Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 17 de 17
Filtrar
1.
Appl Neuropsychol Adult ; : 1-6, 2024 Apr 14.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38615907

RESUMO

The past decade has witnessed amplified public awareness of age-related dementias. This has resulted in a dramatic rise in the number of older persons referred to memory clinics with a primary complaint of self-reported memory loss without an antecedent neurological event (e.g., stroke) who produce neuropsychological test profiles that lack evidence of such impairment. Since the latter part of the 19th century, a confusing array of changing terminology, criteria, and perceived causation have been ascribed to patients with unverified medical symptoms to implicate psychological causation. Such terms are often misperceived by laypersons as reflecting character flaws or malingering. Of import for clinical neuropsychologists, the 11th edition of the International Classification of Diseases added cognitive to symptoms eligible for a diagnosis of the modern formulation, dissociative neurological symptom disorder. One dissociative option for referrals with self-reported neurocognitive symptoms not validated on neuropsychological testing is functional memory disorder, conceived as a psychological disorder where emotional distress is plausibly related to the perceived memory loss but is of less severity than would warrant a major depressive or anxiety diagnosis. If evidence of psychological distress or behavioral impairment is not present the referral likely reflects the increased public awareness of age-related dementias interacting with the high base rate of self-perceived memory loss in the general population. In such cases, a dissociative diagnosis should be avoided as there is evidence of neither a medical nor a psychological disorder. A summary statement of not dementia or similar is likely sufficient to help the patient.

2.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37738469

RESUMO

OBJECTIVE: To assess the association between language proficiency and absolute dominance on language versus visual-perceptual formatted and executive versus delayed memory neuropsychological measures in bilingual adults. METHODS: Data from 55 bilingual, conversationally fluent, neurologically intact, Mexican American, consecutive, adults tested in separate sessions in Spanish and English in a counterbalanced order were analyzed. Age, years of education, self-reported language proficiency, Woodcock-Muñoz Language Survey-Revised (WMLS-R) picture vocabulary measures of language proficiency, and dominance (absolute Spanish-English WMLS-R difference scores) were correlated with 11 measures from La Batería Neuropsicólogica en Español and its original English language tests. RESULTS: Self-reported and WMLS-R measures of language proficiency were significantly correlated in each language. Absolute language dominance was not significantly associated with any Spanish or English neuropsychological raw score. The WMLS-R language proficiency, but not age or years of education, was significantly correlated with language-formatted neuropsychological measures of California Verbal Learning Test delayed free recall number of words (both languages), letter fluency (both languages), delayed story memory (in English), and Stroop interference (Spanish). Linear regression models using age, years of education, and WMLS-R picture vocabulary scores as predictors were significant for all these measures excepting the last. The WMLS-R language proficiency was not significantly associated with raw scores on any visual-perceptual formatted measure. CONCLUSIONS: Monolingual neuropsychological test norms for language-formatted tests likely overestimate bilingual Mexican Americans' performance.

3.
Appl Neuropsychol Adult ; 29(2): 203-211, 2022.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32125909

RESUMO

Epidemiological studies have found that more physically active older participants have a reduced risk of Alzheimer disease. Based on enriched environment animal models, this effect is considered to result from physical exercise-induced molecular brain changes. This hypothesis has been tested in humans with randomized controlled trials involving physical exercise vs. more sedentary interventions with neuropsychological outcome measures. Fifty-one such randomized controlled trials were identified from Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analysis guidelines with keywords related to physical activity, cognition, and aging supplemented with reference list search. The five most popular executive function measures (each used in ≥8 trials) were meta-analyzed. Digit symbol was the only measure with a significant estimated overall effect size, indicating that physical exercise had a small (0.17) positive effect on change scores. Estimated overall effect sizes for physical exercise on Digit Span backward, Trails B, letter fluency, and Stroop Color-Word Interference time with/without correction were all not significantly different from zero. These results provide weak support for the notion that physical exercise produces molecular brain changes that enhance executive function test scores in older, nonclinical, participants.


