outcomes in clinical populations.


Assuntos
Percepção de Profundidade/fisiologia , Acuidade Visual/fisiologia , Envelhecimento/fisiologia , Envelhecimento/psicologia , Pré-Escolar , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Valores de Referência , Disparidade Visual/fisiologia , Testes Visuais/métodos , Visão Binocular/fisiologia
2.
Surg Endosc ; 17(5): 787-90, 2003 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12582759

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: Pilots undergo many visual tests for both selection and assessment, and we know that there are many similarities between pilots and surgeons. Hence, it would not be unreasonable to bring similar visual tests into surgery. Tonic accommodation (TA) is a stable parameter that is adopted by the eye in the absence of any stimulation. Over recent years, surgery has undergone change from traditional open surgery to minimally invasive procedures, bringing many advantages. However, not every surgeon has the ability to perform under conditions where the operative field is represented on a flat monitor. METHOD: We determined the TA values in medical students and then correlated this with their performance on a virtual reality surgical simulator. RESULTS: We found that TA values predicted the number of errors made with the dominant hand, accounting for 27% of the variance. CONCLUSION: The data suggest that TA may play a role in the individual differences that are noted when surgeons perform laparoscopic surgery. Further studies are needed to evaluate the exact role of TA in surgical performance.


Assuntos
Acomodação Ocular/fisiologia , Competência Clínica/normas , Procedimentos Cirúrgicos Minimamente Invasivos/educação , Procedimentos Cirúrgicos Minimamente Invasivos/normas , Adulto , Simulação por Computador/normas , Instrução por Computador/métodos , Avaliação Educacional/métodos , Tecnologia Educacional/instrumentação , Lateralidade Funcional , Humanos , Projetos Piloto , Valor Preditivo dos Testes , Desempenho Psicomotor , Estudantes de Medicina
3.
Med Biol Eng Comput ; 40(2): 260-8, 2002 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12043810

RESUMO

The aim of the study was to investigate the effect of signal length on the performance of a signal source separation method, independent component analysis (ICA), when extracting the visual evoked potential (EP) lambda wave from saccade-related electro-encephalogram (EEG) waveforms. A method was devised that enabled the effective length of the recorded EEG traces to be increased prior to processing by ICA. This involved abutting EEG traces from an appropriate number of successive trials (a trial was a set of waveforms recorded from 64 electrode locations in a study investigating saccade performance). ICA was applied to the saccade-related EEG and electro-oculogram (EOG) waveforms recorded from the electrode locations. One spatial and five temporal features of the lambda wave were monitored to assess the performance of ICA applied to both abutted and non-abutted waveforms. ICA applied to abutted trials managed to extract all six features across all seven subjects included in the study. This was not the case when ICA was applied to the non-abutted trials. It was quantitatively demonstrated that the process of abutting EEG waveforms was useful for ICA preprocessing when extracting lambda waves.


Assuntos
Algoritmos , Potenciais Evocados Visuais , Movimentos Sacádicos , Processamento de Sinais Assistido por Computador , Adulto , Ensaios Clínicos como Assunto , Eletroencefalografia , Eletroculografia , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino
5.
Vision Res ; 40(24): 3373-90, 2000.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11058735

RESUMO

We used the sequential stereopsis paradigm and apparatus described by Enright (Vision Research, 36, (1996) 307-312). The observer's task was to set targets to equidistance in Experiments 1-3, and to make them co-planar in Experiment 4. However, it is argued that in all experiments observers exploited a co-planarity setting strategy. Sequential stereopsis produced good performance throughout in terms of low disparity thresholds when head position was varied by rotations around three axes: vertical (azimuth condition); horizontal (elevation); and midline (tilt). It also produced good performance when the targets were shifted in position so that they both lay on one side of the median plane of the head. These results cannot be accounted for by Enright's isovergence hypothesis unless it is extended to incorporate other information about eye positions. Performance was better but not greatly so in control simultaneous stereopsis conditions, nor did it deteriorate much when the observer's view was restricted solely to the targets by removing visibility of the room in which the apparatus was located. Target settings were typically located on a concave arc centred on the median plane. This effect was quantitatively modelled using disparity correction for a relief task of co-planarity (Gârding, Porrill, Mayhew, & Frisby. Vision Research, 35 (1995) 703-722). This modelling indicated over-estimations of c.10-20 cm in fixation distance for target distances in the range 71.5-112.5 cm.


