Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 44
Filtrar
Mais filtros










Base de dados
Intervalo de ano de publicação
1.
Anim Cogn ; 25(4): 783-792, 2022 Aug.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34997367

RESUMO

Mirror self-recognition (MSR), widely regarded as an indicator of self-awareness, has not been demonstrated consistently in gorillas. We aimed to examine this issue by setting out a method to evaluate gorilla self-recognition studies that is objective, quantifiable, and easy to replicate. Using Suarez and Gallup's (J Hum Evol 10:175-183, 1981) study as a reference point, we drew up a list of 15 methodological criteria and assigned scores to all published studies of gorilla MSR for both methodology and outcomes. Key features of studies finding both mark-directed and spontaneous self-directed responses included visually inaccessible marks, controls for tactile and olfactory cues, subjects who were at least 5 years old, and clearly distinguishing between responses in front of versus away from the mirror. Additional important criteria include videotaping the tests, having more than one subject, subjects with adequate social rearing, reporting post-marking observations with mirror absent, and giving mirror exposure in a social versus individual setting. Our prediction that MSR studies would obtain progressively higher scores as procedures and behavioural coding practices improved over time was supported for methods, but not for outcomes. These findings illustrate that methodological rigour does not guarantee stronger evidence of self-recognition in gorillas; methodological differences alone do not explain the inconsistent evidence for MSR in gorillas. By implication, it might be suggested that, in general, gorillas do not show compelling evidence of MSR. We advocate that future MSR studies incorporate the same criteria to optimize the quality of attempts to clarify the self-recognition abilities of gorillas as well as other species.


Assuntos
Comportamento Animal , Gorilla gorilla , Animais , Humanos
2.
Anim Cogn ; 25(1): 1-4, 2022 Feb.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34313872

RESUMO

The recent article by Baragli, Scopa, Maglieri, and Palagi (Anim Cogn https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-021-01502-7 , 2021) that claims to demonstrate mirror self-recognition (MSR) in horses is not based on compelling evidence. We identify problems with their experimental procedures, data, and assertion about "demonstrating MSR at group level." Examples of these problems include incomplete experimental design, absence of important control conditions, inappropriate terminology, suboptimal mark application procedures and coding of videos, ambiguity of videos presented as supporting evidence, and inconsistencies in data presentation and interpretation. It is not the case that their study "marks a turning point in the analytical technique of MSR exploration."


Assuntos
Comportamento Animal , Reconhecimento Psicológico , Animais
3.
Brain Sci ; 11(11)2021 Oct 26.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34827408

RESUMO

In this article, we detail the advantages of self-face identification latencies over more traditional tests of mirror self-recognition. Using reaction time latencies (measured in milliseconds) to identify different dimensions of the self, instead of relying on a simple dichotomous pass/fail mirror mark-test outcome, enables investigators to examine individual differences in self-processing time. This is a significant methodological step forward with important implications. The point of departure for our article is to detail research we and others have conducted on latencies for self-face identification, to show how self-processing occurs in the right side of the brain, how schizophrenia is a self-processing disorder, how self-face reaction time latencies implicate the existence of an underlying multiple modal self-processing system, and to explore ideas for future research.

4.
Arch Sex Behav ; 50(2): 495-509, 2021 02.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32440927

RESUMO

We examined sex differences in preferences for sexual variety and novelty to determine whether the Coolidge effect plays a role in human sexuality. In two experimental studies that employed different manipulations, we found converging evidence that men showed a greater preference for variety in potential short-term mates than did women. In the first study, men (n = 281) were more likely than women (n = 353) to select a variety of mates when given the opportunity to distribute chances to have sex with different individuals in hypothetical situations. This sex difference was evident regardless of the targets' attractiveness and age. Further, men found it more appealing if their committed romantic/sexual partners frequently changed their physical appearance, while women reported that they modified their physical appearance more frequently than did men, potentially appealing to male desires for novelty. In the second study, when participants were given a hypothetical dating task using photographs of potential short-term mates, men (n = 40) were more likely than women (n = 56) to select a novel person to date. Collectively, these findings lend support to the idea that sex differences in preferences for sexual variety and novelty are a salient sex-specific evolved component of the repertoire of human mating strategies.