Assuntos
Função Executiva , Terapia por Exercício , Idoso , Cognição , Exercício Físico , Humanos , Ensaios Clínicos Controlados Aleatórios como Assunto
4.
Arch Clin Neuropsychol ; 36(7): 1350-1360, 2021 Oct 13.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33522567

RESUMO

OBJECTIVE: To identify the Spanish language neuropsychological tests most frequently used in the evaluation of Hispanic Americans/Latino/as/x across eight neuropsychological domains. To compare these with previously surveyed most frequently used English language tests in the US/Canada. To describe the norms used with Spanish language tests. METHOD: An anonymous, uncompensated, 12-min survey was emailed to 169 members of the American Academy of Clinical Neuropsychology, Hispanic Neuropsychological Society, and National Academy of Neuropsychology who self-identified as providing neuropsychological evaluations in Spanish via organizational websites. RESULTS: The response rate was 36%. Respondents were all licensed U.S. psychologists with the majority fluent in Spanish, conducting less than half their evaluations in Spanish. There was an overlap between the versions of the three most frequently used Spanish versus English neuropsychological tests, but it varied by domain with 3/3 similar for visual-spatial/visuoconstructional skills and sensory/motor functioning domains and 2/3 similar for all other domains except memory (0/3). English language norm use predominated within the mood/personality and sensory/motor functioning domains. In all other domains, Spanish language norms collected in the continental US were preferred to those from foreign Spanish-speaking countries. The most frequently used foreign norms were from Mexico. CONCLUSION: Except for the memory domain, there was a sizeable overlap between the three most frequently used Spanish and English language neuropsychological tests in the US. Spanish language tests are primarily interpreted with Spanish norms collected in the continental US except within the mood/personality and sensory/motor functioning domains where English language norms predominate.


Assuntos
Hispânico ou Latino , Idioma , Humanos , Testes de Linguagem , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Inquéritos e Questionários
5.
J Hist Neurosci ; 29(2): 234-245, 2020.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31986104

RESUMO

The introduction of railway transportation in Great Britain in the early-nineteenth century saw an increased frequency of trauma cases involving persisting symptoms without objective evidence of injury. In 1866, a prominent surgeon, Sir John Eric Erichsen, attributed such symptoms to concussion of the spine (popularized as "railway spine") that involved an organic pathology, inflammation of the spinal cord in the absence of spinal fracture, with potential psychological overlay. This was widely accepted within the medico-legal context throughout the 1870s, whereby passengers sought compensation for collision-related injuries. In 1883, a railway surgeon named Herbert William Page countered the assertion that many of Erichsen's cases likely had sustained direct physical injury to the spine, the cord, and/or the spinal nerves; and in cases without such injury, the symptoms were psychogenic, as in traumatic neurasthenia and/or hysteria. Similarities between Erichsen's and Page's medico-legal positions, such as conscious and unconscious forms of symptom exaggeration that would both resolve upon settlement of the case, ushered in the era of medical injury compensation.


Assuntos
Concussão Encefálica/história , Compensação e Reparação/história , Ferrovias , Traumatismos da Medula Espinal , Ferimentos e Lesões , Compensação e Reparação/legislação & jurisprudência , Cirurgia Geral , História do Século XIX , Humanos , Responsabilidade Legal , Masculino , Traumatismos da Medula Espinal/complicações , Traumatismos da Medula Espinal/história , Reino Unido
6.
Clin Neuropsychol ; 34(2): 278-295, 2020 02.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31146639

RESUMO

Objective: Concussion is a clinical syndrome of biomechanically induced alteration in brain function. The historical derivation of current operational definitions of uncomplicated concussive brain injury based on acute injury variables especially posttraumatic amnesia (PTA) vs. symptom presence (no PTA) are reviewed.Methods: Major milestones from 150 plus years of concussion research include (a) railway spine; (b) traumatic neurosis; (c) duration of loss of consciousness predicting severity of brain injury; (d) early neuropsychological studies; (e) sport related concussion; and (f) chronic traumatic encephalopathy.Results: After initial organic explanations proved untenable, 19th century conceptions of concussion were operationally defined from symptom presence, attributed primarily to psychogenic (emotional, motivational, and/or predispositional) causes. In the 1930s, duration of PTA was linked to brain injury severity via outcome studies. Concussion (as defined by PTA < 1 h) was later found to cause a transient lowering of neuropsychological test scores in memory and executive function. In the 1990s, organizational definitions of sport-related concussion began to include biomechanical events without PTA that produced symptoms. Such events have not been linked to neuropsychological impairment.Conclusions: Two differing operational definitions of concussion are in the current use, involving the presence vs. absence of PTA. These two groupings are not clinically similar, although they are currently being widely treated as such. Operational definitions of concussion should address both minimum and maximums for duration of PTA, initial Glasgow Coma Scale score, complications, and symptom presence.