Assuntos
Percepção de Profundidade/fisiologia , Disparidade Visual/fisiologia , Adolescente , Adulto , Movimentos da Cabeça/fisiologia , Humanos , Psicofísica , Visão Binocular/fisiologia
6.
Vision Res ; 39(5): 993-1009, 1999 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10341951

RESUMO

Several categories of torsional eye movements obey Listing's law; however, systematic deviations from this law occur during vergence. Two kinematic models attempt to incorporate these deviations, both of which are supported by experimental evidence; however, they lead to different torsion predictions. These discrepancies have been explained in terms of experimental procedures, but it now seems likely from several recent studies that individual differences in torsion patterns may also be important. This study therefore examines the variation of torsion during a smooth asymmetric vergence task in which a fixation target was moved along the line-of-sight of the right eye at 15 degrees elevation; each of five subjects observed five trials of both inward and outward target motion, repeated in two sessions several weeks apart. There were no significant group differences in left or right eye torsion between trials or sessions, suggesting that monocular torsion patterns were relatively stable over time. When examined more closely, however, the torsion patterns shown by some individuals did vary for inward versus outward target motion. Hence, monocular torsion was idiosyncratic and depended on the direction in which fixation was changing (convergence or divergence). In a binocular analysis, cycloversion varied dramatically between subjects and depended on the direction of target motion; however, this was not the case for cyclovergence. In summary, cyclovergence is relatively stable and depends on where the eyes are looking, whereas cycloversion (and hence monocular torsion) is relatively unstable and depends on how they came to be in that particular horizontal and vertical orientation. These findings help to explain the controversy surrounding the torsional behaviour of the human eye during vergence.


Assuntos
Convergência Ocular/fisiologia , Movimentos Oculares/fisiologia , Adulto , Fixação Ocular/fisiologia , Humanos , Masculino , Percepção de Movimento/fisiologia , Visão Binocular/fisiologia , Visão Monocular/fisiologia
7.
Vision Res ; 39(3): 481-92, 1999 Feb.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10341978

RESUMO

Gårding et al. (Vis Res 1995;35:703-722) proposed a two-stage theory of stereopsis. The first uses horizontal disparities for relief computations after they have been subjected to a process called disparity correction that utilises vertical disparities. The second stage, termed disparity normalisation, is concerned with computing metric representations from the output of stage one. It uses vertical disparities to a much lesser extent, if at all, for small field stimuli. We report two psychophysical experiments that tested whether human vision implements this two-stage theory. They tested the prediction that scaling vertical disparities to simulate different viewing distances to the fixation point should affect the perceived amplitudes of vertically but not horizontally oriented ridges. The first used elliptical half-cylinders and the 'apparently circular cylinder' judgement task of Johnston (Vis Res 1991;31:1351-1360). The second experiment used parabolic ridges and the amplitude judgement task of Buckley and Frisby (Vis Res 1993;33:919-934). Both studies broadly confirmed the anisotropy prediction by finding that large scalings of vertical disparities simulating near distances had a strong effect on the perceived amplitudes of the vertically oriented stimuli but little effect on the horizontal ones. When distances > 25 cm were simulated there were no significant differential effects and various methodological reasons are offered for this departure from expectations.


Assuntos
Percepção de Profundidade/fisiologia , Adulto , Anisotropia , Feminino , Percepção de Forma/fisiologia , Humanos , Julgamento/fisiologia , Masculino , Orientação/fisiologia , Disparidade Visual/fisiologia , Visão Binocular/fisiologia
8.
Nature ; 397(6714): 63-6, 1999 Jan 07.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9892353

RESUMO

Differences between the left and right eye's views of the world carry information about three-dimensional scene structure and about the position of the eyes in the head. The contemporary Bayesian approach to perception implies that human performance in using this source of eye-position information can be analysed most usefully by comparison with the performance of a statistically optimal observer. Here we argue that the comparison observer should also be statistically robust, and we find that this requirement leads to qualitatively new behaviours. For example, when presented with a class of stereoscopic stimuli containing inconsistent information about eccentricity of gaze, estimates of this gaze parameter recorded from one robust ideal observer bifurcate at a critical value of stimulus inconsistency. We report an experiment in which human observers also show this phenomenon and we use the experimentally determined critical value to estimate the vertical acuity of the visual system. The Bayesian analysis also provides a highly reliable and biologically plausible algorithm that can recover eye positions even before the classic stereo-correspondence problem is solved, that is, before deciding which features in the left and right images are to be matched.