Assuntos
Comportamento de Escolha , Corte/psicologia , Comportamento Sexual/psicologia , Parceiros Sexuais/psicologia , Pessoa Solteira/psicologia , Adulto , Coito/psicologia , Feminino , Humanos , Relações Interpessoais , Masculino , Caracteres Sexuais , Fatores Sexuais , Sexualidade/psicologia , Adulto Jovem
5.
Med Hypotheses ; 141: 109750, 2020 Aug.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32388138

RESUMO

Although not widely studied, behavioral host manipulation by various pathogens has been documented. Host manipulation is the process by which a pathogen evolves adaptations to manipulate the behavior of the host to maximize reproduction (Ro) of the pathogen. The most notable example is rabies. When a host is infected with the rabies virus it gets into the host's central nervous system and triggers hyper aggression. The virus is also present in the rabid animal's saliva so being bitten transmits the infection to a new host and the old host is left to eventually die if untreated. Toxoplasmosis is another example. When mice are infected they demonstrate a fearlessness toward cats, thus increasing their chances of being eaten. Toxoplasmosis needs the digestive tract of the feline to survive. Recent studies have shown that exposure to toxoplasmosis in humans (e.g., through cat feces) has also been associated with behavioral changes that are predicted to enhance the spread of the pathogen. Even the common influenza virus has been shown to selectively increase in-person sociality during the 48-hour incubation period, thus producing an obvious vector for transmission. Here we hypothesize that the novel coronavirus, SARS-CoV2, which produces the COVID-19 disease may produce similar host manipulations that maximize its transmission between humans.


Assuntos
Betacoronavirus/patogenicidade , Infecções por Coronavirus/virologia , Interações Hospedeiro-Patógeno , Modelos Biológicos , Pneumonia Viral/virologia , Comportamento Social , Adulto , Animais , Doenças Assintomáticas/psicologia , Betacoronavirus/genética , Betacoronavirus/fisiologia , Evolução Biológica , COVID-19 , Cuidadores , Criança , Comportamento Infantil , Pré-Escolar , Colesterol/sangue , Infecções por Coronavirus/transmissão , Coleta de Dados , Feminino , Feto/virologia , Giro do Cíngulo/fisiopatologia , Especificidade de Hospedeiro , Interações Hospedeiro-Patógeno/fisiologia , Humanos , Lactente , Recém-Nascido , Período de Incubação de Doenças Infecciosas , Masculino , Pandemias , Pneumonia Viral/transmissão , Gravidez , Complicações Infecciosas na Gravidez/virologia , Efeitos Tardios da Exposição Pré-Natal , SARS-CoV-2
7.
Behav Processes ; 148: 16-19, 2018 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29274762

RESUMO

The recent attempt by Horowitz (2017) to develop an "olfactory mirror" test of self-recognition in domestic dogs raises some important questions about the kinds of data that are required to provide definitive evidence for self-recognition in dogs and other species. We conclude that the "olfactory mirror" constitutes a compelling analog to the mark test for mirror self-recognition in primates, but despite claims to the contrary neither dogs, elephants, dolphins, magpies, horses, manta rays, squid, nor ants have shown compelling, reproducible evidence for self-recognition in any modality.


Assuntos
Comportamento Animal/fisiologia , Ego , Percepção Olfatória/fisiologia , Reconhecimento Psicológico/fisiologia , Animais , Olfato/fisiologia
8.
Primates ; 56(4): 317-26, 2015 Oct.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26341947

RESUMO

We review research on reactions to mirrors and self-recognition in nonhuman primates, focusing on methodological issues. Starting with the initial demonstration in chimpanzees in 1970 and subsequent attempts to extend this to other species, self-recognition in great apes is discussed with emphasis on spontaneous manifestations of mirror-guided self-exploration as well as spontaneous use of the mirror to investigate foreign marks on otherwise nonvisible body parts-the mark test. Attempts to show self-recognition in other primates are examined with particular reference to the lack of convincing examples of spontaneous mirror-guided self-exploration, and efforts to engineer positive mark test responses by modifying the test or using conditioning techniques. Despite intensive efforts to demonstrate self-recognition in other primates, we conclude that to date there is no compelling evidence that prosimians, monkeys, or lesser apes-gibbons and siamangs-are capable of mirror self-recognition.


