Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 74
Filtrar
1.
J Intell ; 12(2)2024 Jan 26.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38392169

RESUMO

The term "empathic accuracy" has been applied to people's ability to infer the contents of other people's minds-that is, other people's varying feelings and/or thoughts over the course of a social interaction. However, despite the ease of intuitively linking this skill to competence in helping professions such as counseling, the "empathic" prefix in its name may have contributed to overestimating its association with prosocial traits and behaviors. Accuracy in reading others' thoughts and feelings, like many other skills, can be used toward prosocial-but also malevolent or morally neutral-ends. Prosocial intentions can direct attention towards other people's thoughts and feelings, which may, in turn, increase accuracy in inferring those thoughts and feelings, but attention to others' thoughts and feelings does not necessarily heighten prosocial intentions, let alone outcomes.

2.
Patient Educ Couns ; 119: 108041, 2024 Feb.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37945425

RESUMO

OBJECTIVE: To offer a critique of empathy concept usage in healthcare and medical education research. METHODS: Analysis of current usage and suggestions for authors and researchers. RESULTS: Empathy is often undefined or inconsistently defined, and "empathy" as represented in research covers an unmanageably wide and varied range of intentions, attitudes, emotions, and behaviors. The ubiquitous use of "empathy" as a vague and often undefined umbrella term hinders comprehension and, therefore, scientific progress. Patients are rarely asked directly about empathy; instead, measures of so-called perceived empathy contain descriptive items that could as well be called quality of care, patient-centeredness, or patient satisfaction. CONCLUSIONS: Although "empathy" in medical care is widely valued by researchers, educators, and practitioners, the empathy concept as used in the published literature is overused and unclear, and potentially damaging to scholarship, medical education, and ultimately healthcare. The vague term empathy should be replaced as much as possible with concrete descriptions of what is actually measured, experimentally manipulated, or taught. PRACTICE IMPLICATIONS: Identifying patients' own empathy definitions will improve medical education and medical care through clarifying what clinical behaviors will best fulfill patients' needs and desires. This approach allows for greater specificity and personalized care delivery.


Assuntos
Educação Médica , Empatia , Humanos , Emoções , Satisfação do Paciente , Proliferação de Células
4.
Psychol Assess ; 34(4): 397-404, 2022 Apr.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35377686

RESUMO

Conceptual flaws can undermine even rigorous test development efforts, especially in the broad empathy and social cognition domains, which are characterized by measure proliferation and inconsistently used construct terms. We discuss these issues, focusing on a new instrument of "mentalizing" as a case study. Across several studies, Clutterbuck et al. (2021a) developed the Four-Item Mentalising Index (FIMI). They described it as the first self-report measure of mentalizing ability and suggested that it offers substantial advances for research and assessment. As we demonstrate with conceptual arguments and empirical data, the FIMI embodies several major problems that are common in this area of research. Using the FIMI as a case study, we underline the importance for test developers of attending to the nonnegotiable necessity of discriminant validity analyses, the challenge of choosing appropriate convergent validity measures, and the difficulties of navigating the jingle-jangle jungle of empathy and social cognition construct terms. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).


Assuntos
Empatia , Mentalização , Cognição , Humanos , Autorrelato
5.
Patient Educ Couns ; 105(7): 2299-2306, 2022 07.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35287992

RESUMO

OBJECTIVES: Training in emotion management is not a standard part of medical education. This study's objective was to understand physicians' challenges navigating emotion (their own and their patients') and identify areas for intervention to support physician wellness and enhance patient care. METHODS: In 2019, we surveyed 103 physicians in emergency medicine, internal medicine, family medicine, and neurology. Participants quantitatively reported emotion training, emotions that were challenging, and barriers to addressing emotion. They provided qualitative examples of emotion challenges and successes that we analyzed using an inductive thematic analysis. RESULTS: There were no significant differences in responses by specialty. Only 10% reported receiving emotion management training, with no evidence that more recently trained physicians received more. Those who had received training on emotion reported greater comfort in dealing with patients' emotions and were more likely to engage in teaching on emotion. There were gender and career stage differences regarding which emotions physicians found most challenging. The authors identified central themes of emotion-related challenges and successes. CONCLUSIONS: Targeted educational initiatives are needed to advance physicians' ability to navigate emotion in clinical encounters. PRACTICE IMPLICATIONS: Developing strategies for managing patients' emotions may better prepare physicians for navigating the emotional demands of practicing medicine.