Assuntos
Percepção de Profundidade/fisiologia , Algoritmos , Humanos , Modelos Estatísticos , Método de Monte Carlo , Variações Dependentes do Observador
9.
Vision Res ; 39(23): 3934-50, 1999 Nov.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10748926

RESUMO

Two recently developed kinematic models of human eye movements predict systematic departures from Listing's law which are associated with changes in vergence. This vergence-dependent torsion t is proportional to elevation e and vergence v, that is t = kev/2. The proposed value for k is either 1 (Van Rijn, L. J., & Van den Berg, A. V. (1993). Vision Research, 33, 691-708) or 1/2 (Minken, A. W. H., Gielen, C. C. A. M., & Van Gisbergen, J. A. M. (1995). Vision Research, 35, 93-102). One implication of both models is that an eye with a constant fixation direction should exhibit systematic torsional variation during movements of the other eye. This paper therefore examines the torsion produced by moving a fixation target inwards and outwards along the line-of-sight of the right eye at five different viewing elevations (0, +/- 15 and +/- 30 degrees). In a monocular analysis, each eye generally showed intorsion during convergence at positive elevation angles, whereas extorsion occurred at negative elevations; the opposite was true during divergence. However, the torsion response was visibly different between the five subjects, and depended on the direction of target motion. In a binocular analysis, cycloversion (mean of left and right eye torsion) varied dramatically both between subjects and between convergence and divergence; however, cyclovergence (torsional difference) was much less variable. Least-squares methods were used to estimate the constant k from monocular torsion, yielding values between 0.2 and 1.0; however, corresponding estimates based on cyclovergence were all close to 1/2. These findings support suggestions that a binocular control system couples the three-dimensional movements of the eyes, and that an existing model of monocular torsion should be generalised to the binocular case.


Assuntos
Convergência Ocular/fisiologia , Movimentos Oculares/fisiologia , Fixação Ocular/fisiologia , Humanos , Visão Binocular/fisiologia , Visão Monocular/fisiologia
10.
Ophthalmic Physiol Opt ; 18(5): 452-62, 1998 Sep.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10023479

RESUMO

Many aspects of vision have been investigated in developmental dyslexia. Some research suggests deficits in vergence control (e.g. Buzzelli, 1991, Optom. Vision Sci. 68, 842-846), although ability to control vergence across saccades has not yet been investigated. We have explored this question indirectly using Enright's (1996 Vision Res. 36, 307-312.) sequential stereopsis task. The task requires observers to set two adjacent targets (whose textures cannot be resolved simultaneously if either is fixated) to appear equi-distant. Enright has argued that sequential stereopsis stereoacuity thresholds offer an indication of vergence control across saccades. We report two experiments using a total of 17 dyslexic and 18 control adults. Performance was measured on a sequential stereopsis task and an ordinary 'simultaneous' stereopsis task. No significant differences between groups were found. However, whereas practice of the sequential task lowered control group thresholds on the simultaneous task, for the dyslexic group it significantly raised thresholds, suggesting that visual fatigue is especially important in investigations of visual functions in dyslexia. Although the small samples used limit conclusions at this stage, the main sequential stereopsis results suggest that, if Enright is correct, dyslexic adults can show normal vergence control across saccades.