Assuntos
Primatas/psicologia , Reconhecimento Psicológico , Percepção Visual , Animais , Comportamento Animal , Primatas/fisiologia , Autoimagem
9.
Evol Psychol ; 12(5): 958-78, 2014 Nov 06.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25376054

RESUMO

The current study represents a preliminary investigation of the extent to which female orgasm functions to promote good mate choices. Based on a survey of heterosexual female college students in committed relationships, how often women experienced orgasm as a result of sexual intercourse was related to their partner's family income, his self-confidence, and how attractive he was. Orgasm intensity was also related to how attracted they were to their partners, how many times they had sex per week, and ratings of sexual satisfaction. Those with partners who their friends rated as more attractive also tended to have more intense orgasms. Orgasm frequency was highly correlated (r = .82) with orgasm intensity, and orgasm intensity was a marginally better predictor of sexual satisfaction than orgasm frequency. Sexual satisfaction was related to how physically attracted women were to their partner and the breadth of his shoulders. Women who began having sexual intercourse at earlier ages had more sex partners, experienced more orgasms, and were more sexually satisfied with their partners. We also identified an ensemble of partner psychological traits (motivation, intelligence, focus, and determination) that predicted how often women initiated sexual intercourse. Their partner's sense of humor not only predicted his self-confidence and family income, but it also predicted women's propensity to initiate sex, how often they had sex, and it enhanced their orgasm frequency in comparison with other partners.


Assuntos
Coito/psicologia , Retroalimentação Psicológica , Casamento/psicologia , Orgasmo , Mulheres/psicologia , Adolescente , Feminino , Humanos , Relações Interpessoais , Riso/psicologia , Satisfação Pessoal , Personalidade , Parceiros Sexuais/psicologia
10.
Evol Psychol ; 12(1): 245-50, 2014 Mar 27.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25299762

RESUMO

Evidence suggests that each male may have a unique semen signature, and there are reasons to consider the possibility that semen sampling (i.e., being inseminated by different prospective mates during courtship) may be part of an evolved female mate assessment strategy. Here we theorize that seminal plasma hypersensitivity represents the extreme negative end of this continuum and functions as a deterrent to mating with genetically incompatible suitors.


Assuntos
Evolução Biológica , Hipersensibilidade/etiologia , Sêmen/imunologia , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino
13.
Evol Psychol ; 10(3): 457-86, 2012 Aug 02.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22947672

RESUMO

Simon Baron-Cohen pioneered the idea that different brain types evolved to process information in gender specific ways. Here we expand this approach to looking at eating disorders as a byproduct of the extreme female brain. The incidence of eating disorders is higher among females, and recent findings show that hormones may play a role in eating disorders. We present new evidence from four studies that both an empathizing bias and hyper-mentalizing (as measures of the extreme female brain; EFB) are related to disordered eating and negative evaluation anxiety in women. We also advance the novel hypothesis that concerns about animal welfare (a unique expression of the EFB) may account for the relationship between vegetarianism and eating disorders.


Assuntos
Ansiedade/psicologia , Química Encefálica/fisiologia , Empatia , Transtornos da Alimentação e da Ingestão de Alimentos/psicologia , Adulto , Bem-Estar do Animal , Ansiedade/fisiopatologia , Dieta Vegetariana , Transtornos da Alimentação e da Ingestão de Alimentos/fisiopatologia , Feminino , Hormônios Esteroides Gonadais/fisiologia , Humanos , Modelos Psicológicos , Caracteres Sexuais , Inquéritos e Questionários
14.
PLoS One ; 7(8): e41811, 2012.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22870251

RESUMO

Evolutionary accounts of human traits are often based on proxies for genetic fitness (e.g., number of sex partners, facial attractiveness). Instead of using proxies, actual differences in reproductive success is a more direct measure of darwinian fitness. Certain voice acoustics such as fundamental frequency and measures of health such as handgrip strength correlate with proxies of fitness, yet there are few studies showing the relation of these traits to reproduction. Here, we explore whether the fundamental frequency of the voice and handgrip strength account for differences in actual reproduction among a population of natural fertility humans. Our results show that both fundamental frequency and handgrip strength predict several measures of reproductive success among a group of indigenous Namibian females, particularly amongst the elderly, with weight also predicting reproductive outcomes among males. These findings demonstrate that both hormonally regulated and phenotypic quality markers can be used as measures of darwinian fitness among humans living under conditions that resemble the evolutionary environment of Homo sapiens. We also argue that these findings provide support for the Grandmother Hypothesis.