Assuntos
Relações Médico-Paciente , Médicos , Emoções , Medicina de Família e Comunidade , Humanos , Médicos/psicologia , Inquéritos e Questionários
6.
J Soc Psychol ; 162(1): 1-6, 2022 Jan 02.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34978951

RESUMO

The concept of empathy as it is used in scholarly discourse has been challenged for over 50 years, yet the same ambiguities and controversies associated with the concept persist and, indeed, have accelerated with the accumulation of definitions, subconstructs that are included under the empathy umbrella, and measuring instruments. In this article we address the following interrelated problems: many definitions, authors not offering definitions, authors using instruments that do not match their definitions, authors not specifying definitions and measurements in cited studies, the jingle-jangle problem, and the persistent need for more construct validity research. In this Special Issue on empathy and its problems, authors bring new theoretical insights, creative research designs, and a critical focus on the empathy concept itself.


Assuntos
Empatia , Humanos
7.
Cancer ; 127(22): 4258-4265, 2021 Nov 15.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34351620

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: Oncology patients and physicians value empathy because of its association with improved health outcomes. Common measures of empathy lack consistency and were developed without direct input from patients. Because of their intense engagement with health care systems, oncology patients may have unique perspectives on what behaviors signal empathy in a clinical setting. METHODS: As part of a cross-sectional study of patient perspectives on clinician empathy at an academic cancer center in the northeastern United States, the authors solicited up to 10 free-text responses to an open-ended question about what clinician behaviors define empathy. RESULTS: The authors categorized open-ended responses from 89 oncology patients into 5 categories representing 14 themes. These categories were relationship sensitivity, focus on the whole person, communication, clinician attributes, and institutional resources and care processes. Frequently represented themes, including listening, understanding, and attention to emotions and what matters most, aligned with existing measures of empathy; behaviors that were not well represented among existing measures included qualities of information sharing and other communication elements. Patients also associated clinician demeanor, accessibility, and competence with empathy. CONCLUSIONS: Oncology patients' perspectives on empathy highlight clinician behaviors and attributes that may help to refine patient experience measures and may be adopted by clinicians and cancer centers to enhance patient care and outcomes. High-quality communication skills training can promote active listening and paying attention to the whole person. A system-level focus on delivering empathic care may improve patients' experiences and outcomes. LAY SUMMARY: Oncology patients' responses to an open-ended question about empathic clinician behavior have revealed insights into a variety of behaviors that are perceived as demonstrative of empathy. These include behaviors that imply sensitivity to the clinician-patient relationship, such as listening and understanding and attention to the whole person. Participants valued caring communication and demeanor and clinician accessibility. Perspective taking was not common among answers. Many existing measures of clinical care quality do not include the behaviors cited by patients as empathic. These results can inform efforts to refine quality measures of empathy-associated behaviors in clinical practice. Cancer centers can use skills training to improve elements of communication.


Assuntos
Empatia , Neoplasias , Comunicação , Estudos Transversais , Humanos , Oncologia , Neoplasias/terapia , Relações Médico-Paciente
8.
Patient Educ Couns ; 104(10): 2425-2431, 2021 10.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34330597