Assuntos
Convergência Ocular/fisiologia , Dislexia/fisiopatologia , Movimentos Sacádicos/fisiologia , Adulto , Percepção de Profundidade/fisiologia , Feminino , Fixação Ocular/fisiologia , Humanos , Masculino , Percepção de Movimento/fisiologia , Disparidade Visual/fisiologia
11.
Vision Res ; 37(4): 467-73, 1997 Feb.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9156177

RESUMO

Adelson has shown how two patches in a 5 by 5 array of grey patches can be perceived to consist of different shades, depending on whether they are represented at a 3-D horizontal or vertical ridge. Adelson interprets the illusion in terms of the orientation of the patches with respect to the inferred illuminant. We investigated: (1) the illusion in the vertical and horizontal stimuli and added a flat (ridgeless) control stimulus; (2) stimuli of varying ridge amplitudes to examine the effect more fully. 3-D renderings of real surfaces were modelled with computer graphics and displayed to observers who used a mouse to alter the brightness of a square to match patches indicated in the stimuli. Five observers were used for the vertical, flat and horizontal stimuli, while a larger group (n = 20) was used for an independent design when varying ridge amplitudes. A significant effect in the flat surface demonstrates that patches lying in the same plane can have their brightness altered without changes in their orientation. When the surface was seen as a 3-D ridge the size of the effect was a function of 3-D slope of the surface. By measuring each patch independently we have shown that the effect changes the brightness of the two patches to differing degrees. We offer an explanation of this based on a proposed qualitative shading rule for identifying reflectance and illumination edges.


Assuntos
Sensibilidades de Contraste/fisiologia , Percepção de Profundidade/fisiologia , Ilusões Ópticas/fisiologia , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino
12.
Vision Res ; 37(22): 3109-16, 1997 Nov.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9463693

RESUMO

Enright [(1995). Perception, 24 (suppl.), 32-33; (1996). Vision Research, 36, 307-312] described a simple piece of equipment for demonstrating a perceptual mechanism he called sequential stereopsis. The equipment requires an observer to set two textured targets seen behind a pair of small viewing ports to appear equi-distant. The principle upon which the apparatus depends is the use of textures whose elements cannot be resolved in peripheral vision at the eccentricity determined by the target separation. Enright used a fine sandpaper for this purpose. We have conducted two similar experiments using high bandpass filtered textures which eliminate any possibility that the low spatial frequency content of sandpaper textures could play a role. Our results corroborate Enright's general conclusions on sequential stereopsis, while at the same time showing that high-pass textures do not give wholly similar results to sandpaper.


Assuntos
Percepção de Profundidade , Psicofísica/instrumentação , Adulto , Seguimentos , Humanos , Estimulação Luminosa/métodos , Limiar Sensorial , Disparidade Visual
13.
Vision Res ; 36(8): 1163-76, 1996 Apr.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8762720

RESUMO

Texture information about surface shape can be decomposed into three constituents: compression, density and scaling. Blake, Bülthoff and Sheinberg's (1993, Vision Research, 33, 1723-1737) Ideal Observer theory of slant from texture predicts that the relative contribution of compression and density to the percept of planar surface slant should vary with field of view (FOV). The contribution of compression and density should both increase as FOV increases but statistical analysis shows that the reliability, and hence the expected contribution, of density increases at a greater rate than compression with increasing FOV. Specific predictions are that at FOV < 20 deg compression should be more effective than density, at FOV approximately 20 deg, compression and density should be equally effective, and at FOV > 20 deg, compression should be less effective than density. These predictions were tested by pitting these components of texture against one another. The method used was similar to that described in Frisby and Buckley [1992 In Orban, G. & Nagel H.-H. (Eds) Artificial and biological visual systems. Berlin: Springer]: observers judged binocularly the slant of a large table on to which was projected a texture created using computer graphics to contain various texture component cues to slant. The size of the projected texture patch determined the FOV which was by this means set to either 10, 20 or 30 deg. It was found that compression was the dominant cue irrespective of FOV, even if it was perturbed by noise. Possible reasons for this divergence of human observers from predictions of the Ideal Observer theory of slant from texture are discussed.


Assuntos
Percepção de Profundidade/fisiologia , Percepção de Forma/fisiologia , Adolescente , Adulto , Sinais (Psicologia) , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Campos Visuais
14.
Eye (Lond) ; 10 ( Pt 2): 286-90, 1996.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8776462

RESUMO

We describe a modified version of the Frisby Stereopsis Screening Test which enables a light to be flashed behind the stereo target when the child being tested makes an appropriate pointing or reaching response. The light can be flashed to gain the interest of the child during a training phase in which they are familiarised with the test and its requirements. This phase is then followed by a test phase in which the child is encouraged to demonstrate unaided clear pointing responses to the target to gain a light flash while the plate is held in two or three different positions. This device has proved effective in increasing the chances of administering the test successfully to a sample of 30 very young children (age range 7-23 months).