Assuntos
Concussão Encefálica/diagnóstico , Lesões Encefálicas/diagnóstico , Testes Neuropsicológicos/normas , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino
7.
Clin Neuropsychol ; 33(8): 1445-1454, 2019 11.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30585760

RESUMO

Objective: To measure failure rates among non-clinical, Mexican American, bilinguals on embedded performance validity measures from the California Verbal Learning Test3 and verbal fluency and the stand-alone Rey 15-item test plus recognition (Rey 15-IR) using valid/invalid performance cut scores developed for monolingual, English-speakers. Method: Participants were 60 consecutive recruits, aged between 18 and 75 years, with ≤16 years of education who self-identified as bilingual (confirmed via formal assessment) and chose the language of assessment, Spanish or English, for the performance validity tests. Results: The only performance validity measure studied with a higher failure rate (19%) than that for monolingual, English-speakers (9%) was semantic fluency administered in Spanish. Conclusions: Valid/invalid performance cut scores derived for monolingual English-speakers generated comparable or lower failure rates among Mexican American bilinguals on CVLT3 forced-choice recognition, CVLT3 yes-no recognition hits, letter fluency, semantic fluency in English, and the Rey 15-IR.


Assuntos
Testes de Linguagem/normas , Testes Neuropsicológicos/normas , Aprendizagem Verbal/fisiologia , Adolescente , Adulto , Idoso , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Americanos Mexicanos , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Multilinguismo , Sensibilidade e Especificidade , Adulto Jovem
8.
J Clin Exp Neuropsychol ; 40(9): 874-886, 2018 11.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29510648

RESUMO

INTRODUCTION: Longitudinal studies have found that physical activity protects against Alzheimer disease, but the mechanism is unknown. The prevailing model derives from animal research and has physical activity directly affecting brain physiology by increasing brain volume, brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF), and hippocampal neurogenesis with consequent gains in neuropsychological test scores. Supporting evidence has been mixed, with physical-activity-related gains across multiple neuropsychological domains considered indicative of the protective effect. Hippocampal-mediated delayed memory functioning is the first neurocognitive skill to be impaired in the early stages of Alzheimer disease, and physical-activity-related gains on delayed memory measures would provide the strongest support for the model. RESULTS: Review of 26 randomized controlled trials with nonclinical older, mild cognitive impairment, and dementia participants found only one with significant physical-activity-related gains in delayed memory compared to controls. This evidence does not support the physiological brain change model. Similarly, there is questionable support from those randomized controlled trials that have measured physical-activity-related brain volume and blood BDNF levels (neurogenesis having no valid labeling technique in living humans). CONCLUSION: Physical-activity-related protective effects against Alzheimer disease are likely mediated through pathways outside the central nervous system.


Assuntos
Disfunção Cognitiva/terapia , Demência/terapia , Terapia por Exercício/psicologia , Exercício Físico/psicologia , Repressão Psicológica , Idoso , Disfunção Cognitiva/fisiopatologia , Disfunção Cognitiva/psicologia , Demência/fisiopatologia , Demência/psicologia , Exercício Físico/fisiologia , Terapia por Exercício/métodos , Humanos , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Ensaios Clínicos Controlados Aleatórios como Assunto
9.
Neuropsychol Rev ; 27(2): 147-157, 2017 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28484905

RESUMO

Distinct forms of acquired neurocognitive impairment are often described by "a" prefixed terms that derive from ancient Greek (and in one case Latin). Two modern English language neurological and neuropsychological reference books were searched to identify 17 such terms in contemporary usage: amnesia, akinesia, ataxia, aphasia, agraphia, anosmia, apraxia, athetosis, ageusia, achromatopsia, agnosia, alexia, amusia, anomia, anarthria, anosognosia, and acalculia. These were traced to their initial association with acquired neurocognitive impairment in German, English, and French language medical publications from the late 18th, 19th, and early 20th centuries (1770 through 1920). Some of these terms (e.g., agnosia) were used in ancient Greek, although not associated with neurocognitive impairment. The remainder constitute novel semantically plausible (e.g., anosmia) and unclear (e.g., alexia) formulations. In the localizationist thinking of the time, neurocognition was conceived as being organized within specialized "centers" in specific locations connected by pathways within the brain.