RESUMO

OBJECTIVES: To explore how physicians in neurology, family medicine, internal medicine, and emergency medicine characterize clinical empathy. METHODS: Physicians (N = 94) were asked to describe up to 10 examples of empathic physician behavior. Data were analyzed using template analysis. RESULTS: Physicians' descriptions of clinical empathy patterned into three themes: Clinical Performance and Professionalism, Interpersonal Communication, and Clinician Orientation. Clinical Performance and Professionalism subthemes included physician competency and accessibility; intersection with institutional resources; and spending/making/taking time with patients. Interpersonal Communication subthemes involved information sharing; verbal and nonverbal approaches; interpersonal sensitivity; physician self-disclosure; and attention to emotion. Clinician Orientation encompassed general physician demeanor and internal thoughts and feelings that might be unobservable by patients. Physicians varied widely in the themes they mentioned in their definition of empathy. CONCLUSION: Physicians hold diverse notions of clinical empathy. These extend beyond traditional affective and cognitive empathy definitions to include structural elements like team-based care and accessibility after hours. Communication behaviors were perceived as important for demonstrating empathy. Some physician descriptions of empathy may not be perceptible to patients. PRACTICE IMPLICATIONS: Training physicians to engage in behaviors that both they and patients perceive as empathic may lead to higher patient and physician satisfaction.


Assuntos
Empatia , Médicos , Comunicação , Medicina de Família e Comunidade , Humanos , Relações Médico-Paciente
9.
Front Psychol ; 12: 667326, 2021.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33995225

RESUMO

Thin slices are used across a wide array of research domains to observe, measure, and predict human behavior. This article reviews the thin-slice method as a measurement technique and summarizes current comparative thin-slice research regarding the reliability and validity of thin slices to represent behavior or social constructs. We outline decision factors in using thin-slice behavioral coding and detail three avenues of thin-slice comparative research: (1) assessing whether thin slices can adequately approximate the total of the recorded behavior or be interchangeable with each other (representativeness); (2) assessing how well thin slices can predict variables that are different from the behavior measured in the slice (predictive validity), and (3) assessing how interpersonal judgment accuracy can depend on the length of the slice (accuracy-length validity). The aim of the review is to provide information researchers may use when designing and evaluating thin-slice behavioral measurement.

10.
Patient Educ Couns ; 104(9): 2378-2381, 2021 09.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33593645

RESUMO

Researchers in the healthcare communication field come from many different educational backgrounds. Such diversity generally strengthens a field, but sometimes a set of beliefs or a particular orthodoxy may predominate in ways that are negative. We discuss one such example, noting how the research culture deriving from training in schools of education treats the concepts of reliability and validity. We note that some researchers working in fields such as medical education and healthcare communication use the terms "reliable" and "valid" loosely or even incorrectly, often referring to them as a single catch-phrase. More importantly, we caution healthcare communication researchers against a tyranny of reliability and validity in which researchers feel pressure to avoid creating unique instruments to study new questions, instead using instruments with previously demonstrated reliability and validity even when these may not directly capture the concept of interest. This practice is motivated by realistic fears that reviewers and editors will disapprove of their work because the instruments used are not known to be "reliable and valid." We encourage the research community to take a more balanced approach wherein originality is not stifled, and in which creativity and rigor exist side by side.


Assuntos
Comunicação , Hospitais , Humanos , Psicometria , Reprodutibilidade dos Testes
11.
J Soc Psychol ; 161(1): 5-24, 2021 Jan 02.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32870130

RESUMO

The term "empathy" is popular, yet fuzzy. How laypeople define it has not been investigated. In Study 1, we analyzed 99 participants' free narratives describing their personal definition, and in Study 1 (N = 191) and Study 2 (N = 351), we asked participants to rate a list of specific behaviors and tendencies for how well each one matched their personal definition. Out of 10 coded components, perspective taking was mentioned most often and personal distress (anxious reactivity in emotional situations) was never mentioned. Item ratings revealed four Empathy Concept factors: Prosocial Emotional Response, Interpersonal Perceptiveness, Other Perspective, and Anxious Reactivity. Other Perspective and Prosocial Emotional Response were most highly endorsed while Anxious Reactivity showed the lowest endorsement. Individuals varied widely in their endorsements of the factors. These results demonstrate that laypeople hold a multifactorial set of definitions of empathy and differ widely from one another in which ones they endorse.