Assuntos
Percepção de Profundidade , Transtornos da Percepção/prevenção & controle , Seleção Visual/métodos , Feminino , Humanos , Lactente , Masculino , Educação de Pacientes como Assunto , Estimulação Luminosa , Transtornos da Visão/prevenção & controle
15.
Perception ; 25(2): 129-54, 1996.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8733143

RESUMO

Six experiments are described in which good performance of the task of matching the lengths of two stationary real objects, gnarled wooden sticks, under a variety of binocular viewing conditions, including variations in viewing distances was demonstrated. Relatively poor matching performance was observed when the sticks were viewed monocularly in oscillatory motion, or monocularly and stationary. The results suggest that stereo can support good representations of metric scene structure when length judgments of natural objects are required under (quasi-)natural viewing. The implications of these results for theories of structure from stereo and structure from motion are discussed.


Assuntos
Percepção de Profundidade , Percepção de Forma , Percepção de Tamanho , Adolescente , Adulto , Aprendizagem por Discriminação , Percepção de Distância , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Percepção de Movimento , Psicofísica , Visão Binocular , Visão Monocular
16.
Perception ; 25(2): 165-76, 1996.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8733145

RESUMO

Two experiments are described in which the effects of scaling vertical disparities on the perceived amplitudes of dome-shaped surfaces depicted with horizontal disparities were examined. The Mayhew and Longuet-Higgins's theory and the regional-disparity-correction theory of Garding et al predict that scaling should generate a change in perceived depth appropriate to the viewing distance simulated by the scaled vertical disparities. Significant depth changes were observed, by means of a nulling task in which the vertical-disparity-scaling effect was cancelled by the observer choosing a pattern of horizontal disparities that made the dome-shaped surface appear flat. The sizes of the scaling effects were less than those predicted by either theory, suggesting that other cues to fixation distance such as oculomotor information played an appreciable role. In conditions in which 50% of the texture elements were given one value of vertical-disparity scaling and the remaining 50% were left unscaled, the size of the scaling effect on perceived depth could be accounted for by equally weighted pooling of the vertical-disparity information unless the two scalings were very dissimilar, in which case the lower scaling factor tended to dominate. These findings are discussed in terms of a Hough parameter estimation model of the vertical-disparity-pooling process.


Assuntos
Atenção , Percepção de Profundidade , Orientação , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos , Disparidade Visual , Aprendizagem por Discriminação , Humanos , Ilusões Ópticas , Psicofísica
17.
Vision Res ; 35(10): 1463-72, 1995 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-7645275

RESUMO

A computational method for calibrating stereo using shape-from-texture is described together with five experiments that tested whether the human visual system implements the method. The experiments all tested the prediction that the perceived size of a step between two planar and slanted real surfaces should be affected by texture slant cues projected on to them that are inconsistent with the disparity cues. The predicted effect was observed but the results could be accounted for by a new phenomenon revealed in control conditions: the perceived size of a step between two slanted planes is in part determined by the size of the slants even when texture and stereo cues are held consistent. We conclude that the hypothesis that human stereo is calibrated by texture is not confirmed.


Assuntos
Percepção de Profundidade/fisiologia , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos/fisiologia , Visão Binocular/fisiologia , Adolescente , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Matemática , Modelos Psicológicos
18.
Vision Res ; 35(5): 703-22, 1995 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-7900308

RESUMO

The pattern of retinal binocular disparities acquired by a fixating visual system depends on both the depth structure of the scene and the viewing geometry. This paper treats the problem of interpreting the disparity pattern in terms of scene structure without relying on estimates of fixation position from eye movement control and proprioception mechanisms. We propose a sequential decomposition of this interpretation process into disparity correction, which is used to compute three-dimensional structure up to a relief transformation, and disparity normalization, which is used to resolve the relief ambiguity to obtain metric structure. We point out that the disparity normalization stage can often be omitted, since relief transformations preserve important properties such as depth ordering and coplanarity. Based on this framework we analyse three previously proposed computational models of disparity processing; the Mayhew and Longuet-Higgins model, the deformation model and the polar angle disparity model. We show how these models are related, and argue that none of them can account satisfactorily for available psychophysical data. We therefore propose an alternative model, regional disparity correction. Using this model we derive predictions for a number of experiments based on vertical disparity manipulations, and compare them to available experimental data. The paper is concluded with a summary and a discussion of the possible architectures and mechanisms underling stereopsis in the human visual system.