Assuntos
Transtornos Cognitivos , Doenças do Sistema Nervoso , Terminologia como Assunto , Transtornos Cognitivos/história , História do Século XVIII , História do Século XIX , História do Século XX , Humanos , Doenças do Sistema Nervoso/história
10.
Arch Clin Neuropsychol ; 32(5): 610-617, 2017 Aug 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28430846

RESUMO

OBJECTIVE: To describe the theoretical and clinical implications of the neuropsychological evaluation of a case of bariatric surgery-related Wernicke-Korsakoff syndrome. METHOD: The patient was a 37-year old, female, bilingual, bachelor's degree educated, Mexican American public relations consultant without preexisting psychiatric, neurological, or substance abuse history. Recovery from laparoscopic sleeve gastrectomy surgery for morbid obesity was complicated by intraabdominal abscess, multibacterial infection, and prolonged nausea and vomiting. About 15 weeks post-surgery she was diagnosed with Wernicke's encephalopathy. She had a positive response to thiamine supplement but was left with persisting self-reported memory problems that were confirmed by family members. Multiple neuroimaging studies were all normal. RESULTS: A neuropsychological evaluation at 14 months post-surgery revealed anterograde amnesia for verbal and visual-perceptual material. There was no clear period of temporally graded retrograde amnesia. Scores on tests of visual-perceptual, language, fine motor, and executive functions were unimpaired. She had awareness of her neurocognitive impairment, but did not exhibit emotional distress. Follow-up neuropsychological evaluation at 17 months showed a similar neurocognitive profile with increased emotional distress. CONCLUSIONS: Her preserved executive functioning is theoretically important as it supports arguments that such impairment in alcohol use-related Korsakoff syndrome derives from the toxic effects of the prolonged misuse of alcohol and not vitamin deficiency. From a clinical perspective, neuropsychological evaluation of thiamine treated, bariatric surgery-related, Wernicke's encephalopathy cases is indicated if there is suspicion of residual memory impairment.


Assuntos
Amnésia Anterógrada/etiologia , Cirurgia Bariátrica/efeitos adversos , Síndrome de Korsakoff/etiologia , Obesidade Mórbida/cirurgia , Encefalopatia de Wernicke/etiologia , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos
11.
Neuropsychology ; 30(8): 988-997, 2016 11.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27175942

RESUMO

OBJECTIVE: To review the current literature on the effects of bilingualism on vocabulary, executive functions, age of dementia onset, and regional brain structure. METHOD: PubMed and PsycINFO databases were searched (from January 1999 to present) for relevant original research and review articles on bilingualism (but not multilingualism) paired with each target neuropsychological variable published in English. A qualitative review of these articles was conducted. RESULTS: It has long been known that mean scores of bilinguals fall below those of monolinguals on vocabulary and other language, but not visual-perceptual, format cognitive tests. Contemporary studies that have reported higher mean scores for bilinguals than monolinguals on executive function task-switching or inhibition tasks have not always been replicated, leading to concerns of publication bias, statistical flaws, and failures to match groups on potentially confounding variables. Studies suggesting the onset of Alzheimer's disease occurred about 4 years later for bilinguals versus monolinguals have not been confirmed in longitudinal, cohort, community-based, incidence studies that have used neuropsychological testing and diagnostic criteria to establish an age of dementia diagnosis. Neuroimaging studies of regional gray and white matter volume in bilinguals versus monolinguals show inconsistencies in terms of both the regions of difference and the nature of the difference. CONCLUSIONS: Resolving inconsistencies in the behavioral data is necessary before searching in the brain for neuroanatomical correlation. Comparisons of balanced versus language-dominant groups within the same ethnoculture combined with objective measurement of bilingualism could better match groups on potentially confounding variables. (PsycINFO Database Record


Assuntos
Idade de Início , Doença de Alzheimer/diagnóstico , Doença de Alzheimer/psicologia , Encéfalo/fisiopatologia , Função Executiva/fisiologia , Multilinguismo , Vocabulário , Idoso , Mapeamento Encefálico , Criança , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Neuroimagem , Testes Neuropsicológicos
12.
Neuropsychol Rehabil ; 26(2): 261-85, 2016.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25686381