Assuntos
Emoções , Empatia , Humanos , Percepção Social
12.
Patient Educ Couns ; 104(2): 315-321, 2021 02.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32811749

RESUMO

OBJECTIVE: To identify differences in patient-physician interactions associated with improvements in GERD symptoms in a randomized controlled trial comparing integrative medicine and primary care/standard visits. METHODS: We analyzed video recordings of 2-minute excerpts (thin slices) from the beginning, middle, and end of 21 study visits (11 standard, 10 integrative medicine). RESULTS: According to blind coders' analysis of the excerpts, prospective improvement in GERD symptoms was most highly correlated with patients appearing pleased (r = 0.71, p < 0.01) and friendly (r = 0.67, p < 0.01) at the end of the visit, controlling for visit type. The combination of patient and physician smiling at the end of the visit was associated with improvement in GERD symptoms (r2 = 0.45, p = 0.004). The physician in the integrative visits was more engaged (p = 0.009), friendly (p = 0.005), relaxed (p = 0.002), smiled longer (p = 0.006), gazed longer (p = 0.02), and gestured more (p = 0.007), compared to standard visits. Patients in integrative visits also smiled longer (p = 0.004). CONCLUSION: The expanded history-taking questions asked by integrative clinicians may enhance relationship building, modifying patients' responses and improving patient-centered behaviors from clinicians,ultimately facilitating symptom improvement. PRACTICE IMPLICATIONS: Analysis of nonverbal behaviors may facilitate a better understanding of patient-clinician interactions in integrative medicine visits and yield insights to improve clinical interactions in conventional medicine.


Assuntos
Medicina Integrativa , Médicos , Comunicação , Humanos , Visita a Consultório Médico , Avaliação de Resultados em Cuidados de Saúde , Relações Médico-Paciente , Estudos Prospectivos
13.
Patient Educ Couns ; 104(5): 1237-1245, 2021 05.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33234440

RESUMO

OBJECTIVES: To explore what undergraduates, community members, oncology patients, and physicians consider empathic behavior in a physician. METHODS: 150 undergraduates, 152 community members, 95 physicians, and 89 oncology patients rated 49 hypothetical physician behaviors for how well they fit their personal definition of physician empathy. Dimensions of empathy were explored and compared across groups. RESULTS: Three dimensions of empathy were Conscientious and Reassuring, Relationship Oriented, and Emotionally Involved. Relationship Oriented was the most strongly endorsed, followed by Emotionally Involved, with Conscientious and Reassuring coming in last. There were no group differences for Conscientious and Reassuring, but the Relationship Oriented factor was more endorsed by the clinical groups (physicians and patients) than the non-clinical groups. The Emotionally Involved factor was endorsed by physicians notably more than by patients. CONCLUSION: What is considered clinical empathy is not the same across individuals and stakeholder groups. PRACTICE IMPLICATIONS: Physicians and patients differ in how much they include the physicians' emotionality and emotion-related actions in their definition of empathy. Communication training for physicians that emphasizes behaviors associated with empathy (listening, understanding a person's feelings and perspectives, and showing interest in and concern for the whole person) may enhance patients' perception of clinical empathy.


Assuntos
Neoplasias , Médicos , Comunicação , Empatia , Humanos , Relações Médico-Paciente , Estudantes , Universidades
14.
Patient Educ Couns ; 103(6): 1143-1149, 2020 06.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31964578

RESUMO

OBJECTIVE: To determine associations between patient affect and physician liking of the patient, and their associations with physician behavior and patient-reported outcomes. METHODS: Structural equation modeling based on coding of 497 videotaped hospital encounters, with questionnaires assessing pre-visit patient affect, post-visit patient affect and encounter evaluations, and physician liking of the patient, involving 71 physicians. RESULTS: In first visits, patient reported outcomes were strongly correlated with physician behavior and less so with physician liking, while in later visits, patient reported outcomes were directly related to physician liking and not mediated by physician behavior. Physician liking predicted physician behavior, more for female physicians in first visits. Patient negative affect before the visit was negatively associated with male physicians' liking. When acquainted, both patient positive and negative affect were associated with physician liking. CONCLUSION: Physician liking of the patient plays a dynamic role in a consultation, is influenced by patient pre-encounter affect, and influences physician behavior. The dynamics are different in first and later visits, and influenced by physician gender. PRACTICE IMPLICATIONS: Physicians should be aware how patient affect influences their behavior, and administrators should take any prior relationship between patient and physician into account when evaluating patient reported outcomes.


Assuntos
Comunicação , Relações Médico-Paciente , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Medidas de Resultados Relatados pelo Paciente , Satisfação do Paciente , Inquéritos e Questionários
15.
Pers Soc Psychol Bull ; 46(6): 856-868, 2020 06.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31610740

RESUMO

Zuckerman et al. (2013) conducted a meta-analysis of 63 studies that showed a negative intelligence-religiosity relation (IRR). As more studies have become available and because some of Zuckerman et al.'s (2013) conclusions have been challenged, we conducted a new meta-analysis with an updated data set of 83 studies. Confirming previous conclusions, the new analysis showed that the correlation between intelligence and religious beliefs in college and noncollege samples ranged from -.20 to -.23. There was no support for mediation of the IRR by education but there was support for partial mediation by analytic cognitive style. Thus, one possible interpretation for the IRR is that intelligent people are more likely to use analytic style (i.e., approach problems more rationally). An alternative (and less interesting) reason for the mediation is that tests of both intelligence and analytic style assess cognitive ability. Additional empirical and theoretical work is needed to resolve this issue.


Assuntos
Inteligência , Religião , Adulto , Cognição , Escolaridade , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino
16.
Cogn Emot ; 34(2): 329-351, 2020 03.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31221021

RESUMO

The ability to recognise others' emotions from nonverbal cues (emotion recognition ability, ERA) is measured with performance-based tests and has many positive correlates. Although researchers have long proposed that ERA is related to general mental ability or intelligence, a comprehensive analysis of this relationship is lacking. For instance, it remains unknown whether the magnitude of the association varies by intelligence type, ERA test features, as well as demographic variables. The present meta-analysis examined the relationship between ERA and intelligence based on 471 effect sizes from 133 samples and found a significant mean effect size (controlled for nesting within samples) of r = .19. Different intelligence types (crystallized, fluid, spatial, memory, information processing speed and efficiency) yielded similar effect sizes, whereas academic achievement measures (e.g. SAT scores) were unrelated to ERA. Effect sizes were higher for ERA tests that simultaneously present facial, vocal, and bodily cues (as compared to tests using static pictures) and for tests with higher reliability and more emotions. Results were unaffected by most study and sample characteristics, but effect size increased with higher mean age of the sample. These findings establish ERA as sensory-cognitive ability that is distinct from, yet related to, intelligence.


Assuntos
Emoções , Inteligência , Reconhecimento Psicológico , Sucesso Acadêmico , Adolescente , Adulto , Fatores Etários , Idoso , Sinais (Psicologia) , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Adulto Jovem
17.
Anesthesiology ; 132(1): 159-169, 2020 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31770142

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: Compassionate behavior in clinicians is described as seeking to understand patients' psychosocial, physical and medical needs, timely attending to these needs, and involving patients as they desire. The goal of our study was to evaluate compassionate behavior in patient interactions, pain management, and the informed consent process of anesthesia residents in a simulated preoperative evaluation of a patient in pain scheduled for urgent surgery. METHODS: Forty-nine Clinical Anesthesia residents in year 1 and 16 Clinical Anesthesia residents in year 3 from three residency programs individually obtained informed consent for anesthesia for an urgent laparotomy from a standardized patient complaining of pain. Encounters were assessed for ordering pain medication, for patient-resident interactions by using the Empathic Communication Coding System to code responses to pain and nausea cues, and for the content of the informed consent discussion. RESULTS: Of the 65 residents, 56 (86%) ordered pain medication, at an average of 4.2 min (95% CI, 3.2 to 5.1) into the encounter; 9 (14%) did not order pain medication. Resident responses to the cues averaged between perfunctory recognition and implicit recognition (mean, 1.7 [95% CI, 1.6 to 1.9]) in the 0 (less empathic) to 6 (more empathic) system. Responses were lower for residents who did not order pain medication (mean, 1.2 [95% CI, 0.8 to 1.6]) and similar for those who ordered medication before informed consent signing (mean, 1.9 [95% CI, 1.6 to 2.1]) and after signing (mean, 1.9 [95% CI, 1.6 to 2.0]; F (2, 62) = 4.21; P = 0.019; partial η = 0.120). There were significant differences between residents who ordered pain medication before informed consent and those who did not order pain medication and between residents who ordered pain medication after informed consent signing and those who did not. CONCLUSIONS: In a simulated preoperative evaluation, anesthesia residents have variable and, at times, flawed recognition of patient cues, responsiveness to patient cues, pain management, and patient interactions.


Assuntos
Anestesiologia/educação , Empatia , Consentimento Livre e Esclarecido/psicologia , Internato e Residência/métodos , Relações Médico-Paciente , Cuidados Pré-Operatórios/psicologia , Anestesiologia/métodos , Competência Clínica/estatística & dados numéricos , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Simulação de Paciente , Cuidados Pré-Operatórios/métodos
18.
J Soc Psychol ; 159(3): 225-243, 2019.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29781776

RESUMO

The empathy concept has central significance for social and personality psychology and in many other domains, including neuroscience, clinical/abnormal psychology, and the health professions. However, the current diversity in conceptual and operational definitions, and the promiscuous use of the term "empathy," threaten the ability of researchers to advance the field. The present article provides a quantitative review and conceptual analysis of empathy definitions and usages by examining 393 studies published between 2001 and 2013, and 96 studies published in 2017. We document the prevalence and diversity of definitions, as well as inconsistencies between conceptual definitions and measurements employed. We discuss ways to refine the conceptualization and operationalization of the empathy construct, including for many purposes, bypassing the term empathy in favor of lower-level construct labels that more precisely describe what is actually being measured. In many cases we see no added theoretical or empirical value in applying the term empathy.


Assuntos
Empatia , Psicologia Social , Terminologia como Assunto , Humanos
19.
Annu Rev Psychol ; 70: 271-294, 2019 01 04.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30256720

RESUMO

The field of nonverbal communication (NVC) has a long history involving many cue modalities, including face, voice, body, touch, and interpersonal space; different levels of analysis, including normative, group, and individual differences; and many substantive themes that cross from psychology into other disciplines. In this review, we focus on NVC as it pertains to individuals and social interaction. We concentrate specifically on ( a) the meanings and correlates of cues that are enacted (sent) by encoders and ( b) the perception of nonverbal cues and the accuracy of such perception. Frameworks are presented for conceptualizing and understanding the process of sending and receiving nonverbal cues. Measurement issues are discussed, and theoretical issues and new developments are covered briefly. Although our review is primarily oriented within social and personality psychology, the interdisciplinary nature of NVC is evident in the growing body of research on NVC across many areas of scientific inquiry.


Assuntos
Relações Interpessoais , Comunicação não Verbal , Percepção Social , Humanos , Comunicação não Verbal/psicologia
20.
Pers Soc Psychol Bull ; 45(7): 983-993, 2019 07.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30400748

RESUMO

We present five studies investigating the predictive validity of thin slices of nonverbal behavior (NVB). Predictive validity of thin slices refers to how well behavior slices excerpted from longer video predict other measured variables. Using six NVBs, we compared predictive validity of slices of different lengths with that obtained when coding is based on full-length (5-min) video, investigating the relative predictive validity of 1-min slices as well as of cumulative slices. Results indicate some loss in predictive validity with 1-min slices, but relatively little loss when Slices 1 and 2 were combined for five of the six NVBs. This research establishes an empirical basis on which researchers can decide how much of their recorded corpus needs to be coded for NVB. The results also provide some guidance on effect sizes in power analyses for researchers coding specific behaviors in a thin-slice design.


Assuntos
Relações Interpessoais , Comunicação não Verbal/psicologia , Adolescente , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Variações Dependentes do Observador , Reprodutibilidade dos Testes , Gravação em Vídeo
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA
...