Assuntos
População Negra , Fertilidade/fisiologia , Força da Mão/fisiologia , Voz/fisiologia , Adolescente , Adulto , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Feminino , Humanos , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Namíbia , Característica Quantitativa Herdável
15.
Front Evol Neurosci ; 4: 1, 2012.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22319496

RESUMO

Body configuration is a sexually dimorphic trait. In humans, men tend to have high shoulder-to-hip ratios. Women in contrast, often have low waist-to-hip ratios (WHR); i.e., narrow waists and broad hips that approximate an hour-glass configuration. Women with low WHR's are rated as more attractive, healthier, and more fertile. They also tend to have more attractive voices, lose their virginity sooner, and have more sex partners. WHR has also been linked with general cognitive performance. In the present study we expand upon previous research examining the role of WHR in cognition. We hypothesized that more feminine body types, as indexed by a low WHR, would be associated with cognitive measures of the female "brain type," such as mental state attribution and empathy because both may depend upon the activational effects of estrogens at puberty. We found that women with low WHRs excel at identifying emotional states of other people and show a cognitive style that favors empathizing over systemizing. We suggest this relationship may be a byproduct of greater gluteofemoral fat stores which are high in the essential fatty acids needed to support brain development and cellular functioning. It is interesting to note that our findings suggest lower WHR females, who are more likely to be targeted for dishonest courtship, may be better at identifying disingenuous claims of commitment.

16.
Infant Behav Dev ; 34(4): 610-6, 2011 Dec.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21794922

RESUMO

Facial attractiveness has been studied extensively, but little research has examined the stability of facial attractiveness of individuals across different stages of development. We conducted a study examining the relationship between facial attractiveness in infants (age 24 months and under) and the same individuals as young adults (age 16-18 years) using infant and adult photographs from high school yearbooks. Contrary to expectations, independent raters' assessments of infant facial attractiveness did not correlate with adult facial attractiveness. These results are discussed in terms of the adaptive function of heightened attractiveness in infancy, which likely evolved to elicit and maintain parental care.


Assuntos
Beleza , Face , Adolescente , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Lactente , Masculino , Valor Preditivo dos Testes , Inquéritos e Questionários
17.
Med Hypotheses ; 77(3): 345-6, 2011 Sep.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21641730

RESUMO

To compensate for the high metabolic costs of lactation, the likelihood of re-impregnation shortly after childbirth is normally reduced due to hormonal changes triggered by breast feeding during the postpartum period. Nowadays, however, bottle feeding as a substitute for breast feeding precludes such changes and leads to early postpartum reinstatement of fertility. We suggest that recent data showing the risk of autism goes up dramatically as the time between pregnancies goes down [1] may be a byproduct of bottle feeding. The decision to bottle feed your last child may unwittingly put your next child at risk of being autistic.


Assuntos
Transtorno Autístico/etiologia , Intervalo entre Nascimentos , Alimentação com Mamadeira/efeitos adversos , Fertilidade/fisiologia , Período Pós-Parto , Evolução Biológica , Humanos , Fatores de Risco
19.
Am J Primatol ; 73(7): 603-6, 2011 Jul.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21442633

RESUMO

Self-recognition continues to attract attention because of the evidence of a striking difference between the great apes and humans, on the one hand, and all other primates; the former are capable of self recognition,whereas no compelling evidence exists for prosimians, monkeys, or lesser apes. This is inspite of numerous attempts to facilitate mirror self-recognition in other primates. Although all previous attempts to find self-recognition in rhesus macaques have failed, a recent article [Rajala et al., PLoS One9:e12865, 2010] claimed the opposite­that adult male rhesus monkeys did recognize their own image in a mirror. We critically examine this claim, and conclude that the article fails to provide acceptable evidence for self-recognition in rhesus monkeys.


Assuntos
Macaca mulatta/psicologia , Reconhecimento Psicológico , Percepção Visual , Animais , Comportamento Animal , Macaca mulatta/fisiologia , Masculino , Autoimagem
20.
Evol Psychol ; 9(3): 390-416, 2011 Sep 12.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22947982

RESUMO

Research shows that sensational news stories as well as popular romance novels often feature themes related to important topics in evolutionary psychology. In the first of four studies described in this paper we examined the song lyrics from three Billboard charts: Country, Pop, and R&B. A content analysis of the lyrics revealed 18 reproductive themes that read like an outline for a course in evolutionary psychology. Approximately 92% of the 174 songs that made it into the Top Ten in 2009 contained one or more reproductive messages, with an average of 10.49 reproductive phrases per song. Although differences in the frequency of different themes between charts were found, further analyses showed that the most popular/bestselling songs contained significantly more reproductive messages. An analysis of the lyrics of opera arias and art songs also revealed evidence for many of the same embedded reproductive messages extending back more than 400 years.


Assuntos
Música/psicologia , Reprodução , Comportamento Sexual/psicologia , Canto , Comércio/estatística & dados numéricos , Humanos , Reprodutibilidade dos Testes
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA
...