RESUMO

Historically, anosognosia referred to under-report of striking symptoms of acquired brain injury (e.g., hemiplegia) with debilitating functional consequences and was linked with anosodiaphoria, an emotional reaction of indifference. It was later extended to include under-report of all manner of symptoms of acquired brain injury by the patient compared to clinicians, family members, or functional performance. Anosognosia is related to time since onset of brain injury but not consistently to demographic variables, lesion location (except that it is more common after unilateral right than left hemispheric injury), or specific neuropsychological test scores. This review considers all manifestations of anosognosia as a unitary phenomenon with differing clinical characteristics dictated by variability in linked cognitive impairments. It is concluded that anosognosia has three chief contributing factors: (1) procedural: measurement differences across studies in terms of symptom selection and the designation of a "gold standard" of patient symptomatology; (2) psychological: a tendency towards positive self-evaluation and the avoidance of adverse information, that also occurs in neurologically intact individuals; and (3) neuropathological: an increased likelihood of error recognition failure from disconnections that disrupt feedback between injured brain regions governing specific behaviours (symptoms) and anterior cingulate/insular cortex. Anosodiaphoria is considered as an associated symptom, resulting from the same psychological and neuropathological factors.


Assuntos
Agnosia/fisiopatologia , Agnosia/psicologia , Lesões Encefálicas/complicações , Transtornos Cognitivos/fisiopatologia , Transtornos Cognitivos/psicologia , Agnosia/diagnóstico , Agnosia/etiologia , Encéfalo/fisiopatologia , Transtornos Cognitivos/diagnóstico , Transtornos Cognitivos/etiologia , Emoções , Humanos
14.
Neuropsychol Rev ; 24(2): 77-87, 2014 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24442602

RESUMO

Historically, the insula was considered primary gustatory cortex. Now it is known to play a more comprehensive role in the processing of sensory information, including acting as primary cortex for interoceptive information, including autonomic nervous system mediated changes. As such, it is critical for emotional feeling in accord with the James-Lange theory, a role previously ascribed to the limbic system. Neuroimaged abnormal grey matter volumes or activity levels in the insula have been associated with schizophrenia, eating disorders, anxiety and mood disorders, conduct disorder, autism, addiction, and chronic pain. The significance of these abnormal activity patterns remains theoretical. Neuropsychological studies have linked dominant insula injury with various symptoms of aphasia, but its exact role in language processing remains uncertain as most cases involve lesions that extend into perisylvian language zones. Functional neuroimaging studies have found insula hyper-activations, typically in conjunction with anterior cingulate cortex, for all manner of experimental tasks including those involving perception, intentional action, and consciousness. Such neuroimaged activity is unlikely to be task-specific, but rather reflective of generic changes in autonomic activity in response to salience, homeostatic incongruence, or cognitive challenge.


Assuntos
Córtex Cerebral/fisiologia , Cognição/fisiologia , Emoções/fisiologia , Animais , Sistema Nervoso Autônomo/fisiologia , Sistema Nervoso Autônomo/fisiopatologia , Mapeamento Encefálico , Córtex Cerebral/anatomia & histologia , Córtex Cerebral/fisiopatologia , Humanos
15.
Neurosci Biobehav Rev ; 37(3): 340-8, 2013 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23313645

RESUMO

Early localizationists linked anterior cingulate cortex (ACC: Brodmann's area 24 and adjacent regions) with emotional behavior, paving the way for bilateral cingulotomy psychosurgery in severe, treatment resistant, cases of obsessive-compulsive disorder, chronic pain, depression, and substance abuse. Neuropsychological follow-up of such cases demonstrated executive function impairment. Abnormal neuroimaged activity in ACC has been found in many psychiatric conditions, including obsessive-compulsive disorder, chronic pain, substance abuse, and schizophrenia. With healthy participants, increased neuroimaged activity in ACC has been linked with challenging executive function tasks, homeostatically incongruous physical states, and the encoding of the pleasant/averseness of stimuli. There is disagreement on the cortical substrate subsumed by the term ACC, the existence of functionally distinct ACC subregions (e.g., dorsal: cognitive vs. ventral: emotion), and the interpretation of functional neuroimaging studies. Synthesis of neuropsychological and functional neuroimaging studies suggests ACC contributes to behavior by modifying responses especially in reaction to challenging cognitive and physical states that require additional effortful cognitive control. This is accomplished by monitoring the emotional salience of stimuli, exerting control over the autonomic nervous system, and modulating cognitive activity.