Assuntos
Modelos Neurológicos , Disparidade Visual/fisiologia , Convergência Ocular , Percepção de Profundidade/fisiologia , Fixação Ocular , Humanos , Rotação
19.
Perception ; 24(2): 181-98, 1995.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-7617424

RESUMO

Three experiments are reported in which the possible role of blur cues as a factor needing to be taken into account in cue-integration studies involving conflicts between stereo and texture/outline cues was investigated. The earlier suggestion was tested that uncontrolled blur cues might have caused the quite different patterns of cue integration reported for real ridge-shaped objects oriented vertically and for stereograms depicting similar surfaces. Blur cues were manipulated by pinhole viewing intended to render accommodation open loop. The results for real ridges were as predicted by the blur-cue hypothesis: pinhole viewing strengthened texture/outline cues in vertically oriented ridges, thereby diminishing the pattern of stereo dominance hitherto observed for these stimuli (and as observed here in non-pinhole-viewing control conditions and in horizontally oriented ridges). The results for the stereograms did not conform to predictions: pinhole viewing, assumed to remove blur cues from the cue-integration process, still produced the pattern observed in control conditions in which a texture/outline cue for a shallow ridge overwhelmed stereo cues for a steep ridge. This result is against the hypothesis that perhaps blur cues for the stereogram projection surface differentially favoured the shallow texture/outline cues. A new variant of the blur-cue hypothesis is offered to account for this result. The main conclusion from the study is: beware drawing firm conclusions from stereograms about the pattern of cue integration that can be expected when real objects are being viewed. The two situations can produce very different results as far as cue integration is concerned. This is a conclusion with serious implications for the use of stereograms for studying the integration of stereo with other cues.


Assuntos
Sensibilidades de Contraste , Sinais (Psicologia) , Disparidade Visual/fisiologia , Adolescente , Adulto , Análise de Variância , Aprendizagem por Discriminação , Movimentos Oculares , Óculos , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino
20.
Perception ; 23(8): 869-81, 1994.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-7870564

RESUMO

It is demonstrated that lightness perception can be affected by shape from stereopsis. The starting point was a report by Knill and Kersten that the perceived lightness of a monocularly viewed surface can be affected by outline-contour cues indicating that the surface is three-dimensional (3-D). In that study stimuli consisted of two equally sized abutting regions each having the same vertical linear-intensity ramp, so that the horizontal abutting boundary of the two patches created a sharp change in intensity. When this version of the Craik-O'Brien-Cornsweet stimulus has a rectangular outline, it exhibits the standard simultaneous contrast illusion: equivalent patches in the top and bottom regions appear to have different brightness despite having the same luminance. Knill and Kersten replicated this phenomenon with stimuli whose outline-contour cues were consistent with a flat (planar) surface. They found, however, that the illusion was greatly reduced in stimuli with outlines consistent with two abutting 3-D quarter cylinders, for which equivalent regions in the two halves appeared of similar lightness. Knill and Kersten interpreted this effect in terms of surface-lightness computations that took into account 3-D surface shape to achieve an integrated interpretation of the luminance and shape data. In the present report three experiments are described for which these earlier findings were taken as the starting point. In the first experiment the results were replicated by the use of a different methodology. In the second experiment it was shown that shape-from-stereo can produce similar effects on lightness perception to that caused by shape-from-contour. Real 3-D objects with curved surfaces, luminance profiles of the Knill and Kersten type, and carefully controlled outline-contour cues were used so that the objects appeared flat when viewed monocularly but curved in 3-D when seen binocularly. The third experiment was a control confirming that the stereo effect was not simply due to differences caused by monocular versus binocular viewing. It is concluded that the human visual system uses stereo cues, as well as outline-contour cues, in the interpretation of luminance data to recover surface lightness.


Assuntos
Percepção de Profundidade , Percepção de Forma , Luz , Adulto , Humanos , Visão Binocular , Visão Monocular
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA
...