Assuntos
Neuroimagem Funcional , Giro do Cíngulo/patologia , Psicocirurgia , Animais , Sistema Nervoso Autônomo/patologia , Sistema Nervoso Autônomo/fisiopatologia , Sistema Nervoso Autônomo/cirurgia , Neuroimagem Funcional/métodos , Giro do Cíngulo/fisiopatologia , Giro do Cíngulo/cirurgia , Humanos , Transtorno Obsessivo-Compulsivo/patologia , Transtorno Obsessivo-Compulsivo/psicologia , Transtorno Obsessivo-Compulsivo/cirurgia , Psicocirurgia/métodos
16.
Arch Clin Neuropsychol ; 27(3): 268-76, 2012 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22337934

RESUMO

Conventional neuropsychological norms developed for monolinguals likely overestimate normal performance in bilinguals on language but not visual-perceptual format tests. This was studied by comparing neuropsychological false-positive rates using the 50th percentile of conventional norms and individual comparison standards (Picture Vocabulary or Matrix Reasoning scores) as estimates of preexisting neuropsychological skill level against the number expected from the normal distribution for a consecutive sample of 56 neurologically intact, bilingual, Hispanic Americans. Participants were tested in separate sessions in Spanish and English in the counterbalanced order on La Bateria Neuropsicologica and the original English language tests on which this battery was based. For language format measures, repeated-measures multivariate analysis of variance showed that individual estimates of preexisting skill level in English generated the mean number of false positives most approximate to that expected from the normal distribution, whereas the 50th percentile of conventional English language norms did the same for visual-perceptual format measures. When using conventional Spanish or English monolingual norms for language format neuropsychological measures with bilingual Hispanic Americans, individual estimates of preexisting skill level are recommended over the 50th percentile.


Assuntos
Idioma , Multilinguismo , Testes Neuropsicológicos/normas , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos , Adolescente , Adulto , Idoso , Análise de Variância , Reações Falso-Positivas , Feminino , Hispânico ou Latino/psicologia , Humanos , Testes de Linguagem , Masculino , Rememoração Mental/fisiologia , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Padrões de Referência , Semântica , Vocabulário , Adulto Jovem
17.
Arch Clin Neuropsychol ; 22(8): 991-1001, 2007 Nov.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17900857

RESUMO

This study compared the performance of Hispanic American bilingual adults on Spanish and English language versions of a neuropsychological test battery. Language achievement test scores were used to divide 36 bilingual, neurologically intact, Hispanic Americans from south Texas into Spanish-dominant, balanced, and English-dominant bilingual groups. They were administered the eight subtests of the Bateria Neuropsicologica and the Matrix Reasoning subtest of the WAIS-III in Spanish and English. Half the participants were tested in Spanish first. Balanced bilinguals showed no significant differences in test scores between Spanish and English language administrations. Spanish and/or English dominant bilinguals showed significant effects of language of administration on tests with higher language compared to visual perceptual weighting (Woodcock-Munoz Language Survey-Revised, Letter Fluency, Story Memory, and Stroop Color and Word Test). Scores on tests with higher visual-perceptual weighting (Matrix Reasoning, Figure Memory, Wisconsin Card Sorting Test, and Spatial Span), were not significantly affected by language of administration, nor were scores on the Spanish/California Verbal Learning Test, and Digit Span. A problem was encountered in comparing false positive rates in each language, as Spanish norms fell below English norms, resulting in a much higher false positive rate in English across all bilingual groupings. Use of a comparison standard (picture vocabulary score) reduced false positive rates in both languages, but the higher false positive rate in English persisted.


Assuntos
Cognição/fisiologia , Hispânico ou Latino/psicologia , Idioma , Multilinguismo , Testes Neuropsicológicos/estatística & dados numéricos , Adulto , Idoso , Feminino , Humanos , Desenvolvimento da Linguagem , Testes de Linguagem , Masculino , Memória , Pessoa de Meia-Idade
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA